Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
A. Klein, B. Vaissière, J. Cane, I. Steffan‐Dewenter, S. Cunningham, C. Kremen, T. Tscharntke (2007)
Importance of pollinators in changing landscapes for world cropsProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274
J. Spaethe, A. Weidenmüller (2002)
Size variation and foraging rate in bumblebees (Bombus terrestris)Insectes Sociaux, 49
F. Bartumeus, M. Luz, G. Viswanathan, J. Catalán (2005)
ANIMAL SEARCH STRATEGIES: A QUANTITATIVE RANDOM‐WALK ANALYSISEcology, 86
Jordanna Sprayberry, Kaitlin Ritter, Jeffrey Riffell (2013)
The Effect of Olfactory Exposure to Non-Insecticidal Agrochemicals on Bumblebee Foraging BehaviorPLoS ONE, 8
A. Riveros, W. Gronenberg (2009)
Olfactory learning and memory in the bumblebee Bombus occidentalisNaturwissenschaften, 96
A. Reynolds, Alan Smith, D. Reynolds, N. Carreck, J. Osborne (2007)
Honeybees perform optimal scale-free searching flights when attempting to locate a food sourceJournal of Experimental Biology, 210
Joshua Martin, Hong Lei, Jeffrey Riffell, J. Hildebrand (2013)
Synchronous firing of antennal-lobe projection neurons encodes the behaviorally effective ratio of sex-pheromone components in male Manduca sextaJournal of Comparative Physiology A, 199
I. Kulahci, A. Dornhaus, D. Papaj (2008)
Multimodal signals enhance decision making in foraging bumble-beesProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275
Amy Parachnowitsch, A. Kessler (2010)
Pollinators exert natural selection on flower size and floral display in Penstemon digitalis.The New phytologist, 188 2
Kazuo Suzuki, Ikumi Dohzono, K. Hiei (2007)
Evolution of pollinator generalization in bumblebee‐pollinated plantsPlant Species Biology, 22
F. Ruedenauer, J. Spaethe, S. Leonhardt (2016)
Hungry for quality—individual bumblebees forage flexibly to collect high-quality pollenBehavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 70
K. Walther-Hellwig, R. Frankl (2000)
Foraging habitats and foraging distances of bumblebees, Bombus spp. (Hym., Apidae), in an agricultural landscapeJournal of Applied Entomology, 124
R. Girling, I. Lusebrink, Emily Farthing, T. Newman, G. Poppy (2013)
Diesel exhaust rapidly degrades floral odours used by honeybeesScientific Reports, 3
Q. McFrederick, J. Kathilankal, Jose Fuentes (2008)
Air pollution modifies floral scent trailsAtmospheric Environment, 42
Nicholas Toda, Jeremy Song, J. Nieh (2009)
Bumblebees exhibit the memory spacing effectDie Naturwissenschaften, 96
J. Ott, L. Real, E. Silverfine (1985)
The effect of nectar variance on bumblebee patterns of movement and potential gene dispersalOikos, 45
A. James, M. Plank, A. Edwards (2011)
Assessing Lévy walks as models of animal foragingJournal of The Royal Society Interface, 8
A. Fraser, W. Mechaber, J. Hildebrand (2003)
Electroantennographic and Behavioral Responses of the Sphinx Moth Manduca sexta to Host Plant Headspace VolatilesJournal of Chemical Ecology, 29
H. Nijhout, G. Davidowitz, D. Roff (2006)
A quantitative analysis of the mechanism that controls body size in Manduca sextaJournal of Biology, 5
Jeffrey Riffell, L. Abrell, J. Hildebrand (2008)
Physical Processes and Real-Time Chemical Measurement of the Insect Olfactory EnvironmentJournal of Chemical Ecology, 34
B. Heinrich (2004)
Resource heterogeneity and patterns of movement in foraging bumblebeesOecologia, 40
A. Edwards, A. Edwards, R. Phillips, N. Watkins, M. Freeman, E. Murphy, V. Afanasyev, S. Buldyrev, S. Buldyrev, M. Luz, E. Raposo, H. Stanley, G. Viswanathan (2007)
Revisiting Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses, bumblebees and deerNature, 449
P. Graham, K. Cheng (2009)
Ants use the panoramic skyline as a visual cue during navigationCurrent Biology, 19
J. Goyret, R. Raguso (2006)
The role of mechanosensory input in flower handling efficiency and learning by Manduca sextaJournal of Experimental Biology, 209
J. Kunze, A. Gumbert (2001)
The combined effect of color and odor on flower choice behavior of bumble bees in flower mimicry systemsBehavioral Ecology, 12
M. Willis, E. Ford, J. Avondet (2013)
Odor tracking flight of male Manduca sexta moths along plumes of different cross-sectional areaJournal of Comparative Physiology A, 199
G. Pyke (2015)
Understanding movements of organisms: it's time to abandon the Lévy foraging hypothesisMethods in Ecology and Evolution, 6
R. Girling, B. Higbee, R. Cardé (2013)
The Plume Also Rises: Trajectories of Pheromone Plumes Issuing from Point Sources in an Orchard Canopy at NightJournal of Chemical Ecology, 39
G. Ramos-Fernández, J. Mateos, O. Miramontes, G. Cocho, H. Larralde, Bárbara Ayala-Orozco (2003)
Lévy walk patterns in the foraging movements of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi)Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 55
J. Osborne, Alan Smith, S. Clark, D. Reynolds, M. Barron, Ka Lim, A. Reynolds (2013)
The Ontogeny of Bumblebee Flight Trajectories: From Naïve Explorers to Experienced ForagersPLoS ONE, 8
K. Lunau (1992)
Innate recognition of flowers by bumble bees: orientation of antennae to visual stamen signalsCanadian Journal of Zoology, 70
R. Cartar (2004)
RESOURCE TRACKING BY BUMBLE BEES: RESPONSES TO PLANT-LEVEL DIFFERENCES IN QUALITYEcology, 85
Anne Leonard, A. Dornhaus, D. Papaj (2011)
Flowers help bees cope with uncertainty: signal detection and the function of floral complexityJournal of Experimental Biology, 214
G. Viswanathan, V. Afanasyev, S. Buldyrev, E. Murphy, P. Prince, H. Stanley (1996)
Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrossesNature, 381
M. Lihoreau, N. Raine, A. Reynolds, Ralph Stelzer, Ka Lim, Alan Smith, J. Osborne, L. Chittka (2012)
Radar Tracking and Motion-Sensitive Cameras on Flowers Reveal the Development of Pollinator Multi-Destination Routes over Large Spatial ScalesPLoS Biology, 10
S. Hegland, Anders Nielsen, A. Lázaro, A. Bjerknes, Ørjan Totland (2009)
How does climate warming affect plant-pollinator interactions?Ecology letters, 12 2
J. Fuentes, M. Chamecki, T. Roulston, Bicheng Chen, K. Pratt (2016)
Air pollutants degrade floral scents and increase insect foraging timesAtmospheric Environment, 141
J. Free (1979)
Bumblebee economicsNature, 280
S. Witjes, T. Eltz (2011)
Influence of scent deposits on flower choice: experiments in an artificial flower array with bumblebeesApidologie, 38
G. Farré-Armengol, J. Peñuelas, Tao Li, P. Yli-Pirilä, I. Filella, J. Llusià, J. Blande (2016)
Ozone degrades floral scent and reduces pollinator attraction to flowers.The New phytologist, 209 1
J. Cnaani, J. Thomson, D. Papaj (2006)
Flower Choice and Learning in Foraging Bumblebees: Effects of Variation in Nectar Volume and ConcentrationEthology, 112
P. Whitehorn, Stephanie O’Connor, F. Wackers, D. Goulson (2012)
Neonicotinoid Pesticide Reduces Bumble Bee Colony Growth and Queen ProductionScience, 336
G. Viswanathan, S. Buldyrev, S. Havlin, M. Luz, E. Raposo, H. Stanley (1999)
Optimizing the success of random searchesNature, 401
P. Skorupski, J. Spaethe, L. Chittka (2006)
Visual Search and Decision Making in Bees: Time, Speed, and AccuracyInternational Journal of Comparative Psychology
S. Potts, J. Biesmeijer, C. Kremen, P. Neumann, O. Schweiger, W. Kunin (2010)
Global pollinator declines: trends, impacts and drivers.Trends in ecology & evolution, 25 6
D. Goulson, J Stout, J. Langley, W. Hughes (2000)
Identity and Function of Scent Marks Deposited by Foraging BumblebeesJournal of Chemical Ecology, 26
Barbara Zorn-Arnold, H. Howe (2007)
Density and seed set in a self-compatible forb, Penstemon digitalis (Plantaginaceae), with multiple pollinators.American journal of botany, 94 10
E. Leadbeater, L. Chittka (2005)
A new mode of information transfer in foraging bumblebees?Current Biology, 15
N. Raine, L. Chittka (2007)
The Adaptive Significance of Sensory Bias in a Foraging Context: Floral Colour Preferences in the Bumblebee Bombus terrestrisPLoS ONE, 2
N. Saleh, A. Scott, G. Bryning, L. Chittka (2007)
Distinguishing signals and cues: bumblebees use general footprints to generate adaptive behaviour at flowers and nestArthropod-Plant Interactions, 1
B. Cole (1995)
Fractal time in animal behaviour: the movement activity of DrosophilaAnimal Behaviour, 50
A. James, J. Pitchford, M. Plank (2010)
Efficient or Inaccurate? Analytical and Numerical Modelling of Random Search StrategiesBulletin of Mathematical Biology, 72
M. Oliveira, B. Murrell, Josué Pérez-Santiago, Milenka Vargas, R. Ellis, S. Letendre, I. Grant, Davey Smith, S. Woods, S. Gianella (2016)
Corrigendum: Circulating HIV DNA Correlates With Neurocognitive Impairment in Older HIV-infected Adults on Suppressive ARTScientific Reports, 6
F. Lenz, A. Chechkin, Rainer Klages (2013)
Constructing a Stochastic Model of Bumblebee Flights from Experimental DataPLoS ONE, 8
(Heinrich, B. Bumblebee economics (Harvard University Press, 2004).)
Heinrich, B. Bumblebee economics (Harvard University Press, 2004).Heinrich, B. Bumblebee economics (Harvard University Press, 2004)., Heinrich, B. Bumblebee economics (Harvard University Press, 2004).
V. Cappello, L. Marchetti, P. Parlanti, S. Landi, I. Tonazzini, M. Cecchini, V. Piazza, M. Gemmi (2016)
Ultrastructural Characterization of the Lower Motor System in a Mouse Model of Krabbe DiseaseScientific Reports, 6
M. Becher, Volker Grimm, J. Knapp, Juliane Horn, Grace Twiston‐Davies, Juliet Osborne (2016)
BEESCOUT: A model of bee scouting behaviour and a software tool for characterizing nectar/pollen landscapes for BEEHAVEEcological Modelling, 340
(Goulson, D., Stout, J. C. & Langley, J. Identity and function of scent marks deposited by foraging bumblebees. J Chem Ecol (2000).)
Goulson, D., Stout, J. C. & Langley, J. Identity and function of scent marks deposited by foraging bumblebees. J Chem Ecol (2000).Goulson, D., Stout, J. C. & Langley, J. Identity and function of scent marks deposited by foraging bumblebees. J Chem Ecol (2000)., Goulson, D., Stout, J. C. & Langley, J. Identity and function of scent marks deposited by foraging bumblebees. J Chem Ecol (2000).
G. Pyke (1984)
Optimal Foraging Theory: A Critical ReviewAnnual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics, 15
Paul Williams, J. Osborne (2009)
Bumblebee vulnerability and conservation world-wideApidologie, 40
M. Molet, L. Chittka, N. Raine (2009)
How floral odours are learned inside the bumblebee (Bombus terrestris) nestNaturwissenschaften, 96
B. Hansson, Mikael Carlsson, B. Kalinová (2003)
Olfactory activation patterns in the antennal lobe of the sphinx moth, Manduca sextaJournal of Comparative Physiology A, 189
Amy Parachnowitsch, R. Raguso, A. Kessler (2012)
Phenotypic selection to increase floral scent emission, but not flower size or colour in bee-pollinated Penstemon digitalis.The New phytologist, 195 3
R. Owen, F. Rodd, R. Plowright (1980)
Sex ratios in bumble bee colonies: Complications due to orphaning?Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 7
J. Osborne, S. Clark, R. Morris, I. Williams, J. Riley, Alan Smith, D. Reynolds, A. Edwards (1999)
A landscape‐scale study of bumble bee foraging range and constancy, using harmonic radarJournal of Applied Ecology, 36
A. Dyer, J. Spaethe, Sabina Prack (2008)
Comparative psychophysics of bumblebee and honeybee colour discrimination and object detectionJournal of Comparative Physiology A, 194
T. Macuda, R. Gegear, T. Laverty, B. Timney (2001)
Behavioural assessment of visual acuity in bumblebees (Bombus impatiens).The Journal of experimental biology, 204 Pt 3
(Leonard, A. S., Dornhaus, A. & Papaj, D. R. In Evolution of Plant-Pollinator Relationships (ed. Patiny, S.) 279–300 (books.google.com, 2011).)
Leonard, A. S., Dornhaus, A. & Papaj, D. R. In Evolution of Plant-Pollinator Relationships (ed. Patiny, S.) 279–300 (books.google.com, 2011).Leonard, A. S., Dornhaus, A. & Papaj, D. R. In Evolution of Plant-Pollinator Relationships (ed. Patiny, S.) 279–300 (books.google.com, 2011)., Leonard, A. S., Dornhaus, A. & Papaj, D. R. In Evolution of Plant-Pollinator Relationships (ed. Patiny, S.) 279–300 (books.google.com, 2011).
Jeffrey Riffell, Eli Shlizerman, Elischa Sanders, L. Abrell, Billie Medina, Armin Hinterwirth, J. Kutz (2014)
Flower discrimination by pollinators in a dynamic chemical environmentScience, 344
J. Murlis, M. Willis, R. Cardé (2000)
Spatial and temporal structures of pheromone plumes in fields and forestsPhysiological Entomology, 25
B Mulloney (1970)
10.1242/jeb.52.1.59J Exp Biol, 52
K. Lunau (2010)
Innate Flower Recognition in Bumblebees (Bombus terrestris, B. lucorum; Apidae): Optical Signals from Stamens as Landing Reaction ReleasersEthology, 88
D. Goulson, G. Lye, B. Darvill (2008)
Decline and conservation of bumble bees.Annual review of entomology, 53
S. Wagenius, S. Lyon (2010)
Reproduction of Echinacea angustjfolia in fragmented prairie is pollen-limited but not pollinator-limited.Ecology, 91 3
Stephan Wolf, Elizabeth Nicholls, Andrew Reynolds, Patricia Wells, Ka Lim, Robert Paxton, Juliet Osborne (2016)
Optimal search patterns in honeybee orientation flights are robust against emerging infectious diseasesScientific Reports, 6
C. Suchet, L. Dormont, B. Schatz, M. Giurfa, V. Simon, C. Raynaud, J. Chave (2011)
Floral scent variation in two Antirrhinum majus subspecies influences the choice of naïve bumblebeesBehavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 65
(Spaethe, J., Chittka, L. & Skorupski, P. Visual search and decision making in bees: time, speed, and accuracy. Int J Comp Psychol (2006).)
Spaethe, J., Chittka, L. & Skorupski, P. Visual search and decision making in bees: time, speed, and accuracy. Int J Comp Psychol (2006).Spaethe, J., Chittka, L. & Skorupski, P. Visual search and decision making in bees: time, speed, and accuracy. Int J Comp Psychol (2006)., Spaethe, J., Chittka, L. & Skorupski, P. Visual search and decision making in bees: time, speed, and accuracy. Int J Comp Psychol (2006).
J. Woodgate, J. Makinson, Ka Lim, A. Reynolds, L. Chittka (2016)
Life-Long Radar Tracking of BumblebeesPLoS ONE, 11
E. Odell, R. Raguso, K. Jones (1999)
Bumblebee Foraging Responses to Variation in Floral Scent and Color in Snapdragons (Antirrhinum: Scrophulariaceae), 142
S. Cameron, Jeffrey Lozier, J. Strange, J. Koch, Nils Cordes, L. Solter, T. Griswold (2011)
Patterns of widespread decline in North American bumble beesProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108
T Macuda (2001)
10.1242/jeb.204.3.559J Exp Biol, 204
J. Wilms, T. Eltz (2008)
Foraging scent marks of bumblebees: footprint cues rather than pheromone signalsNaturwissenschaften, 95
(Odell, E., Raguso, R. A. & Jones, K. N. Bumblebee foraging responses to variation in floral scent and color in snapdragons (Antirrhinum: Scrophulariaceae). Am Midl Nat (1999).)
Odell, E., Raguso, R. A. & Jones, K. N. Bumblebee foraging responses to variation in floral scent and color in snapdragons (Antirrhinum: Scrophulariaceae). Am Midl Nat (1999).Odell, E., Raguso, R. A. & Jones, K. N. Bumblebee foraging responses to variation in floral scent and color in snapdragons (Antirrhinum: Scrophulariaceae). Am Midl Nat (1999)., Odell, E., Raguso, R. A. & Jones, K. N. Bumblebee foraging responses to variation in floral scent and color in snapdragons (Antirrhinum: Scrophulariaceae). Am Midl Nat (1999).
M. Dacke, M. Dacke, B. Jundi, Jochen Smolka, M. Byrne, E. Baird (2014)
The role of the sun in the celestial compass of dung beetlesPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369
T. Ings, N. Raine, L. Chittka (2009)
A population comparison of the strength and persistence of innate colour preference and learning speed in the bumblebee Bombus terrestrisBehavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 63
B. Heinrich (1979)
"Majoring" and "Minoring" by Foraging Bumblebees, Bombus Vagans: An Experimental AnalysisEcology, 60
T. Hudon, C. Plowright (2011)
Trapped: Assessing Attractiveness of Potential Food Sources to BumblebeesJournal of Insect Behavior, 24
Richard Gill, N. Raine (2014)
Chronic impairment of bumblebee natural foraging behaviour induced by sublethal pesticide exposureFunctional Ecology, 28
Jeffrey Riffell, Hong Lei, J. Hildebrand (2009)
Neural correlates of behavior in the moth Manduca sexta in response to complex odorsProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106
(Girling, R. D., Lusebrink, I., Farthing, E., Newman, T. A. & Poppy, G. M. Diesel exhaust rapidly degrades floral odours used by honeybees. Sci Rep3 (2013).)
Girling, R. D., Lusebrink, I., Farthing, E., Newman, T. A. & Poppy, G. M. Diesel exhaust rapidly degrades floral odours used by honeybees. Sci Rep3 (2013).Girling, R. D., Lusebrink, I., Farthing, E., Newman, T. A. & Poppy, G. M. Diesel exhaust rapidly degrades floral odours used by honeybees. Sci Rep3 (2013)., Girling, R. D., Lusebrink, I., Farthing, E., Newman, T. A. & Poppy, G. M. Diesel exhaust rapidly degrades floral odours used by honeybees. Sci Rep3 (2013).
Frank Sommerlandt, W. Rössler, J. Spaethe (2013)
Elemental and non-elemental olfactory learning using PER conditioning in the bumblebee, Bombus terrestrisApidologie, 45
J. Spaethe, L. Chittka (2003)
Interindividual variation of eye optics and single object resolution in bumblebeesJournal of Experimental Biology, 206
Anne Leonard, Pavel Masek (2014)
Multisensory integration of colors and scents: insights from bees and flowersJournal of Comparative Physiology A, 200
B. Mulloney (1970)
Impulse patterns in the flight motor neurones of Bombus californicus and Oncopeltus fasciatus.The Journal of experimental biology, 52 1
J. Spaethe, A. Brockmann, Christine Halbig, J. Tautz (2007)
Size determines antennal sensitivity and behavioral threshold to odors in bumblebee workersNaturwissenschaften, 94
M. Renner, J. Nieh (2008)
Bumble bee olfactory information flow and contact-based foraging activationInsectes Sociaux, 55
J. Schwartz, Raymond Hunce, Brandon Lentine, Kristi Powers (2015)
Calling site choice and its impact on call degradation and call attractiveness in the gray treefrog, Hyla versicolorBehavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 70
M. Knaden, P. Graham (2016)
The Sensory Ecology of Ant Navigation: From Natural Environments to Neural Mechanisms.Annual review of entomology, 61
www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN The prevalence of olfactory- versus visual-signal encounter by searching bumblebees Received: 26 March 2018 Jordanna D. H. Sprayberry Accepted: 18 September 2018 While the phrase ‘foraging bumblebee’ brings to mind a bumbling bee flying flower to flower in a Published: xx xx xxxx sunny meadow, foraging is a complicated series of behaviors such as: locating a floral patch; selecting a flower-type; learning handling skills for pollen and nectar extraction; determining when to move-on from a patch; learning within-patch paths (traplining); and learning efficient hive-to-patch routes (spatial navigation). Thus the term ‘forager’ encompasses multiple distinct behaviors that rely on different sensory modalities. Despite a robust literature on bumblebee foraging behavior, few studies are directly relevant to sensory-guided search; i.e. how workers locate novel patches. The first step in answering this question is to determine what sensory information is available to searching bumblebees. This manuscript presents a computational model that elucidates the relative frequency of visual and olfactory cues that are available to workers searching for floral resources under a range of ecologically relevant scenarios. Model results indicate that odor is the most common sensory cue encountered during search flights. When the likelihood of odor-plume contact is higher, odor-encounter is ubiquitous. While integrative (visual + olfactory) cues are common when foragers are searching for larger flowers (e.g. Echinacea), they become rare when foragers are searching for small flowers (e.g. Penstemon). Visual cues are only encountered in isolation when foragers are seeking large flowers with a low odor- plume contact probability. These results indicate that despite the multisensory nature of floral signals, different modalities may be encountered in isolation during search-behavior, as opposed to the reliably multimodal signals encountered during patch-exploitation or nectar/ pollen acquisition. Bumblebee populations are sensitive to decreases in foraging efficiency. Bumblebees are critical 1–3 pollinators in both agricultural and native ecosystems . Unfortunately these keystone species have experienced 4–7 alarming declines alongside the highly publicized drops in honeybee numbers . Critical work exposing the negative effects of neonicitinoid pesticides on bumblebee fitness indicates that pesticide exposure lowers rates of 8,9 reproduction due, at least in part, to a drop in foraging efficacy of both workers and the colony as a whole . This provides a critical link showing that the modification of worker behavior scales up to impact colony level fitness – a result that is consistent with seminal work showing that a colony’s ability to produce reproductive individuals is directly correlated with their size . Better foragers provide more resources to rear young at the hive, which can increase the size of a colony during a foraging season. Given the current environmental pressures on bumblebees, developing a deeper understanding of their foraging behavior is relevant to conservation efforts. How do foragers search for flowers? While the term “forager” can be defined as an animal locating and consuming food resources, it is a complicated series of behaviors. In bumblebees this includes: locating a 11,12 13,14 15 floral patch ; selecting a flower-type ; learning handling skills for pollen and nectar extraction ; deter- 16,17 12,18 mining when to move-on from a patch ; learning within-patch paths (traplining) ; and learning efficient 11,12,19 hive-to-patch routes (spatial navigation) . Thus the term ‘forager’ encompasses multiple distinct behaviors 20–22 23 that rely on different sensory modalities . A critical component of foraging theory is the search phase ; which would be floral patch location in the case of pollinators. This phase is comprised of: (1) movement through the environment; and (2) recognition of resources, which should terminate the search. There is a wealth of litera- 24–27 ture analyzing forager search paths, from bumblebees to albatrosses . While there is some controversy over 28–33 the precise algorithms that accurately describe these search paths , there is consensus that search paths can be reasonably represented with stochastic models of forward-biased motion (i.e. while turning events happen, Muhlenberg College, 2400 W Chew St, Allentown, PA, 18104, USA. Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to J.D.H.S. (email: [email protected]) Scientific REPO R TS | (2018) 8:14590 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-32897-y 1 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ complete direction reversal will be rare). Once a searching forager recognizes a resource, their behavior should transition from random-search to approach and feeding. In bumble bees the ability to recognize floral resources will be dependent upon perception of floral signals. Flowers provide complex sensory displays, including color, 34–37 shape, nectar guides, odor and morphology . In the case of pollinators searching for novel patches, only those sensory cues capable of operating at a distance will factor into recognition and subsequent sensory-guided navi- gation. Morphological cues are only relevant upon physical contact with flower and are thus not useful for search. Complex patterns on flowers, such as nectar guides or visible stamens, are only resolvable at close distances (4–45 cm) . Thus shape, color and odor are the sensory signals most likely to be available for patch recognition. Odor pollution impacts forager behavior, but the effects on foraging efficiency are unclear. Several studies over the past decade have indicated that anthropogenic odor pollution is both modifying floral odor plumes 40,41 and subsequent behavioral responses of bees . While this work is interesting from a neuroethological standpoint, it is currently unclear how drastically natural foraging populations are impacted by odor pollution. Understanding the potential impact of odor-pollution first requires an understanding of odor’s role in foraging. er Th e is a substantial body of work indicating that olfaction is important in patch exploitation; however, the precise role that odor plays is not completely understood. PER studies indicate that bumblebees are capable of 42–44 associative odor learning , generating the logical hypothesis that floral odor could be used to identify reward- ing flowers. Multimodal studies investigating both vision and olfaction indicate that stimulation of odor pathways improves foraging accuracy, regardless of whether or not floral signals have differentiating odor stimuli . Field experimentation on floral morphs showed that bumblebees prioritized visitation of a learned visual (color) sig- nal over the learned odor . These findings might imply that any odor is effective, and that precise odor identity might be irrelevant. However, work by Leonard et al. showed that when flowers differ in both visual and olfactory modalities, foraging accuracy was higher – pushing back against the idea that odor identity is unimportant. Social odor cues – tarsal scent deposits on flowers, reduce bumblebee visitation rates. This is an example of a ‘contaminating odor’ that increases energy gain by reducing visitation to recently emptied flowers. It is likely that scent marks are perceptually distinct from the floral odor, rather than modifying the floral blend-structure such that it becomes unrecognizable to the bumblebee, as behavioral data have been relatively consistent across 47 48 multiple flower species and with unscented artificial flowers . er Th efore, it appears that the precise odor iden- tification of tarsal scent-marks is quite important to foraging behavior. Given the contradictory nature of current data on odor usage, it is difficult to predict the effects of pollution on foraging efficiency during patch exploitation. er Th e is a paucity of work looking at the impact of odor on navigation to food resources in bumblebees. Several 40,49 lab-based studies indicate that odor alone is sufficient to facilitate navigation . However, the relative roles of odor and vision (which could have implications for how drastic the effects of odor pollution might be) have never been investigated at a spatial scale that would shed light on the role of odor in patch location. For example, lab 13,35,40,48,50–59 studies are typically in arenas that are less than 3.6 m in their largest dimension . However, the foraging range of a bumblebee can reach up to 1.75 km from their nest – a distance that is orders of magnitude larger than typical sensory-behavior studies, even those that are based in the field . An understanding of odor-pollution’s impacts requires a better understanding of the relative roles that vision and olfaction play in navigation to floral resources. If a searching-forager is consistently encountering an odor signal before a visual signal, it stands to rea- son that odor-guided navigation will bring that animal within visual range of a flower. Given that odor plumes are 39,61 38,62 theoretically available at a much greater distance from a flower than visual cues this is a logical assumption. However, odor plume contact is stochastic, and some empirical measurements of odor-plumes indicate much shorter distances travelled . This manuscript presents a computational model that moves beyond assumptions and asks – given the probabilistic nature of odor plume contact- what is the likelihood of a bumblebee encountering resolvable visual versus olfactory cues? Methods In order to determine which sensory cues are available to searching foragers this model creates a random search path for a bumblebee through a simulated meadow and at each step assesses whether or not the bumblebee has encountered a resolvable visual or olfactory cue from flowers populating the meadow. Meadow dimensions (70 m × 160 m) were based upon Google-satellite images of a clearing at Conrad W. Raker Sanctuary, a biological field station owned by Muhlenberg College (Fig. 1). Bumblebee Movement. This model generated a search path for bumblebees in order to walk them through a digital meadow until they encountered a salient and resolvable floral sensory cue. These computational paths utilized a correlated random walk model (CRW) to generate motion in the latitude-longitude plane: αα () tT += () tt + β() xt () += Tx() tl +⋅ cos(α() t ) yt () += Tx() tl +⋅ sin(α() t ) where: α is the current heading angle. β is the turning angle, where the probability of turning is based upon dig- itization of Heinrich’s canonical observations of bumblebee foraging behavior (Table 1). T is the time step – an iterative and scale-less variable whose actual value is represented by step length. x(t + T) gives the longitudinal position of the bumblebee in the next time step. y(t + T) gives the latitudinal position of the bumblebee in the next time step. l(t) is a step length of 0.3 m; determined by the product of bumblebee flight speed (3 m/s ) and the interspike interval of motor neurons (approx. 0.1 s ). This represents a reasonable estimate of how rapidly the flight system could change course. Scientific REPO R TS | (2018) 8:14590 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-32897-y 2 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Figure 1. Sensory construction of the artificial meadow that ‘bumblebees’ searched within. The digitized meadow from a scenario utilizing single-flower Echinacea plants, a plant density of 1/600 m , and a high odor probability. The probability of odor contact is represented by the blue contours. The visual resolvability is represented by the yellow circles; the inset labels the relationship between angular resolution of the searching bumblebee and the distance at which a flower becomes visible. The finest resolution (1.39 degrees) results in the greatest visual detection distance. Turn angle (degrees) Probability (0–1) −135 0.025 −90 0.065 −45 0.19 0 0.44 45 0.19 90 0.065 135 0.025 Table 1. Turning probability digitized from Fig. 3 in Heinrich . This method of search-path computation deviates slightly from the more commonly-referenced Levy 19,24,26–28,67,68 29 walk in that the step length is constant, rather than pulled from a power-law distribution . Maintaining this constant step length allows the model to be tightly parameterized to known flight-speed meas - urements (as in Becher et al. ) rather than incorporate occasional large step lengths that imply biologically implausible flight speeds. Work by James et al . indicates that efficiency in resource location by searching foragers has little to do with the search algorithm and is predominantly driven by the density of food resources ; if so the use of a CRW in this model should not corrupt the results. In addition, the basis for Levy-flights/walks being a 31,33 behaviorally accurate method of modelling forager-search behavior has recently been called into question . However, given the prevalence of Levy flights in foraging literature, it is worthwhile to confirm that using this method would not significantly modify conclusions about floral sensory encounter. Thus, a subset of model-conditions were run with variable step lengths drawn from a Levy Distribution : −μ Pl () = l where: P(l) is the probability of a particular step length. l is the corresponding step length. l is the minimum step length, set to 0.3 m (see justification above). μ is an exponential constant such that if it is between 1 and 3 the distribution meets the requirements for a Levy flight/walk. In this case it is set to 2, which produces an optimal search strategy . Individual model runs started with bumblebees entering the meadow at a randomized edge location – mim- icking arriving at the meadow from adjacent wooded territory. In all cases individual model runs continued until the bumblebee encountered a resolvable sensory signal (see Sensory Performance of Bumblebees) or completed 5000 steps – the equivalent of 1.5 km in the CRW, a value selected because it falls in the upper range of measured Scientific REPO R TS | (2018) 8:14590 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-32897-y 3 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Figure 2. e r Th elationship between flower number and the size of visual stimulus (w). The morphology of Echinacea plants (a) lends itself to polygon packing of blooms (b) while the upright habit of Penstemon (c) makes a double row arrangement (d) a more logical choice for that species. foraging distances . Given the variable step lengths in the Levy-flight condition, each model run has a unique potential maximum distance travelled; the mean for 1000 runs is 3 km. Because this model ends its runs upon floral-signal encounter, it is investigating what sensory information is available to searching bees and does not explicitly simulate floral approach. However, given that the visual acuity 38,62 measurements are derived from behavioral rather than physiological experiments bumblebees should be able to visually navigate to a “found” flower. Likewise, there is a body of work indicating that bumblebees are capable 40,49 of using odor cues at a distance to locate food resources . It is therefore plausible that searching bumblebees would be capable of acting upon salient and resolvable sensory stimuli; i.e. workers would be able to pick up where the model leaves off. This model does not model floral approach because it is outside the scope of our current question. Floral Parameters. Given that bumblebees are likely to encounter environmental variation in the field, the model varied plant density, plant size, and inflorescence size. Floral parameters were based upon published data from Echinacea and Penstemon spp, two common native genuses with wide ranging distributions that are readily 72,73 pollinated by bumblebees . These species provide an ecologically relevant range of bloom sizes, ranging from 0.7 cm diameter (Penstemon ) to 7.6 cm (Echinacea). Previous work on Echinacea spp indicates a wide range of naturally occurring densities (0.001 to 3 plants/m , estimated from nearest neighbor data in Wagenius and Lyon ). For the purpose of this study, which is interested in navigation to novel patches, I tested a realistic range of low densities (i.e. situations where the next nearest patch was not likely to be within visual range of the first): 1 2 2 2 plant/6 m (0.17), 1 plant/60 m (0.017), and 1 plant/ 600 m (0.0017). Field data on Penstemon indicated higher density tendencies, with a range of 0.16–1.64 plants/m . Thus the total set of tested densities was 0.0017, 0.017, 0.17, 0.89, 1.64. Floral patches were then randomly distributed throughout the meadow based upon the overall density (the number of plants per square meter). A brief survey of Echinacea purpurea plants revealed a high variability in number of blooms per plant (3–62). Therefore, I tested five different display sizes based on number of observed flowers: 1, 7, 19, 37, and 61. To determine the diameter of these displays flowers were polygon packed, resulting in diameters of 0.076, 0.228, 0.38, 0.532, and 0.684 meters respectively (Fig. 2a,b). Penstemon plants have a different growth habit than Echinacea , presenting flowers vertically on spikes; therefore, I used a two line packing of multiple blooms to estimate display size from the largest dimension (Fig. 2c,d). Recent work has shown the mean daily number of flowers for Penstemon digitalis to be 5 – the tested number of flowers were 3, 5, 7, 9, and 11 to encompass a range around this mean; resulting in display sizes of 0.014, 0.021, 0.028, 0.035, and 0.042 meters respectively. Sensory Performance of Bumblebees. Bumblebee size has a marked effect on visual acuity . Given the large variability in worker size that is likely to occur in natural populations, the model was tested with three different visual acuity values (1.39, 3.5, and 7 degrees) representing the range of values in the literature for two 62 38 bumblebee species (Bombus terrestris and Bombus impatiens ). Flowers were considered to be resolvable if their angular size from the bumblebee’s current position was equal to or greater than the visual acuity value. Angles were calculated as: w −1 θ = tan where: θ is the angle subtended by the floral display. w is the width of the floral display. D is the bumblebee’s dis- tance to the floral display calculated via the Pythagorean theorem. While the CRW generates planar motion, bumblebees clearly forage in three dimensional environments. er Th efore, distance calculations assumed bees were flying 1 m above vegetation. While variation in body size of workers also impacts olfactory performance , the resolution of olfactory stim- uli is based upon the likelihood of encountering an intact (i.e. not well mixed and therefore diluted) and resolv- able plume-filament. Probability of plume contact is derived from studies investigating odor plume availability in field conditions; this model is making the assumption that an odor filament strong enough to be measured by Scientific REPO R TS | (2018) 8:14590 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-32897-y 4 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Figure 3. e b Th lack lines represent the relationship between distance from flower (odor source) and the likelihood of plume contact, with the solid line representing a ‘high’ probability derived from Murlis et al. and the dashed line representing a ‘low’ probability derived from Riffell et al . . The grey line is an estimated t der fi ived from McFrederick et al .’s calculations on distance travelled by floral odorants, represented by percent of emission concentration. By these calculations common floral odorants do not drop below 80% of original concentration until 25 m from source, indicating the odor probabilities used in this model are quite conservative. Additionally, comparing the concentration decay with the model’s odor probabilities indicates that when bumblebees have a 10% probability of plume contact, that plume is still at >80% original concentration, thus that plume is likely physiologically salient. laboratory equipment would be strong enough to stimulate a response. Given work by Murlis et al. – where they found a 1:1 relationship between antennal response in Manduca sexta and presence of a measured plume – this is a physiologically reasonable assumption. As existing field measurements show variability across environments, I tested two different olfactory probability functions (Fig. 3). The ‘high’ probability function was based upon field measurements by Murlis et al. taken in mid-July in an open field near Amhurst, MA . This study provided field measurements of contact probability up to a distance of 20 m. However, odor plumes tend to be highly mixed and thus undetectable by a distance of 100 M . Thus I bookended the Murlis data with two constraints: a value of 1 at 0 meters, representing the maximum probability of plume contact; and a value of 0.0001 at 100 meters – given that the model was repeated 1000 trials per condition this value functionally represents zero. These data were −0.086× 2 then fit with an exponential in excel (y = 0.54e , R = 0.99, Fig. 3). The ‘low’ probability was taken directly from the exponential fit from Riffell et al .’s work measuring plume structure in a high alpine desert (Fig. 3). Odor probabilities operated in a radially symmetric fashion around floral displays (Fig. 1). Both of these prob- abilities are based upon data from studies on the hawkmoth Manduca sexta, a model organism in the study of 77 78–80 odor-guided flight and olfactory processing . Hawkmoths have both a larger body size and antennal length 81,82 49 than Bombus species . Given that body size in bumblebees correlates with greater olfactory sensitivity , there is the possibility that odor-encounter probabilities for Manduca over-estimate bumblebee olfactory capabilities. Interestingly, a comparison of the odor-behavior literature shows that bumblebee experiments are typically run at 40,49,80,83 much lower odor concentrations (1:1000) than Manduca experiments (neat extracts) . In addition, bum- blebee electroantennogram experiments (EAGs) require significantly higher odor concentrations (1:10–1:100) than bumblebee behavior experiments (Sprayberry unpub data), likely due to the noisy nature of electrophys- iology recordings requiring a stronger stimulus to create a favorable signal: noise ratio. This is relevant because the “high” probability odor encounter plume is derived from EAG recordings, and thus likely underestimates the actual sensing ability of insect antennae. Therefore, while in-vivo hawkmoths may have higher odor sensitivity than bumblebees, the anthropogenically-derived odor probabilities are likely applicable to both insect groups. In addition, even the high-probability fit is conservative when compared to calculations of distance travelled by floral odorants, which indicate a less than 50% loss of volatiles at distances of 100 m (Fig. 3). McFrederick et al.’s computational analysis does not consider plume structure – those remaining molecules may be well mixed and thus at physiologically irrelevant concentrations; however, it does imply that this model is unlikely to overestimate olfactory contact. Results and Discussion This model explored the sensory signals available to bumblebee foragers searching for novel resources by calcu- lating the relative probability of workers encountering the visual and/or olfactory signal from a floral resource while searching in a relatively low-resource environment. The parameters varied in this model were: plant density, number of blooms (and thus the strength of sensory signals from an individual plant), the probability of odor plume encounter, and the visual acuity of the searching “bumblebee”. Olfaction is the dominant sensory modality available to searching bees. Looking holistically at all tested scenarios we see that odor dominated as the available sensory modality; with odor alone representing floral sensory encounter in 179/350 scenarios, an integrated odor-visual signal available in 136/350 scenarios, and vision alone as the dominate modality in only 35/350 (Figs 4 and 5). Odor information is therefore available for Scientific REPO R TS | (2018) 8:14590 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-32897-y 5 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Figure 4. Heat maps indicating the relative likelihood of encountering a resolvable olfactory (blue), visual (yellow), or integrated olfactory and visual (green) sensory signal. These likelihoods were calculated for:1. multiple plant sizes, indicated by a variable number of flowers on the x axes; 2. multiple plant densities, indicated on the y axes; 3. two different plant species, Echinacea (top diagram) and Penstemon (bottom diagram); 4. two different odor probabilities, with high encounter probability represented in the left row and low on the right; and 5. three different visual acuities, labelled with their angular resolution on the right hand side of the figure. Each model scenario was run 1000 times. The number of failures – runs where a bee searched for 1.5 km without encountering a sensory signal- are indicated on the plots themselves. The absence of a number means that all 1000 runs resulted in a successful sensory encounter. decision making in 90% of successful floral encounters, while visual information is only present in 49%. While 35,46,84 there is substantial work indicating that vision is vitally important for patch exploitation behaviors , it is likely that odor is crucial in patch location behavior. Odor landscapes are changing, which could have a considerable impact on bumblebee foraging behavior. Model runs with a higher probability of odor contact demonstrated a larger discovery distance, with bumblebees contacting a resolvable sensory signal in the range of 25–40 meters, as opposed to 2–20 meters (Fig. 6). Additionally, decreased probability of odor-contact drastically increased the likelihood that for- ager searches would end in failure (Figs 4 and 5). Failure rates overall were higher for the smaller bloom size (Penstemon), as the larger plant and flower size of Echinacea ao ff rded a better ability to transition to visual navi - gation when odor was unavailable. These computational results are commensurate with laboratory investigations on visual search time in bumblebees, where bees who have been restricted to solely visual information have higher search times to locate smaller flowers . The low-odor probability tested in this model decays rapidly, tran- sitioning to zero before 10 meters from the point source (Fig. 3) . This empirical measurement may be underes- timating plume strength due to environmental conditions: previous work has shown that odor plumes can rise in altitude and the Riffell et al . measurements were taken at a consistent elevation from the ground. However; the results from this odor t fi are relevant to consider in light of work examining the impacts of anthropogenic pollution on floral odor-plumes. Seminal work by McFrederick et al . indicates that environmental pollutants can interact with floral odorants, reducing their distance travelled by an order of magnitude drop: odorants that historically could travel 1000 m before dropping to 80% of their original concentration would only make it 100 m in worst case scenarios. While McFrederick’s study was computational, subsequent experimental studies have been equally concerning. Girling et al. found that diesel exhaust degrades select floral odorants, modifying odor 87 41 blend structure . Likewise Farre-Armengol et al. found that ozone decreases floral odorant concentrations . Based on our model results it is reasonable to hypothesize that bumblebees will experience higher failure rates in Scientific REPO R TS | (2018) 8:14590 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-32897-y 6 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ Figure 5. Results of model runs using a Levy-walk distribution of step lengths for bumblebees with a visual resolution of 3.5° searching for Echinacea. Despite the difference in search-path calculation methods, the results are nearly identical to those depicted in Figs 4 and 6. Levy-walk searches do lead to a slight reduction in failure rates for low-odor probability scenarios. Figure 6. Heat maps indicating average distance at which a resolvable sensory signal was encountered in successful model runs. These likelihoods were calculated for: 1. multiple plant sizes, indicated by a variable number of flowers on the x axes; 2. multiple plant densities, indicated on the y axes; 3. two different plant species, Echinacea (two left columns) and Penstemon (two right columns); 4. two different odor probabilities (labelled by column); and 5. three different visual acuities, labelled with their angular resolution on the right hand side of the figure. Scientific REPO R TS | (2018) 8:14590 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-32897-y 7 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ locating flowers when searching in polluted environments, particularly if available floral resources are comprised of plants with smaller bloom size and lower bloom number. Indeed, failure to locate a floral signal only occurred in the low-probability odor scenario - when odor information is readily available searching events are universally successful. Bloom size, number of blooms per plant and plant density impact both available sensory modality and distance at which plants are found. Unsurprisingly, plant size and density impact the likelihood that bumblebees will encounter a resolvable sensory signal (Figs 4 and 5). Increased plant density reduced failure rate in low odor probability situations for both large (Echinacea) and small (Penstemon) flowers. However, when plants with small bloom sizes are in low density patches they were only reliably ‘found’ in model runs with a higher probability of odor plume contact. Echinacea simulations were moderately less susceptible to density effects as they can be seen from a greater distance, but higher flower number was still associated with an increased discovery distance. Interestingly, field data on Penstemon indicated that they were typically found at the higher densities this model tested – the lower densities tested here were included purely for comparative purposes. In light of anthropogenic modulation of odor environments, bumblebees may passively select for larger bloom size and higher plant density in polluted environments by virtue of not being able to locate smaller flowers, or those with larger nearest-neighbor distances. Effects of search-path type. The outcome of model runs using a power-law distribution for step lengths (Levy-walk) (Fig. 5) is nearly identical to the results from constant step lengths (Fig. 4). Odor information is ubiq- uitous in the high-odor probability scenarios, with visual information not being encountered in isolation until the low-odor probability scenarios. As in Figs 4 and 6, a shift to low-odor probability both decreases the distance at which flower-signals are encountered and increases failure rates in search flights. The predominant difference between the two search-path methods is a slight decrease in failure rates when using variable step lengths, a finding that is consistent with the fact that the latter method ran for approximately double the distance, creating a longer search path. Limitations and Future Directions. It is worth emphasizing that this experiment was done in silica. As such it is limited by the assumptions used to generate model results. These model results are strongly driven by visual and olfactory resolution: on the plant side from the strength of floral signal, and on the pollinator side from sensory sensitivity. While all of these variables were parameterized based on the ecology and physiology of the relevant plant-pollinator relationships, the absolute values returned by the model are less relevant than the trends. These trends raise interesting questions for future experimental work. For example, the indication that bumblebees with lower visual acuity first encounter smaller floral displays via odor plumes begets the question, will bumblebees searching for novel resources navigate with odor information alone? This phenomenon has pre- 40,49 viously been demonstrated on a small spatial scale , but remains to be shown at field-realistic scales. The sub- stantial number of model runs finishing with simultaneous odor and visual signal encounter raises the question, does odor information make a minimally resolvable visual cue more salient? Again, work on small spatial scales 51,88 shows improved learning and recognition of food resources with multimodal sensory information , but how this operates on large spatial scales is less clear. Finally, this computational model provides an alarming context for recent work on odor pollution in bee behavior. While that work has largely focused on laboratory investiga- tions, decreasing plume distance is likely to have profound impacts on foraging efficiency in bumblebees and other odor-guided pollinators. These results, in combination with recent computational findings on air pollution decreasing distance travelled by floral scent , strongly indicate that relevant field work to ground-truth theoret - ical concerns is necessary. Data Availability Data generated by model runs are available upon request. References 1. Hegland, S. J., Nielsen, A., Lazaro, A., Bjerknes, A. L. & Totland, Ø. How does climate warming ae ff ct plant‐pollinator interactions? Ecol Letters 12, 184–195 (2009). 2. Klein, A. M. et al. Importance of pollinators in changing landscapes for world crops. Proc R Soc Biol 274, 303–313 (2008). 3. Suzuki, K., Dohzono, I. & Hiei, K. Evolution of pollinator generalization in bumblebee-pollinated plants. Plant Spec Biol 22, 141–159 (2007). 4. Williams, P. H. & Osborne, J. L. Bumblebee vulnerability and conservation world-wide. Apidologie 40, 367–387 (2009). 5. Cameron, S. A. et al. Patterns of widespread decline in North American bumble bees. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108, 662–667 (2011). 6. Goulson, D., Lye, G. C. & Darvill, B. Decline and conservation of bumble bees. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 53, 191–208 (2008). 7. Potts, S. G. et al. Global pollinator declines: trends, impacts and drivers. Trends Ecol Evol 25, 345–353 (2010). 8. Whitehorn, P. R., O’Connor, S., Wackers, F. L. & Goulson, D. Neonicotinoid pesticide reduces bumble bee colony growth and queen production. Science 336, 351–352 (2012). 9. Gill, R. J. & Raine, N. E. Chronic impairment of bumblebee natural foraging behaviour induced by sublethal pesticide exposure. Funct Ecol 28, 1459–1471 (2014). 10. Owen, R. E., Rodd, F. H. & Plowright, R. C. Sex ratios in bumble bee colonies: complications due to orphaning? Behav Ecol Sociobiol 7, 287–291 (1980). 11. Woodgate, J. L., Makinson, J. C., Lim, K. S., Reynolds, A. M. & Chittka, L. Life-long radar tracking of bumblebees. Plos One 11, e0160333 (2016). 12. Lihoreau, M. et al. Radar tracking and motion-sensitive cameras on flowers reveal the development of pollinator multi-destination routes over large spatial scales. Plos Biol 10, e1001392 (2012). 13. Lunau, K. Innate recognition of flowers by bumble bees: orientation of antennae to visual stamen signals. Can. J. Zool. 70, 2139–2144 (1992). 14. Cnaani, J., o Th mson, J. D. & Papaj, D. R. Flower Choice and Learning in Foraging Bumblebees: Ee ff cts of Variation in Nectar Volume and Concentration. Ethology 112, 278–285 (2006). Scientific REPO R TS | (2018) 8:14590 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-32897-y 8 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ 15. Heinrich, B. “Majoring” and ‘Minoring’ by Foraging Bumblebees, Bombus Vagans: An Experimental Analysis. Ecology 60, 245–255 (1979). 16. Cartar, R. V. Resource tracking by bumble bees: responses to plant-level differences in quality. Ecology 85, 2764–2771 (2004). 17. Ott, J. R., Real, L. A. & Silverfine, E. M. The effect of nectar variance on bumblebee patterns of movement and potential gene dispersal. Oikos 45, 333–340 (1985). 18. Saleh, N., Scott, A. G., Bryning, G. P. & Chittka, L. Distinguishing signals and cues: bumblebees use general footprints to generate adaptive behaviour at flowers and nest. Arthropod-Plant Inte 1, 119–127 (2007). 19. Osborne, J. L. et al. The ontogeny of bumblebee flight trajectories: from naïve explorers to experienced foragers. Plos One 8, e78681 (2013). 20. Graham, P. & Cheng, K. Ants use the panoramic skyline as a visual cue during navigation. Curr Biol 19, R935–R937 (2009). 21. Knaden, M. & Graham, P. The sensory ecology of ant navigation: from natural environments to neural mechanisms. Annu Rev Entomol 61, 63–76 (2015). 22. Dacke, M., Jundi, elB., Smolka, J., Byrne, M. & Baird, E. The role of the sun in the celestial compass of dung beetles. Phil Trans R Soc Biol 369, 20130036 (2014). 23. Pyke, G. H. Optimal foraging theory: a critical review. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 15, 523–575 (1984). 24. Viswanathan, G. M. et al. Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses. Nature 381, 413–415 (1996). 25. Osborne, J. L. et al. A landscape‐scale study of bumble bee foraging range and constancy, using harmonic radar. J Appl Ecol 36, 519–533 (1999). 26. Cole, B. J. Fractal time in animal behaviour: the movement activity of Drosophila. Anim Behav 50, 1317–1324 (1995). 27. Ramos-Fernández, G., Mateos, J. L. & Miramontes, O. Lévy walk patterns in the foraging movements of spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi). Behav Ecol Sociobiol 55, 223–230 (2004). 28. Reynolds, A. M., Smith, A. D., Reynolds, D. R., Carreck, N. L. & Osborne, J. L. Honeybees perform optimal scale-free searching flights when attempting to locate a food source. J Exp Biol 210, 3763–3770 (2007). 29. Lenz, F., Chechkin, A. V. & Klages, R. Constructing a Stochastic Model of Bumblebee Flights from Experimental Data. Plos One 8, e59036 (2013). 30. James, A., Plank, M. J. & Edwards, A. M. Assessing Levy walks as models of animal foraging. J Roy Soc Interface 8, 1233–1247 (2011). 31. Pyke, G. H. Understanding movements of organisms: it’s time to abandon the Lévy foraging hypothesis. Methods Ecol Evol 6, 1–16 (2015). 32. Bartumeus, F., da Luz, M. G. E., Viswanathan, G. M. & Catalan, J. Animal search strategies: a quantitative random-walk analysis. Ecology 86, 3078–3087 (2005). 33. Edwards, A. M. et al. Revisiting Lévy flight search patterns of wandering albatrosses, bumblebees and deer. Nature 449, 1044–1048 (2007). 34. Leonard, A. S., Dornhaus, A. & Papaj, D. R. In Evolution of Plant-Pollinator Relationships (ed. Patiny, S.) 279–300 (books.google.com, 2011). 35. Leonard, A. S., Dornhaus, A. & Papaj, D. R. Flowers help bees cope with uncertainty: signal detection and the function of floral complexity. J Exp Biol 214, 113–121 (2011). 36. Ruedenauer, F. A., Spaethe, J. & Leonhardt, S. D. Hungry for quality—individual bumblebees forage flexibly to collect high-quality pollen. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 70, 1–9 (2016). 37. Goyret, J. The role of mechanosensory input in flower handling efficiency and learning by Manduca sexta. J Exp Biol 209, 1585–1593 (2006). 38. Macuda, T., Gegear, R. J., Laverty, T. M. & Timney, B. Behavioural assessment of visual acuity in bumblebees (Bombus impatiens). J Exp Biol 204, 559–564 (2001). 39. McFrederick, Q. S., Kathilankal, J. C. & Fuentes, J. D. Air pollution modifies floral scent trails. Atmos Environ 42, 2336–2348 (2008). 40. Sprayberry, J. D. H., Ritter, K. A. & Riffell, J. A. The effect of olfactory exposure to non-insecticidal agrochemicals on bumblebee foraging behavior. Plos One 8, e76273 (2013). 41. Farré-Armengol, G. et al. Ozone degrades floral scent and reduces pollinator attraction to flowers. New Phytol 209, 152–160 (2015). 42. Riveros, A. J. & Gronenberg, W. Olfactory learning and memory in the bumblebee Bombus occidentalis. Naturwissenschaen ft 96, 851–856 (2009). 43. Sommerlandt, F. M. J., Rössler, W. & Spaethe, J. Elemental and non-elemental olfactory learning using PER conditioning in the bumblebee, Bombus terrestris. Apidologie 45, 106–115 (2013). 44. Toda, N. R. T., Song, J. & Nieh, J. C. Bumblebees exhibit the memory spacing effect. Naturwissenschaen ft 96, 1185–1191 (2009). 45. Kunze, J. & Gumbert, A. The combined effect of color and odor on flower choice behavior of bumble bees in flower mimicry systems. Behav Ecol 12, 447–456 (2001). 46. Odell, E., Raguso, R. A. & Jones, K. N. Bumblebee foraging responses to variation in floral scent and color in snapdragons (Antirrhinum: Scrophulariaceae). Am Midl Nat (1999). 47. Goulson, D., Stout, J. C. & Langley, J. Identity and function of scent marks deposited by foraging bumblebees. J Chem Ecol (2000). 48. Wilms, J. & Eltz, T. Foraging scent marks of bumblebees: footprint cues rather than pheromone signals. Naturwissenschaen ft 95, 149–153 (2007). 49. Spaethe, J., Brockmann, A., Halbig, C. & Tautz, J. Size determines antennal sensitivity and behavioral threshold to odors in bumblebee workers. Naturwissenschaen ft 94, 733–739 (2007). 50. Raine, N. E. & Chittka, L. The adaptive significance of sensory bias in a foraging context: floral colour preferences in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. Plos One 2, e556–8 (2007). 51. Kulahci, I. G., Dornhaus, A. & Papaj, D. R. Multimodal signals enhance decision making in foraging bumble-bees. Proc R Soc Biol 275, 797–802 (2008). 52. Molet, M., Chittka, L. & Raine, N. E. How floral odours are learned inside the bumblebee (Bombus terrestris) nest. Naturwissenschaen ft 96, 213–219 (2008). 53. Witjes, S. & Eltz, T. Influence of scent deposits on flower choice: experiments in an artificial flower array with bumblebees. Apidologie 38, 12–18 (2007). 54. Suchet, C. et al. Floral scent variation in two Antirrhinum majus subspecies influences the choice of naïve bumblebees. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 65, 1015–1027 (2010). 55. Hudon, T. M. & Plowright, C. M. S. Trapped: assessing attractiveness of potential food sources to bumblebees. J Insect Behav 24, 144–158 (2010). 56. Dyer, A. G., Spaethe, J. & Prack, S. Comparative psychophysics of bumblebee and honeybee colour discrimination and object detection. J Comp Physiol A 194, 617–627 (2008). 57. Leadbeater, E. & Chittka, L. A new mode of information transfer in foraging bumblebees? Curr Biol 15, R447–8 (2005). 58. Ings, T. C., Raine, N. E. & Chittka, L. A population comparison of the strength and persistence of innate colour preference and learning speed in the bumblebee Bombus terrestris. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 63, 1207–1218 (2009). 59. Renner, M. A. & Nieh, J. C. Bumble bee olfactory information flow and contact-based foraging activation. Insect. Soc. 55, 417–424 (2008). 60. Walther Hellwig, K. & Frankl, R. Foraging habitats and foraging distances of bumblebees, Bombus spp. (Hym., Apidae), in an agricultural landscape. J Appl Entomol 124, 299–306 (2000). Scientific REPO R TS | (2018) 8:14590 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-32897-y 9 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ 61. Murlis, J., Willis, M. A. & Carde, R. T. Spatial and temporal structures of pheromone plumes in fields and forests. Physiol Entomol 25, 211–222 (2000). 62. Spaethe, J. & Chittka, L. Interindividual variation of eye optics and single object resolution in bumblebees. J Exp Biol 206, 3447–3453 (2003). 63. Riffell, J. A. et al . Flower discrimination by pollinators in a dynamic chemical environment. Science 344, 1515–1518 (2014). 64. Heinrich, B. Resource heterogeneity and patterns of movement in foraging bumblebees. Oecologia 40, 235–245 (1979). 65. Heinrich, B. Bumblebee economics (Harvard University Press, 2004). 66. Mulloney, B. Impulse patterns in the flight motor neurones of Bombus californicus and Oncopeltus fasciatus. J Exp Biol 52, 59–77 (1970). 67. Fuentes, J. D., Chamecki, M., Roulston, T., Chen, B. & Pratt, K. R. Air pollutants degrade floral scents and increase insect foraging times. Atmos Environ 141, 361–374 (9999). 68. Wolf, S. et al. Optimal search patterns in honeybee orientation flights are robust against emerging infectious diseases. Sci. Rep. 6, 1–10 (2016). 69. Becher, M. A. et al. BEESCOUT: A model of bee scouting behaviour and a software tool for characterizing nectar/pollen landscapes for BEEHAVE. Ecol Model 340, 126–133 (2016). 70. James, A., Pitchford, J. W. & Plank, M. J. Efficient or Inaccurate? Analytical and Numerical Modelling of Random Search Strategies. Bull. Math. Biol. 72, 896–913 (2009). 71. Viswanathan, G. M. et al. Optimizing the success of random searches. Nature 401, 911–914 (1999). 72. Wagenius, S. & Lyon, S. P. Reproduction of Echinacea angustjfolia in fragmented prairie is pollen-limited but not pollinator-limited. Ecology 91, 733–742 (2010). 73. Zorn-Arnold, B. & Howe, H. F. Density and seed set in a self-compatible forb, Penstemon digitalis (Plantaginaceae), with multiple pollinators. Am J Bot 94, 1594–1602 (2007). 74. Parachnowitsch, A. L. & Kessler, A. Pollinators exert natural selection on flower size and floral display in Penstemon digitalis . New Phytol (2010). 75. Parachnowitsch, A. L., Raguso, R. A. & Kessler, A. Phenotypic selection to increase floral scent emission, but not flower size or colour in bee-pollinated Penstemon digitalis. New Phytol 195, 667–675 (2012). 76. Riffell, J. A., Abrell, L. & Hildebrand, J. G. Physical processes and real-time chemical measurement of the insect olfactory environment. Journal Of Chemical Ecology 34, 837–853 (2008). 77. Willis, M. A., Ford, E. A. & Avondet, J. L. Odor tracking flight of male Manduca sexta moths along plumes of different cross- sectional area. J Comp Physiol A 199, 1015–1036 (2013). 78. Hansson, B. S., Carlsson, M. A. & Kalinovà, B. Olfactory activation patterns in the antennal lobe of the sphinx moth, Manduca sexta. J Comp Physiol A 189, 301–308 (2003). 79. Martin, J. P., Lei, H., Riffell, J. A. & Hildebrand, J. G. Synchronous firing of antennal-lobe projection neurons encodes the behaviorally effective ratio of sex-pheromone components in male Manduca sexta. J Comp Physiol A 199, 963–979 (2013). 80. Riffell, J. A., Lei, H. & Hildebrand, J. G. Neural correlates of behavior in the moth Manduca sexta in response to complex odors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106, 19219–19226 (2009). 81. Nijhout, H. F., Davidowitz, G. & Ro, D ff . A. A quantitative analysis of the mechanism that controls body size in Manduca sexta. J Biol 5, 16 (2006). 82. Spaethe, J. & Weidenmüller, A. Size variation and foraging rate in bumblebees (Bombus terrestris). Insect. Soc. 49, 142–146 (2002). 83. Fraser, A. M., Mechaber, W. L. & Hildebrand, J. G. Electroantennographic and behavioral responses of the sphinx moth Manduca sexta to host plant headspace volatiles. J Chem Ecol 29, 1813–1833 (2003). 84. Lunau, K. Innate flower recognition in bumblebees (Bombus terrestris, B. lucorum; Apidae): optical signals from stamens as landing reaction releasers. Ethology 88, 203–214 (1991). 85. Spaethe, J., Chittka, L. & Skorupski, P. Visual search and decision making in bees: time, speed, and accuracy. Int J Comp Psychol (2006). 86. Girling, R. D., Higbee, B. S. & Carde, R. T. The plume also rises: trajectories of pheromone plumes issuing from point sources in an orchard canopy at night. J Chem Ecol 39, 1150–1160 (2013). 87. Girling, R. D., Lusebrink, I., Farthing, E., Newman, T. A. & Poppy, G. M. Diesel exhaust rapidly degrades floral odours used by honeybees. Sci Rep 3 (2013). 88. Leonard, A. S. & Masek, P. Multisensory integration of colors and scents: insights from bees and flowers. J Comp Physiol A 200, 463–474 (2014). Acknowledgements J.S. thanks Muhlenberg College for research and travel funding that contributed to the development of this work. J.S. thanks the anonymous reviewers for valuable feedback on this manuscript. Author Contributions J.S. constructed the computational model, made the figures, wrote the manuscript and reviewed the manuscript. Additional Information Competing Interests: The author declares no competing interests. Publisher's note: Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Cre- ative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons license, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons license and your intended use is not per- mitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. © The Author(s) 2018 Scientific REPO R TS | (2018) 8:14590 | DOI:10.1038/s41598-018-32897-y 10
Scientific Reports – Springer Journals
Published: Oct 1, 2018
You can share this free article with as many people as you like with the url below! We hope you enjoy this feature!
Read and print from thousands of top scholarly journals.
Already have an account? Log in
Bookmark this article. You can see your Bookmarks on your DeepDyve Library.
To save an article, log in first, or sign up for a DeepDyve account if you don’t already have one.
Copy and paste the desired citation format or use the link below to download a file formatted for EndNote
Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
All DeepDyve websites use cookies to improve your online experience. They were placed on your computer when you launched this website. You can change your cookie settings through your browser.