Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
S. Emlen, L. Oring (1977)
Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems.Science, 197 4300
D. Rubenstein, C. Botero, E. Lacey (2016)
Discrete but variable structure of animal societies leads to the false perception of a social continuumRoyal Society Open Science, 3
D. Lukas, T. Clutton‐Brock (2013)
The Evolution of Social Monogamy in MammalsScience, 341
( Clutton-BrockTH 1989 Female transfer and inbreeding avoidance in social mammals. Nature. 337:70–72.2909891)
Clutton-BrockTH 1989 Female transfer and inbreeding avoidance in social mammals. Nature. 337:70–72.2909891Clutton-BrockTH 1989 Female transfer and inbreeding avoidance in social mammals. Nature. 337:70–72.2909891, Clutton-BrockTH 1989 Female transfer and inbreeding avoidance in social mammals. Nature. 337:70–72.2909891
C. Kvarnemo, I. Ahnesjö (1996)
The dynamics of operational sex ratios and competition for mates.Trends in ecology & evolution, 11 10
( RichardA 1974 Intra-specific variation in the social organization and ecology of Propithecus verreauxi. Folia Primatol (Basel). 22:178–207.4474117)
RichardA 1974 Intra-specific variation in the social organization and ecology of Propithecus verreauxi. Folia Primatol (Basel). 22:178–207.4474117RichardA 1974 Intra-specific variation in the social organization and ecology of Propithecus verreauxi. Folia Primatol (Basel). 22:178–207.4474117, RichardA 1974 Intra-specific variation in the social organization and ecology of Propithecus verreauxi. Folia Primatol (Basel). 22:178–207.4474117
Nathan Myhrvold, Elita Baldridge, B. Chan, D. Sivam, Daniel Freeman, S. Ernest (2015)
An amniote life-history database to perform comparative analyses with birds, mammals, and reptilesEcology, 96
G. Stone, S. Nee, J. Felsenstein (2011)
Controlling for non-independence in comparative analysis of patterns across populations within speciesPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366
C. Nievergelt, T. Mutschler, A. Feistner, D. Woodruff (2002)
Social system of the Alaotran gentle lemur (Hapalemur griseus alaotrensis): genetic characterization of group composition and mating systemAmerican Journal of Primatology, 57
M. McCarthy, R. Citroen, S. McCall (2008)
Allometric Scaling and Bayesian Priors for Annual Survival of Birds and MammalsThe American Naturalist, 172
Andrea Weidt, A. Lindholm, B. König (2014)
Communal nursing in wild house mice is not a by-product of group living: Females chooseDie Naturwissenschaften, 101
( LukasD, Clutton-BrockTH 2013 The evolution of social monogamy in mammals. Science. 341:526–530.23896459)
LukasD, Clutton-BrockTH 2013 The evolution of social monogamy in mammals. Science. 341:526–530.23896459LukasD, Clutton-BrockTH 2013 The evolution of social monogamy in mammals. Science. 341:526–530.23896459, LukasD, Clutton-BrockTH 2013 The evolution of social monogamy in mammals. Science. 341:526–530.23896459
J. Rolland, F. Condamine, F. Jiguet, H. Morlon (2014)
Faster Speciation and Reduced Extinction in the Tropics Contribute to the Mammalian Latitudinal Diversity GradientPLoS Biology, 12
Jácint Tökölyi, Julia Schmidt, Z. Barta (2014)
Climate and mammalian life historiesBiological Journal of The Linnean Society, 111
( HindeK, MilliganLA 2011 Primate milk: proximate mechanisms and ultimate perspectives. Evol Anthropol. 20:9–23.22034080)
HindeK, MilliganLA 2011 Primate milk: proximate mechanisms and ultimate perspectives. Evol Anthropol. 20:9–23.22034080HindeK, MilliganLA 2011 Primate milk: proximate mechanisms and ultimate perspectives. Evol Anthropol. 20:9–23.22034080, HindeK, MilliganLA 2011 Primate milk: proximate mechanisms and ultimate perspectives. Evol Anthropol. 20:9–23.22034080
L. Ebensperger (2001)
A review of the evolutionary causes of rodent group-livingActa Theriologica, 46
( HarveyPH, PagelMD 1991 The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.)
HarveyPH, PagelMD 1991 The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.HarveyPH, PagelMD 1991 The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press., HarveyPH, PagelMD 1991 The comparative method in evolutionary biology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
C. Schradin (2017)
Comparative studies need to rely both on sound natural history data and on excellent statistical analysisRoyal Society Open Science, 4
S. Shultz, Robin Dunbar (2007)
The evolution of the social brain: anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebratesProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 274
P. Kappeler (1998)
Nests, tree holes, and the evolution of primate life histories.American journal of primatology, 46 1
( van SchaikCP, KappelerPM 1993 Life history, activity period and lemur social systems. In: GanzhornJ, KappelerPM, editors. Lemur social systems and their ecological basis. Boston: Springer p. 241–260.)
van SchaikCP, KappelerPM 1993 Life history, activity period and lemur social systems. In: GanzhornJ, KappelerPM, editors. Lemur social systems and their ecological basis. Boston: Springer p. 241–260.van SchaikCP, KappelerPM 1993 Life history, activity period and lemur social systems. In: GanzhornJ, KappelerPM, editors. Lemur social systems and their ecological basis. Boston: Springer p. 241–260., van SchaikCP, KappelerPM 1993 Life history, activity period and lemur social systems. In: GanzhornJ, KappelerPM, editors. Lemur social systems and their ecological basis. Boston: Springer p. 241–260.
( JohnsonMS, ThomsonSC, SpeakmanJR 2001 Limits to sustained energy intake: II. Inter-relationships between resting metabolic rate, life-history traits and morphology in Mus musculus. J Exp Biol. 204:1937–1946.11441035)
JohnsonMS, ThomsonSC, SpeakmanJR 2001 Limits to sustained energy intake: II. Inter-relationships between resting metabolic rate, life-history traits and morphology in Mus musculus. J Exp Biol. 204:1937–1946.11441035JohnsonMS, ThomsonSC, SpeakmanJR 2001 Limits to sustained energy intake: II. Inter-relationships between resting metabolic rate, life-history traits and morphology in Mus musculus. J Exp Biol. 204:1937–1946.11441035, JohnsonMS, ThomsonSC, SpeakmanJR 2001 Limits to sustained energy intake: II. Inter-relationships between resting metabolic rate, life-history traits and morphology in Mus musculus. J Exp Biol. 204:1937–1946.11441035
J. Magalhães, Joana Cósta (2009)
A database of vertebrate longevity records and their relation to other life‐history traitsJournal of Evolutionary Biology, 22
( RubensteinDI 1989 Life history and social organization in arid adapted ungulates. J Arid Environ. 17:145–156.)
RubensteinDI 1989 Life history and social organization in arid adapted ungulates. J Arid Environ. 17:145–156.RubensteinDI 1989 Life history and social organization in arid adapted ungulates. J Arid Environ. 17:145–156., RubensteinDI 1989 Life history and social organization in arid adapted ungulates. J Arid Environ. 17:145–156.
(1968)
Factors influencing clutch size, egg size and the incubation period in waterfowl and other freshwater aquatic birds
( WeidtA, LindholmAK, KönigB 2014 Communal nursing in wild house mice is not a by-product of group living: females choose. Naturwissenschaften. 101:73–76.24389536)
WeidtA, LindholmAK, KönigB 2014 Communal nursing in wild house mice is not a by-product of group living: females choose. Naturwissenschaften. 101:73–76.24389536WeidtA, LindholmAK, KönigB 2014 Communal nursing in wild house mice is not a by-product of group living: females choose. Naturwissenschaften. 101:73–76.24389536, WeidtA, LindholmAK, KönigB 2014 Communal nursing in wild house mice is not a by-product of group living: females choose. Naturwissenschaften. 101:73–76.24389536
( BradburyJW, VehrencampSL 1977 Social organization and foraging in emballonurid bats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2:1–17.)
BradburyJW, VehrencampSL 1977 Social organization and foraging in emballonurid bats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2:1–17.BradburyJW, VehrencampSL 1977 Social organization and foraging in emballonurid bats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2:1–17., BradburyJW, VehrencampSL 1977 Social organization and foraging in emballonurid bats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2:1–17.
M. Hamilton, Ana Davidson, R. Sibly, James Brown (2011)
Universal scaling of production rates across mammalian lineagesProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278
( KamilarJM, CooperN 2013 Phylogenetic signal in primate behaviour, ecology and life history. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 368:20120341.23569289)
KamilarJM, CooperN 2013 Phylogenetic signal in primate behaviour, ecology and life history. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 368:20120341.23569289KamilarJM, CooperN 2013 Phylogenetic signal in primate behaviour, ecology and life history. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 368:20120341.23569289, KamilarJM, CooperN 2013 Phylogenetic signal in primate behaviour, ecology and life history. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 368:20120341.23569289
( AndelmanSJ 1986 Ecological and social determinants of cercopithecine mating patterns. In: RubensteinDI, WranghamRW, editors. Ecological aspects of social evolution: birds and mammals. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press p. 201–216.)
AndelmanSJ 1986 Ecological and social determinants of cercopithecine mating patterns. In: RubensteinDI, WranghamRW, editors. Ecological aspects of social evolution: birds and mammals. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press p. 201–216.AndelmanSJ 1986 Ecological and social determinants of cercopithecine mating patterns. In: RubensteinDI, WranghamRW, editors. Ecological aspects of social evolution: birds and mammals. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press p. 201–216., AndelmanSJ 1986 Ecological and social determinants of cercopithecine mating patterns. In: RubensteinDI, WranghamRW, editors. Ecological aspects of social evolution: birds and mammals. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press p. 201–216.
( WilliamsGC 1966 Natural selection, the costs of reproduction, and a refinement of Lack’s principle. Am Naturalist. 100:687–690.)
WilliamsGC 1966 Natural selection, the costs of reproduction, and a refinement of Lack’s principle. Am Naturalist. 100:687–690.WilliamsGC 1966 Natural selection, the costs of reproduction, and a refinement of Lack’s principle. Am Naturalist. 100:687–690., WilliamsGC 1966 Natural selection, the costs of reproduction, and a refinement of Lack’s principle. Am Naturalist. 100:687–690.
E. Charnov (1991)
Evolution of life history variation among female mammals.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 88
( LewisSE, PuseyAE 1997 Factors influencing the occurrence of communal care in plural breeding mammals. In: SolomonNG, FrenchJA, editors. Cooperative breeding in mammals. New York: Cambridge University Press p. 335–363.)
LewisSE, PuseyAE 1997 Factors influencing the occurrence of communal care in plural breeding mammals. In: SolomonNG, FrenchJA, editors. Cooperative breeding in mammals. New York: Cambridge University Press p. 335–363.LewisSE, PuseyAE 1997 Factors influencing the occurrence of communal care in plural breeding mammals. In: SolomonNG, FrenchJA, editors. Cooperative breeding in mammals. New York: Cambridge University Press p. 335–363., LewisSE, PuseyAE 1997 Factors influencing the occurrence of communal care in plural breeding mammals. In: SolomonNG, FrenchJA, editors. Cooperative breeding in mammals. New York: Cambridge University Press p. 335–363.
( RubensteinDR, AbbotP 2017 Conclusion: social synthesis: opportunities for comparative social evolution. In: RubensteinDR, AbbotP, editors. Comparative social evolution. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press p. 284–319.)
RubensteinDR, AbbotP 2017 Conclusion: social synthesis: opportunities for comparative social evolution. In: RubensteinDR, AbbotP, editors. Comparative social evolution. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press p. 284–319.RubensteinDR, AbbotP 2017 Conclusion: social synthesis: opportunities for comparative social evolution. In: RubensteinDR, AbbotP, editors. Comparative social evolution. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press p. 284–319., RubensteinDR, AbbotP 2017 Conclusion: social synthesis: opportunities for comparative social evolution. In: RubensteinDR, AbbotP, editors. Comparative social evolution. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press p. 284–319.
( BraudeS 2000 Dispersal and new colony formation in wild naked mole-rats: evidence against inbreeding as the system of mating. Behav Ecol. 11:7–12.)
BraudeS 2000 Dispersal and new colony formation in wild naked mole-rats: evidence against inbreeding as the system of mating. Behav Ecol. 11:7–12.BraudeS 2000 Dispersal and new colony formation in wild naked mole-rats: evidence against inbreeding as the system of mating. Behav Ecol. 11:7–12., BraudeS 2000 Dispersal and new colony formation in wild naked mole-rats: evidence against inbreeding as the system of mating. Behav Ecol. 11:7–12.
JL Gittleman, JL Gittleman (1989)
Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution
D. Lukas, V. Reynolds, C. Boesch, L. Vigilant (2005)
To what extent does living in a group mean living with kin?Molecular Ecology, 14
C. Riehl (2013)
Evolutionary routes to non-kin cooperative breeding in birdsProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 280
( HamiltonMJ, DavidsonAD, SiblyRM, BrownJH 2011 Universal scaling of production rates across mammalian lineages. Proc Biol Sci. 278:560–566.20798111)
HamiltonMJ, DavidsonAD, SiblyRM, BrownJH 2011 Universal scaling of production rates across mammalian lineages. Proc Biol Sci. 278:560–566.20798111HamiltonMJ, DavidsonAD, SiblyRM, BrownJH 2011 Universal scaling of production rates across mammalian lineages. Proc Biol Sci. 278:560–566.20798111, HamiltonMJ, DavidsonAD, SiblyRM, BrownJH 2011 Universal scaling of production rates across mammalian lineages. Proc Biol Sci. 278:560–566.20798111
S. Braude (2000)
Dispersal and new colony formation in wild naked mole-rats: evidence against inbreeding as the system of matingBehavioral Ecology, 11
J. Kamilar, N. Cooper (2013)
Phylogenetic signal in primate behaviour, ecology and life historyPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368
E. Lacey (2017)
Mammal SocietiesJournal of Mammalogy, 98
( WardA, WebsterM 2016 Sociality: the behaviour of group-living animals. Berlin (Germany): Springer.)
WardA, WebsterM 2016 Sociality: the behaviour of group-living animals. Berlin (Germany): Springer.WardA, WebsterM 2016 Sociality: the behaviour of group-living animals. Berlin (Germany): Springer., WardA, WebsterM 2016 Sociality: the behaviour of group-living animals. Berlin (Germany): Springer.
R. Wrangham (1980)
An Ecological Model of Female-Bonded Primate GroupsBehaviour, 75
C. Schradin, L. Hayes, N. Pillay, C. Bertelsmeier (2018)
The evolution of intraspecific variation in social organizationEthology, 124
D. Rubenstein, P. Abbot (2017)
Social Synthesis: Opportunities for Comparative Social Evolution
( ShultzS, OpieC, AtkinsonQD 2011 Stepwise evolution of stable sociality in primates. Nature. 479:219–222.22071768)
ShultzS, OpieC, AtkinsonQD 2011 Stepwise evolution of stable sociality in primates. Nature. 479:219–222.22071768ShultzS, OpieC, AtkinsonQD 2011 Stepwise evolution of stable sociality in primates. Nature. 479:219–222.22071768, ShultzS, OpieC, AtkinsonQD 2011 Stepwise evolution of stable sociality in primates. Nature. 479:219–222.22071768
T. Clutton‐Brock (1989)
Female transfer and inbreeding avoidance in social mammalsNature, 337
( KoenigWD, DickinsonJL, editors 2016 Cooperative breeding in vertebrates: studies of ecology, evolution, and behavior. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press.)
KoenigWD, DickinsonJL, editors 2016 Cooperative breeding in vertebrates: studies of ecology, evolution, and behavior. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press.KoenigWD, DickinsonJL, editors 2016 Cooperative breeding in vertebrates: studies of ecology, evolution, and behavior. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press., KoenigWD, DickinsonJL, editors 2016 Cooperative breeding in vertebrates: studies of ecology, evolution, and behavior. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press.
W. Leutenegger (1979)
Evolution of Litter Size in PrimatesThe American Naturalist, 114
P. Agnani, Cécile Kauffmann, L. Hayes, C. Schradin (2018)
Intra‐specific variation in social organization of StrepsirrhinesAmerican Journal of Primatology, 80
( RubensteinDR, BoteroCA, LaceyEA 2016 Discrete but variable structure of animal societies leads to the false perception of a social continuum. R Soc Open Sci. 3:160147.27293796)
RubensteinDR, BoteroCA, LaceyEA 2016 Discrete but variable structure of animal societies leads to the false perception of a social continuum. R Soc Open Sci. 3:160147.27293796RubensteinDR, BoteroCA, LaceyEA 2016 Discrete but variable structure of animal societies leads to the false perception of a social continuum. R Soc Open Sci. 3:160147.27293796, RubensteinDR, BoteroCA, LaceyEA 2016 Discrete but variable structure of animal societies leads to the false perception of a social continuum. R Soc Open Sci. 3:160147.27293796
A. Richard (1974)
Intra-specific variation in the social organization and ecology of Propithecus verreauxi.Folia primatologica; international journal of primatology, 22 2-3
Thaís Battistella, F. Cerezer, Jamile Bubadué, G. Melo, M. Graipel, N. Cáceres (2018)
Litter size variation in didelphid marsupials: evidence of phylogenetic constraints and adaptationBiological Journal of the Linnean Society
D. Lukas, T. Clutton‐Brock (2018)
Social complexity and kinship in animal societies.Ecology letters, 21 8
W. Koenig, J. Dickinson (2016)
Cooperative breeding in vertebrates : studies of ecology, evolution, and behavior
T. Clutton‐Brock, P. Harvey (2009)
Primates, brains and ecologyJournal of Zoology, 190
( LeuteneggerW 1979 Evolution of litter size in primates. Am Naturalist. 114:525–531.)
LeuteneggerW 1979 Evolution of litter size in primates. Am Naturalist. 114:525–531.LeuteneggerW 1979 Evolution of litter size in primates. Am Naturalist. 114:525–531., LeuteneggerW 1979 Evolution of litter size in primates. Am Naturalist. 114:525–531.
David Stephens, J. Krebs (2019)
Foraging Theory
( MartinRD, MacLarnonAM 1985 Gestation period, neonatal size and maternal investment in placental mammals. Nature. 313:220–223.)
MartinRD, MacLarnonAM 1985 Gestation period, neonatal size and maternal investment in placental mammals. Nature. 313:220–223.MartinRD, MacLarnonAM 1985 Gestation period, neonatal size and maternal investment in placental mammals. Nature. 313:220–223., MartinRD, MacLarnonAM 1985 Gestation period, neonatal size and maternal investment in placental mammals. Nature. 313:220–223.
S. Hamel, J. Gaillard, N. Yoccoz, A. Loison, C. Bonenfant, S. Descamps (2010)
Fitness costs of reproduction depend on life speed: empirical evidence from mammalian populations.Ecology letters, 13 7
P. Harvey, M. Pagel (1991)
The comparative method in evolutionary biology
K. S., Morgan Ernest (2003)
LIFE HISTORY CHARACTERISTICS OF PLACENTAL NONVOLANT MAMMALSEcology, 84
P. Jarman (1974)
The Social Organisation of Antelope in Relation To Their EcologyBehaviour, 48
C. Schradin, A. Lindholm, J. Johannesen, I. Schoepf, C. Yuen, B. König, N. Pillay (2012)
Social flexibility and social evolution in mammals: a case study of the African striped mouse (Rhabdomys pumilio)Molecular Ecology, 21
( LackD 1968 Factors influencing clutch size, egg size and the incubation period in waterfowl and other freshwater aquatic birds. In: LackD, editor. Ecological adaptations for breeding in birds. London (UK): Methuen p. 222–237.)
LackD 1968 Factors influencing clutch size, egg size and the incubation period in waterfowl and other freshwater aquatic birds. In: LackD, editor. Ecological adaptations for breeding in birds. London (UK): Methuen p. 222–237.LackD 1968 Factors influencing clutch size, egg size and the incubation period in waterfowl and other freshwater aquatic birds. In: LackD, editor. Ecological adaptations for breeding in birds. London (UK): Methuen p. 222–237., LackD 1968 Factors influencing clutch size, egg size and the incubation period in waterfowl and other freshwater aquatic birds. In: LackD, editor. Ecological adaptations for breeding in birds. London (UK): Methuen p. 222–237.
( SchradinC 2017 Comparative studies need to rely both on sound natural history data and on excellent statistical analysis. R Soc Open Sci. 4:170346.28989742)
SchradinC 2017 Comparative studies need to rely both on sound natural history data and on excellent statistical analysis. R Soc Open Sci. 4:170346.28989742SchradinC 2017 Comparative studies need to rely both on sound natural history data and on excellent statistical analysis. R Soc Open Sci. 4:170346.28989742, SchradinC 2017 Comparative studies need to rely both on sound natural history data and on excellent statistical analysis. R Soc Open Sci. 4:170346.28989742
R. Team (2014)
R: A language and environment for statistical computing.MSOR connections, 1
( StephensDW, KrebsJR 1986 Foraging theory. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press.)
StephensDW, KrebsJR 1986 Foraging theory. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press.StephensDW, KrebsJR 1986 Foraging theory. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press., StephensDW, KrebsJR 1986 Foraging theory. Princeton (NJ): Princeton University Press.
S. Lewis, A. Pusey, N. Solomon, J. French (1996)
Factors Influencing the Occurrence of Communal Care in Plural Breeding Mammals
( SpeakmanJR 2008 The physiological costs of reproduction in small mammals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 363:375–398.17686735)
SpeakmanJR 2008 The physiological costs of reproduction in small mammals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 363:375–398.17686735SpeakmanJR 2008 The physiological costs of reproduction in small mammals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 363:375–398.17686735, SpeakmanJR 2008 The physiological costs of reproduction in small mammals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 363:375–398.17686735
( MyhrvoldNP, BaldridgeE, ChanB, SivamD, FreemanDL, ErnestSKM 2015 An amniote life-history database to perform comparative analyses with birds, mammals, and reptiles. Ecology. 96:3109.)
MyhrvoldNP, BaldridgeE, ChanB, SivamD, FreemanDL, ErnestSKM 2015 An amniote life-history database to perform comparative analyses with birds, mammals, and reptiles. Ecology. 96:3109.MyhrvoldNP, BaldridgeE, ChanB, SivamD, FreemanDL, ErnestSKM 2015 An amniote life-history database to perform comparative analyses with birds, mammals, and reptiles. Ecology. 96:3109., MyhrvoldNP, BaldridgeE, ChanB, SivamD, FreemanDL, ErnestSKM 2015 An amniote life-history database to perform comparative analyses with birds, mammals, and reptiles. Ecology. 96:3109.
D. Promislow, P. Harvey (1990)
Living fast and dying young: A comparative analysis of life‐history variation among mammalsJournal of Zoology, 220
( JonesKE, BielbyJ, CardilloM, FritzSA, O’DellJ, OrmeCDL, SafiK, SechrestW, BoakesEH, CarboneC, et al 2009 PanTHERIA: a species‐level database of life history, ecology, and geography of extant and recently extinct mammals. Ecological Archives E090‐184. Ecology. 90:2648–2648.)
JonesKE, BielbyJ, CardilloM, FritzSA, O’DellJ, OrmeCDL, SafiK, SechrestW, BoakesEH, CarboneC, et al 2009 PanTHERIA: a species‐level database of life history, ecology, and geography of extant and recently extinct mammals. Ecological Archives E090‐184. Ecology. 90:2648–2648.JonesKE, BielbyJ, CardilloM, FritzSA, O’DellJ, OrmeCDL, SafiK, SechrestW, BoakesEH, CarboneC, et al 2009 PanTHERIA: a species‐level database of life history, ecology, and geography of extant and recently extinct mammals. Ecological Archives E090‐184. Ecology. 90:2648–2648., JonesKE, BielbyJ, CardilloM, FritzSA, O’DellJ, OrmeCDL, SafiK, SechrestW, BoakesEH, CarboneC, et al 2009 PanTHERIA: a species‐level database of life history, ecology, and geography of extant and recently extinct mammals. Ecological Archives E090‐184. Ecology. 90:2648–2648.
A. Ward, M. Webster (2016)
Sociality: The Behaviour of Group-Living Animals
( JarvisJU 1981 Eusociality in a mammal: cooperative breeding in naked mole-rat colonies. Science. 212:571–573.7209555)
JarvisJU 1981 Eusociality in a mammal: cooperative breeding in naked mole-rat colonies. Science. 212:571–573.7209555JarvisJU 1981 Eusociality in a mammal: cooperative breeding in naked mole-rat colonies. Science. 212:571–573.7209555, JarvisJU 1981 Eusociality in a mammal: cooperative breeding in naked mole-rat colonies. Science. 212:571–573.7209555
( LukasD, HuchardE 2019 The evolution of infanticide by females in mammals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 374:20180075.31303157)
LukasD, HuchardE 2019 The evolution of infanticide by females in mammals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 374:20180075.31303157LukasD, HuchardE 2019 The evolution of infanticide by females in mammals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 374:20180075.31303157, LukasD, HuchardE 2019 The evolution of infanticide by females in mammals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 374:20180075.31303157
D. Rubenstein (1989)
Life history and social organization in arid adapted ungulatesJournal of Arid Environments, 17
J. Eisenberg (1981)
The Mammalian Radiations: An Analysis of Trends in Evolution, Adaptation, and Behavior
J. Gittleman (1986)
Carnivore brain size, behavioral ecology and phylogenyJournal of Mammalogy, 67
M. Miles, A. Jaeggi, M. Festa‐Bianchet, C. Schradin, L. Hayes (2019)
Intraspecific variation in Artiodactyla social organisation: A Bayesian phylogenetic multilevel analysis of detailed population-level databioRxiv
( HadfieldJD, NakagawaS 2010 General quantitative genetic methods for comparative biology: phylogenies, taxonomies and multi-trait models for continuous and categorical characters. J Evol Biol. 23:494–508.20070460)
HadfieldJD, NakagawaS 2010 General quantitative genetic methods for comparative biology: phylogenies, taxonomies and multi-trait models for continuous and categorical characters. J Evol Biol. 23:494–508.20070460HadfieldJD, NakagawaS 2010 General quantitative genetic methods for comparative biology: phylogenies, taxonomies and multi-trait models for continuous and categorical characters. J Evol Biol. 23:494–508.20070460, HadfieldJD, NakagawaS 2010 General quantitative genetic methods for comparative biology: phylogenies, taxonomies and multi-trait models for continuous and categorical characters. J Evol Biol. 23:494–508.20070460
( EbenspergerLA 2001 A review of the evolutionary causes of rodent group-living. Acta Theriol. 46:115–144.)
EbenspergerLA 2001 A review of the evolutionary causes of rodent group-living. Acta Theriol. 46:115–144.EbenspergerLA 2001 A review of the evolutionary causes of rodent group-living. Acta Theriol. 46:115–144., EbenspergerLA 2001 A review of the evolutionary causes of rodent group-living. Acta Theriol. 46:115–144.
( HamelS, GaillardJM, YoccozNG, LoisonA, BonenfantC, DescampsS 2010 Fitness costs of reproduction depend on life speed: empirical evidence from mammalian populations. Ecol Lett. 13:915–935.20482573)
HamelS, GaillardJM, YoccozNG, LoisonA, BonenfantC, DescampsS 2010 Fitness costs of reproduction depend on life speed: empirical evidence from mammalian populations. Ecol Lett. 13:915–935.20482573HamelS, GaillardJM, YoccozNG, LoisonA, BonenfantC, DescampsS 2010 Fitness costs of reproduction depend on life speed: empirical evidence from mammalian populations. Ecol Lett. 13:915–935.20482573, HamelS, GaillardJM, YoccozNG, LoisonA, BonenfantC, DescampsS 2010 Fitness costs of reproduction depend on life speed: empirical evidence from mammalian populations. Ecol Lett. 13:915–935.20482573
S. Shultz, Chris Opie, Q. Atkinson (2011)
Stepwise evolution of stable sociality in primatesNature, 479
J. Hadfield, S Nakagawa (2010)
General quantitative genetic methods for comparative biology: phylogenies, taxonomies and multi‐trait models for continuous and categorical charactersJournal of Evolutionary Biology, 23
D. Fisher, I. Owens (2000)
Female home range size and the evolution of social organization in macropod marsupialsJournal of Animal Ecology, 69
( WolffJO, ShermanPW 2007 Rodent societies: an ecological and evolutionary perspective. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.)
WolffJO, ShermanPW 2007 Rodent societies: an ecological and evolutionary perspective. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.WolffJO, ShermanPW 2007 Rodent societies: an ecological and evolutionary perspective. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press., WolffJO, ShermanPW 2007 Rodent societies: an ecological and evolutionary perspective. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.
( HadfieldJD 2010 MCMCglmm: Markov chain Monte Carlo methods for generalized linear mixed models Available from: cran.uvigo.es/web/packages/MCMCglmm/vignettes/Tutorial.pdf (accessed 26 March 2015).)
HadfieldJD 2010 MCMCglmm: Markov chain Monte Carlo methods for generalized linear mixed models Available from: cran.uvigo.es/web/packages/MCMCglmm/vignettes/Tutorial.pdf (accessed 26 March 2015).HadfieldJD 2010 MCMCglmm: Markov chain Monte Carlo methods for generalized linear mixed models Available from: cran.uvigo.es/web/packages/MCMCglmm/vignettes/Tutorial.pdf (accessed 26 March 2015)., HadfieldJD 2010 MCMCglmm: Markov chain Monte Carlo methods for generalized linear mixed models Available from: cran.uvigo.es/web/packages/MCMCglmm/vignettes/Tutorial.pdf (accessed 26 March 2015).
( McCarthyMA, CitroenR, McCallSC 2008 Allometric scaling and Bayesian priors for annual survival of birds and mammals. Am Nat. 172:216–222.18605902)
McCarthyMA, CitroenR, McCallSC 2008 Allometric scaling and Bayesian priors for annual survival of birds and mammals. Am Nat. 172:216–222.18605902McCarthyMA, CitroenR, McCallSC 2008 Allometric scaling and Bayesian priors for annual survival of birds and mammals. Am Nat. 172:216–222.18605902, McCarthyMA, CitroenR, McCallSC 2008 Allometric scaling and Bayesian priors for annual survival of birds and mammals. Am Nat. 172:216–222.18605902
SJ Andelman, DI Rubenstein, RW Wrangham (1986)
Ecological aspects of social evolution: birds and mammals
J. Bradbury, S. Vehrencamp (1977)
Social organization and foraging in emballonurid batsBehavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 2
( CharnovEL 1991 Evolution of life history variation among female mammals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 88:1134–1137.1996315)
CharnovEL 1991 Evolution of life history variation among female mammals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 88:1134–1137.1996315CharnovEL 1991 Evolution of life history variation among female mammals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 88:1134–1137.1996315, CharnovEL 1991 Evolution of life history variation among female mammals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 88:1134–1137.1996315
F. Dalerum (2007)
Phylogenetic reconstruction of carnivore social organizationsJournal of Zoology, 273
( BielbyJ, MaceGM, Bininda-EmondsOR, CardilloM, GittlemanJL, JonesKE, OrmeCD, PurvisA 2007 The fast-slow continuum in mammalian life history: an empirical reevaluation. Am Nat. 169:748–757.17479461)
BielbyJ, MaceGM, Bininda-EmondsOR, CardilloM, GittlemanJL, JonesKE, OrmeCD, PurvisA 2007 The fast-slow continuum in mammalian life history: an empirical reevaluation. Am Nat. 169:748–757.17479461BielbyJ, MaceGM, Bininda-EmondsOR, CardilloM, GittlemanJL, JonesKE, OrmeCD, PurvisA 2007 The fast-slow continuum in mammalian life history: an empirical reevaluation. Am Nat. 169:748–757.17479461, BielbyJ, MaceGM, Bininda-EmondsOR, CardilloM, GittlemanJL, JonesKE, OrmeCD, PurvisA 2007 The fast-slow continuum in mammalian life history: an empirical reevaluation. Am Nat. 169:748–757.17479461
J. Gittleman (1989)
Carnivore Group Living: Comparative Trends
C. Schaik, P. Kappeler (1993)
Life History, Activity Period and Lemur Social Systems
J. Wolff, P. Sherman (2007)
Rodent Societies: An Ecological and Evolutionary Perspective
( KappelerPM, PereiraME, editors. 2003 Primate life histories and socioecology. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.)
KappelerPM, PereiraME, editors. 2003 Primate life histories and socioecology. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.KappelerPM, PereiraME, editors. 2003 Primate life histories and socioecology. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press., KappelerPM, PereiraME, editors. 2003 Primate life histories and socioecology. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.
(2010)
MCMCglmm: Markov chain Monte Carlo methods for generalized linear mixed models
( KaufmannJH 1974 Habitat use and social organization of nine sympatric species of macropodid marsupials. J Mammal. 55:66–80.)
KaufmannJH 1974 Habitat use and social organization of nine sympatric species of macropodid marsupials. J Mammal. 55:66–80.KaufmannJH 1974 Habitat use and social organization of nine sympatric species of macropodid marsupials. J Mammal. 55:66–80., KaufmannJH 1974 Habitat use and social organization of nine sympatric species of macropodid marsupials. J Mammal. 55:66–80.
( RevellLJ 2012 phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol Evol. 3:217–223.)
RevellLJ 2012 phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol Evol. 3:217–223.RevellLJ 2012 phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol Evol. 3:217–223., RevellLJ 2012 phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things). Methods Ecol Evol. 3:217–223.
J. Bielby, G. Mace, O. Bininda-Emonds, M. Cardillo, J. Gittleman, Kate Jones, C. Orme, A. Purvis (2007)
The Fast‐Slow Continuum in Mammalian Life History: An Empirical ReevaluationThe American Naturalist, 169
( WranghamRW 1980 An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour. 75(3–4):262–300.)
WranghamRW 1980 An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour. 75(3–4):262–300.WranghamRW 1980 An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour. 75(3–4):262–300., WranghamRW 1980 An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. Behaviour. 75(3–4):262–300.
( Clutton‐BrockTH, HarveyPH 1980 Primates, brains and ecology. J Zool. 190:309–323.)
Clutton‐BrockTH, HarveyPH 1980 Primates, brains and ecology. J Zool. 190:309–323.Clutton‐BrockTH, HarveyPH 1980 Primates, brains and ecology. J Zool. 190:309–323., Clutton‐BrockTH, HarveyPH 1980 Primates, brains and ecology. J Zool. 190:309–323.
( LukasD, ReynoldsV, BoeschC, VigilantL 2005 To what extent does living in a group mean living with kin?Mol Ecol. 14:2181–2196.15910336)
LukasD, ReynoldsV, BoeschC, VigilantL 2005 To what extent does living in a group mean living with kin?Mol Ecol. 14:2181–2196.15910336LukasD, ReynoldsV, BoeschC, VigilantL 2005 To what extent does living in a group mean living with kin?Mol Ecol. 14:2181–2196.15910336, LukasD, ReynoldsV, BoeschC, VigilantL 2005 To what extent does living in a group mean living with kin?Mol Ecol. 14:2181–2196.15910336
Andelman (1986)
201
( BordesF, GuéganJF, MorandS 2011 Microparasite species richness in rodents is higher at lower latitudes and is associated with reduced litter size. Oikos. 120:1889–1896.)
BordesF, GuéganJF, MorandS 2011 Microparasite species richness in rodents is higher at lower latitudes and is associated with reduced litter size. Oikos. 120:1889–1896.BordesF, GuéganJF, MorandS 2011 Microparasite species richness in rodents is higher at lower latitudes and is associated with reduced litter size. Oikos. 120:1889–1896., BordesF, GuéganJF, MorandS 2011 Microparasite species richness in rodents is higher at lower latitudes and is associated with reduced litter size. Oikos. 120:1889–1896.
J. Kaufmann (1974)
Habitat Use and Social Organization of Nine Sympatric Species of Macropodid MarsupialsJournal of Mammalogy, 55
C. Schradin (2013)
Intraspecific variation in social organization by genetic variation, developmental plasticity, social flexibility or entirely extrinsic factorsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368
L. Revell (2012)
phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative biology (and other things)Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 3
( BlombergSP, GarlandTJr, IvesAR 2003 Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution. 57:717–745.12778543)
BlombergSP, GarlandTJr, IvesAR 2003 Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution. 57:717–745.12778543BlombergSP, GarlandTJr, IvesAR 2003 Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution. 57:717–745.12778543, BlombergSP, GarlandTJr, IvesAR 2003 Testing for phylogenetic signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution. 57:717–745.12778543
( KappelerPM 1998 Nests, tree holes, and the evolution of primate life histories. Am J Primatol. 46:7–33.9730211)
KappelerPM 1998 Nests, tree holes, and the evolution of primate life histories. Am J Primatol. 46:7–33.9730211KappelerPM 1998 Nests, tree holes, and the evolution of primate life histories. Am J Primatol. 46:7–33.9730211, KappelerPM 1998 Nests, tree holes, and the evolution of primate life histories. Am J Primatol. 46:7–33.9730211
( MilesMI, JaeggiAV, Festa-BianchetM, SchradinC, HayesLD 2019 Intraspecific variation in Artiodactyla social organisation: a Bayesian phylogenetic multilevel analysis of detailed population-level data. bioRxiv 603399. doi: 10.1101/603399.)
MilesMI, JaeggiAV, Festa-BianchetM, SchradinC, HayesLD 2019 Intraspecific variation in Artiodactyla social organisation: a Bayesian phylogenetic multilevel analysis of detailed population-level data. bioRxiv 603399. doi: 10.1101/603399.MilesMI, JaeggiAV, Festa-BianchetM, SchradinC, HayesLD 2019 Intraspecific variation in Artiodactyla social organisation: a Bayesian phylogenetic multilevel analysis of detailed population-level data. bioRxiv 603399. doi: 10.1101/603399., MilesMI, JaeggiAV, Festa-BianchetM, SchradinC, HayesLD 2019 Intraspecific variation in Artiodactyla social organisation: a Bayesian phylogenetic multilevel analysis of detailed population-level data. bioRxiv 603399. doi: 10.1101/603399.
M. Valomy, Loren Hayes, C. Schradin (2015)
Social organization in Eulipotyphla: evidence for a social shrewBiology Letters, 11
( ErnestSM 2003 Life history characteristics of placental nonvolant mammals. Ecological Archives E084‐093. Ecology. 84:3402–3402.)
ErnestSM 2003 Life history characteristics of placental nonvolant mammals. Ecological Archives E084‐093. Ecology. 84:3402–3402.ErnestSM 2003 Life history characteristics of placental nonvolant mammals. Ecological Archives E084‐093. Ecology. 84:3402–3402., ErnestSM 2003 Life history characteristics of placental nonvolant mammals. Ecological Archives E084‐093. Ecology. 84:3402–3402.
R. Martin, A. MacLarnon (1985)
Gestation period, neonatal size and maternal investment in placental mammalsNature, 313
D. Lott (1991)
Intraspecific Variation in the Social Systems of Wild Vertebrates
I. Werneburg, M. Laurin, D. Koyabu, M. Sánchez-Villagra (2016)
Evolution of organogenesis and the origin of altriciality in mammalsEvolution & Development, 18
Dieter Lukas, Tim Clutton-Brock (2011)
Group structure, kinship, inbreeding risk and habitual female dispersal in plural‐breeding mammalsJournal of Evolutionary Biology, 24
( EisenbergJF 1983 The mammalian radiations: an analysis of trends in evolution, adaptation, and behaviour. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.)
EisenbergJF 1983 The mammalian radiations: an analysis of trends in evolution, adaptation, and behaviour. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.EisenbergJF 1983 The mammalian radiations: an analysis of trends in evolution, adaptation, and behaviour. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press., EisenbergJF 1983 The mammalian radiations: an analysis of trends in evolution, adaptation, and behaviour. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press.
H. Wilman, J. Belmaker, J. Simpson, C. Rosa, M. Rivadeneira, W. Jetz (2014)
EltonTraits 1.0: Species-level foraging attributes of the world's birds and mammalsEcology, 95
( SchradinC, HayesLD, PillayN, BertelsmeierC 2018 The evolution of intraspecific variation in social organization. Ethology. 124:527–536.)
SchradinC, HayesLD, PillayN, BertelsmeierC 2018 The evolution of intraspecific variation in social organization. Ethology. 124:527–536.SchradinC, HayesLD, PillayN, BertelsmeierC 2018 The evolution of intraspecific variation in social organization. Ethology. 124:527–536., SchradinC, HayesLD, PillayN, BertelsmeierC 2018 The evolution of intraspecific variation in social organization. Ethology. 124:527–536.
( SmithJE, LaceyEA, HayesLD 2017 Sociality in non-primate mammals. In: RubensteinDR, AbbotP, editors. Comparative social evolution. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press p. 284–319.)
SmithJE, LaceyEA, HayesLD 2017 Sociality in non-primate mammals. In: RubensteinDR, AbbotP, editors. Comparative social evolution. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press p. 284–319.SmithJE, LaceyEA, HayesLD 2017 Sociality in non-primate mammals. In: RubensteinDR, AbbotP, editors. Comparative social evolution. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press p. 284–319., SmithJE, LaceyEA, HayesLD 2017 Sociality in non-primate mammals. In: RubensteinDR, AbbotP, editors. Comparative social evolution. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press p. 284–319.
D. Lukas, E. Huchard (2018)
The evolution of infanticide by females in mammalsbioRxiv
(2002)
Longevity records: life spans of mammals, birds, amphibians, reptiles, and fish monographs on population aging. Monographs on population aging
( de MagalhãesJP, CostaJ 2009 A database of vertebrate longevity records and their relation to other life-history traits. J Evol Biol. 22:1770–1774.19522730)
de MagalhãesJP, CostaJ 2009 A database of vertebrate longevity records and their relation to other life-history traits. J Evol Biol. 22:1770–1774.19522730de MagalhãesJP, CostaJ 2009 A database of vertebrate longevity records and their relation to other life-history traits. J Evol Biol. 22:1770–1774.19522730, de MagalhãesJP, CostaJ 2009 A database of vertebrate longevity records and their relation to other life-history traits. J Evol Biol. 22:1770–1774.19522730
F. Bordes, J. Guégan, S. Morand (2011)
Microparasite species richness in rodents is higher at lower latitudes and is associated with reduced litter sizeOikos, 120
( ShultzS, DunbarRI 2007 The evolution of the social brain: anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proc Biol Sci. 274:2429–2436.17652066)
ShultzS, DunbarRI 2007 The evolution of the social brain: anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proc Biol Sci. 274:2429–2436.17652066ShultzS, DunbarRI 2007 The evolution of the social brain: anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proc Biol Sci. 274:2429–2436.17652066, ShultzS, DunbarRI 2007 The evolution of the social brain: anthropoid primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proc Biol Sci. 274:2429–2436.17652066
( DalerumF 2007 Phylogenetic reconstruction of carnivore social organizations. J Zool. 273:90–97.)
DalerumF 2007 Phylogenetic reconstruction of carnivore social organizations. J Zool. 273:90–97.DalerumF 2007 Phylogenetic reconstruction of carnivore social organizations. J Zool. 273:90–97., DalerumF 2007 Phylogenetic reconstruction of carnivore social organizations. J Zool. 273:90–97.
( StearnsSC 2000 Life history evolution: successes, limitations, and prospects. Naturwissenschaften. 87:476–486.11151666)
StearnsSC 2000 Life history evolution: successes, limitations, and prospects. Naturwissenschaften. 87:476–486.11151666StearnsSC 2000 Life history evolution: successes, limitations, and prospects. Naturwissenschaften. 87:476–486.11151666, StearnsSC 2000 Life history evolution: successes, limitations, and prospects. Naturwissenschaften. 87:476–486.11151666
( TökölyiJ, SchmidtJ, BartaZ 2014 Climate and mammalian life histories. Biol J Linn Soc. 111:719–736.)
TökölyiJ, SchmidtJ, BartaZ 2014 Climate and mammalian life histories. Biol J Linn Soc. 111:719–736.TökölyiJ, SchmidtJ, BartaZ 2014 Climate and mammalian life histories. Biol J Linn Soc. 111:719–736., TökölyiJ, SchmidtJ, BartaZ 2014 Climate and mammalian life histories. Biol J Linn Soc. 111:719–736.
( LukasD 2020 Data from: monotocy and the evolution of plural breeding in mammals. Behav Ecol. doi: 10.5063/F1P8497S.)
LukasD 2020 Data from: monotocy and the evolution of plural breeding in mammals. Behav Ecol. doi: 10.5063/F1P8497S.LukasD 2020 Data from: monotocy and the evolution of plural breeding in mammals. Behav Ecol. doi: 10.5063/F1P8497S., LukasD 2020 Data from: monotocy and the evolution of plural breeding in mammals. Behav Ecol. doi: 10.5063/F1P8497S.
( SterckEH, WattsDP, van SchaikCP 1997 The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 41:291–309.)
SterckEH, WattsDP, van SchaikCP 1997 The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 41:291–309.SterckEH, WattsDP, van SchaikCP 1997 The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 41:291–309., SterckEH, WattsDP, van SchaikCP 1997 The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 41:291–309.
( NievergeltCM, MutschlerT, FeistnerAT, WoodruffDS 2002 Social system of the Alaotran gentle lemur (Hapalemur griseus alaotrensis): genetic characterization of group composition and mating system. Am J Primatol. 57:157–176.12210669)
NievergeltCM, MutschlerT, FeistnerAT, WoodruffDS 2002 Social system of the Alaotran gentle lemur (Hapalemur griseus alaotrensis): genetic characterization of group composition and mating system. Am J Primatol. 57:157–176.12210669NievergeltCM, MutschlerT, FeistnerAT, WoodruffDS 2002 Social system of the Alaotran gentle lemur (Hapalemur griseus alaotrensis): genetic characterization of group composition and mating system. Am J Primatol. 57:157–176.12210669, NievergeltCM, MutschlerT, FeistnerAT, WoodruffDS 2002 Social system of the Alaotran gentle lemur (Hapalemur griseus alaotrensis): genetic characterization of group composition and mating system. Am J Primatol. 57:157–176.12210669
( EmlenST, OringLW 1977 Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science. 197:215–223.327542)
EmlenST, OringLW 1977 Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science. 197:215–223.327542EmlenST, OringLW 1977 Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science. 197:215–223.327542, EmlenST, OringLW 1977 Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science. 197:215–223.327542
S. Andelman (1986)
Ecological and social determinants of cercopithecine mating patterns
( Clutton-BrockT 2016 Mammal societies. Chichester (UK): John Wiley & Sons.)
Clutton-BrockT 2016 Mammal societies. Chichester (UK): John Wiley & Sons.Clutton-BrockT 2016 Mammal societies. Chichester (UK): John Wiley & Sons., Clutton-BrockT 2016 Mammal societies. Chichester (UK): John Wiley & Sons.
( PagelM, MeadeA, BarkerD 2004 Bayesian estimation of ancestral character states on phylogenies. Syst Biol. 53:673–684.15545248)
PagelM, MeadeA, BarkerD 2004 Bayesian estimation of ancestral character states on phylogenies. Syst Biol. 53:673–684.15545248PagelM, MeadeA, BarkerD 2004 Bayesian estimation of ancestral character states on phylogenies. Syst Biol. 53:673–684.15545248, PagelM, MeadeA, BarkerD 2004 Bayesian estimation of ancestral character states on phylogenies. Syst Biol. 53:673–684.15545248
( PromislowDE, HarveyPH 1990 Living fast and dying young: a comparative analysis of life‐history variation among mammals. J Zool. 220:417–437.)
PromislowDE, HarveyPH 1990 Living fast and dying young: a comparative analysis of life‐history variation among mammals. J Zool. 220:417–437.PromislowDE, HarveyPH 1990 Living fast and dying young: a comparative analysis of life‐history variation among mammals. J Zool. 220:417–437., PromislowDE, HarveyPH 1990 Living fast and dying young: a comparative analysis of life‐history variation among mammals. J Zool. 220:417–437.
( SchradinC, LindholmAK, JohannesenJ, SchoepfI, YuenCH, KönigB, PillayN 2012 Social flexibility and social evolution in mammals: a case study of the African striped mouse (Rhabdomys pumilio). Mol Ecol. 21:541–553.21883591)
SchradinC, LindholmAK, JohannesenJ, SchoepfI, YuenCH, KönigB, PillayN 2012 Social flexibility and social evolution in mammals: a case study of the African striped mouse (Rhabdomys pumilio). Mol Ecol. 21:541–553.21883591SchradinC, LindholmAK, JohannesenJ, SchoepfI, YuenCH, KönigB, PillayN 2012 Social flexibility and social evolution in mammals: a case study of the African striped mouse (Rhabdomys pumilio). Mol Ecol. 21:541–553.21883591, SchradinC, LindholmAK, JohannesenJ, SchoepfI, YuenCH, KönigB, PillayN 2012 Social flexibility and social evolution in mammals: a case study of the African striped mouse (Rhabdomys pumilio). Mol Ecol. 21:541–553.21883591
( KvarnemoC, AhnesjoI 1996 The dynamics of operational sex ratios and competition for mates. Trends Ecol Evol. 11:404–408.21237898)
KvarnemoC, AhnesjoI 1996 The dynamics of operational sex ratios and competition for mates. Trends Ecol Evol. 11:404–408.21237898KvarnemoC, AhnesjoI 1996 The dynamics of operational sex ratios and competition for mates. Trends Ecol Evol. 11:404–408.21237898, KvarnemoC, AhnesjoI 1996 The dynamics of operational sex ratios and competition for mates. Trends Ecol Evol. 11:404–408.21237898
( StoneGN, NeeS, FelsensteinJ 2011 Controlling for non-independence in comparative analysis of patterns across populations within species. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 366:1410–1424.21444315)
StoneGN, NeeS, FelsensteinJ 2011 Controlling for non-independence in comparative analysis of patterns across populations within species. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 366:1410–1424.21444315StoneGN, NeeS, FelsensteinJ 2011 Controlling for non-independence in comparative analysis of patterns across populations within species. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 366:1410–1424.21444315, StoneGN, NeeS, FelsensteinJ 2011 Controlling for non-independence in comparative analysis of patterns across populations within species. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 366:1410–1424.21444315
D. Lukas, T. Clutton‐Brock (2017)
Climate and the distribution of cooperative breeding in mammalsRoyal Society Open Science, 4
( FisherDO, OwensIP 2000 Female home range size and the evolution of social organization in macropod marsupials. J Anim Ecol. 69:1083–1098.)
FisherDO, OwensIP 2000 Female home range size and the evolution of social organization in macropod marsupials. J Anim Ecol. 69:1083–1098.FisherDO, OwensIP 2000 Female home range size and the evolution of social organization in macropod marsupials. J Anim Ecol. 69:1083–1098., FisherDO, OwensIP 2000 Female home range size and the evolution of social organization in macropod marsupials. J Anim Ecol. 69:1083–1098.
D. Lukas, T. Clutton‐Brock (2012)
Cooperative breeding and monogamy in mammalian societiesProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279
K. Hinde, Lauren Milligan (2011)
Primate milk: Proximate mechanisms and ultimate perspectivesEvolutionary Anthropology: Issues, 20
G. Williams (1966)
Natural Selection, the Costs of Reproduction, and a Refinement of Lack's PrincipleThe American Naturalist, 100
M. Pagel, A. Meade, D. Barker (2004)
Bayesian estimation of ancestral character states on phylogenies.Systematic biology, 53 5
Michael Johnson, S. Thomson, J. Speakman (2001)
Limits to sustained energy intake. II. Inter-relationships between resting metabolic rate, life-history traits and morphology in Mus musculus.The Journal of experimental biology, 204 Pt 11
S. Stearns (2000)
Life history evolution: successes, limitations, and prospectsNaturwissenschaften, 87
( GittlemanJL 1989 Carnivore group living: comparative trends. In: GittlemanJL, editor. Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution. Boston: Springer p. 183–207.)
GittlemanJL 1989 Carnivore group living: comparative trends. In: GittlemanJL, editor. Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution. Boston: Springer p. 183–207.GittlemanJL 1989 Carnivore group living: comparative trends. In: GittlemanJL, editor. Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution. Boston: Springer p. 183–207., GittlemanJL 1989 Carnivore group living: comparative trends. In: GittlemanJL, editor. Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution. Boston: Springer p. 183–207.
( BattistellaT, CerezerF, BubaduéJ, MeloG, GraipelM, CáceresN 2019 Litter size variation in didelphid marsupials: evidence of phylogenetic constraints and adaptation. Biol J Linn Soc. 126:40–54.)
BattistellaT, CerezerF, BubaduéJ, MeloG, GraipelM, CáceresN 2019 Litter size variation in didelphid marsupials: evidence of phylogenetic constraints and adaptation. Biol J Linn Soc. 126:40–54.BattistellaT, CerezerF, BubaduéJ, MeloG, GraipelM, CáceresN 2019 Litter size variation in didelphid marsupials: evidence of phylogenetic constraints and adaptation. Biol J Linn Soc. 126:40–54., BattistellaT, CerezerF, BubaduéJ, MeloG, GraipelM, CáceresN 2019 Litter size variation in didelphid marsupials: evidence of phylogenetic constraints and adaptation. Biol J Linn Soc. 126:40–54.
( LukasD, Clutton-BrockT 2018 Social complexity and kinship in animal societies. Ecol Lett. 21:1129–1134.29797749)
LukasD, Clutton-BrockT 2018 Social complexity and kinship in animal societies. Ecol Lett. 21:1129–1134.29797749LukasD, Clutton-BrockT 2018 Social complexity and kinship in animal societies. Ecol Lett. 21:1129–1134.29797749, LukasD, Clutton-BrockT 2018 Social complexity and kinship in animal societies. Ecol Lett. 21:1129–1134.29797749
P. Wright (1999)
Lemur traits and Madagascar ecology: coping with an island environment.American journal of physical anthropology, Suppl 29
Kate Jones, J. Bielby, M. Cardillo, Susanne Fritz, J. O'Dell, C. Orme, K. Safi, W. Sechrest, E. Boakes, C. Carbone, Christina Connolly, Michael Cutts, J. Foster, Richard Grenyer, Michael Habib, C. Plaster, S. Price, Elizabeth Rigby, J. Rist, A. Teacher, O. Bininda-Emonds, J. Gittleman, G. Mace, A. Purvis (2009)
PanTHERIA: a species‐level database of life history, ecology, and geography of extant and recently extinct mammalsEcology, 90
( R Core Team 2019 R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna (Austria): R Foundation for Statistical Computing Available from: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed 03 May 2019).)
R Core Team 2019 R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna (Austria): R Foundation for Statistical Computing Available from: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed 03 May 2019).R Core Team 2019 R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna (Austria): R Foundation for Statistical Computing Available from: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed 03 May 2019)., R Core Team 2019 R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna (Austria): R Foundation for Statistical Computing Available from: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed 03 May 2019).
Jennifer Smith, E. Lacey, L. Hayes, D. Rubenstein, P. Abbot (2017)
Sociality in Non-Primate Mammals
P. Kappeler, Michael Pereira (2003)
Primate life histories and socioecology
J. Jarvis (1981)
Eusociality in a mammal: cooperative breeding in naked mole-rat colonies.Science, 212 4494
(2003)
Life history characteristics of placental nonvolant mammals. Ecological Archives E084‐093Ecology, 84
E. Sterck, D. Watts, C. Schaik (1997)
The evolution of female social relationships in nonhuman primatesBehavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 41
( RollandJ, CondamineFL, JiguetF, MorlonH 2014 Faster speciation and reduced extinction in the tropics contribute to the Mammalian latitudinal diversity gradient. PLoS Biol. 12:e1001775.24492316)
RollandJ, CondamineFL, JiguetF, MorlonH 2014 Faster speciation and reduced extinction in the tropics contribute to the Mammalian latitudinal diversity gradient. PLoS Biol. 12:e1001775.24492316RollandJ, CondamineFL, JiguetF, MorlonH 2014 Faster speciation and reduced extinction in the tropics contribute to the Mammalian latitudinal diversity gradient. PLoS Biol. 12:e1001775.24492316, RollandJ, CondamineFL, JiguetF, MorlonH 2014 Faster speciation and reduced extinction in the tropics contribute to the Mammalian latitudinal diversity gradient. PLoS Biol. 12:e1001775.24492316
T. Clutton‐Brock, P. Harvey (2009)
Primate ecology and social organizationJournal of Zoology, 183
( LottDF 1991 Intraspecific variation in the social systems of wild vertebrates. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press.)
LottDF 1991 Intraspecific variation in the social systems of wild vertebrates. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press.LottDF 1991 Intraspecific variation in the social systems of wild vertebrates. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press., LottDF 1991 Intraspecific variation in the social systems of wild vertebrates. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press.
J. Speakman (2008)
The physiological costs of reproduction in small mammalsPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363
( RiehlC 2013 Evolutionary routes to non-kin cooperative breeding in birds. Proc Biol Sci. 280:20132245.24132311)
RiehlC 2013 Evolutionary routes to non-kin cooperative breeding in birds. Proc Biol Sci. 280:20132245.24132311RiehlC 2013 Evolutionary routes to non-kin cooperative breeding in birds. Proc Biol Sci. 280:20132245.24132311, RiehlC 2013 Evolutionary routes to non-kin cooperative breeding in birds. Proc Biol Sci. 280:20132245.24132311
S. Blomberg, T. Garland, A. Ives (2003)
TESTING FOR PHYLOGENETIC SIGNAL IN COMPARATIVE DATA: BEHAVIORAL TRAITS ARE MORE LABILE, 57
( CareyJR, JudgeDS 2002 Longevity records: life spans of mammals, birds, amphibians, reptiles, and fish monographs on population aging. Monographs on population aging, 8. Odense (DK): Odense University Press.)
CareyJR, JudgeDS 2002 Longevity records: life spans of mammals, birds, amphibians, reptiles, and fish monographs on population aging. Monographs on population aging, 8. Odense (DK): Odense University Press.CareyJR, JudgeDS 2002 Longevity records: life spans of mammals, birds, amphibians, reptiles, and fish monographs on population aging. Monographs on population aging, 8. Odense (DK): Odense University Press., CareyJR, JudgeDS 2002 Longevity records: life spans of mammals, birds, amphibians, reptiles, and fish monographs on population aging. Monographs on population aging, 8. Odense (DK): Odense University Press.
( ValomyM, HayesLD, SchradinC 2015 Social organization in Eulipotyphla: evidence for a social shrew. Biol Lett. 11:20150825.26559515)
ValomyM, HayesLD, SchradinC 2015 Social organization in Eulipotyphla: evidence for a social shrew. Biol Lett. 11:20150825.26559515ValomyM, HayesLD, SchradinC 2015 Social organization in Eulipotyphla: evidence for a social shrew. Biol Lett. 11:20150825.26559515, ValomyM, HayesLD, SchradinC 2015 Social organization in Eulipotyphla: evidence for a social shrew. Biol Lett. 11:20150825.26559515
( WerneburgI, LaurinM, KoyabuD, Sánchez-VillagraMR 2016 Evolution of organogenesis and the origin of altriciality in mammals. Evol Dev. 18:229–244.27402569)
WerneburgI, LaurinM, KoyabuD, Sánchez-VillagraMR 2016 Evolution of organogenesis and the origin of altriciality in mammals. Evol Dev. 18:229–244.27402569WerneburgI, LaurinM, KoyabuD, Sánchez-VillagraMR 2016 Evolution of organogenesis and the origin of altriciality in mammals. Evol Dev. 18:229–244.27402569, WerneburgI, LaurinM, KoyabuD, Sánchez-VillagraMR 2016 Evolution of organogenesis and the origin of altriciality in mammals. Evol Dev. 18:229–244.27402569
(1986)
Ecological Behavioral Ecology aspects of social evolution: birds and mammals
( WilmanH, BelmakerJ, SimpsonJ, de la RosaC, RivadeneiraMM, JetzW 2014 EltonTraits 1.0: species‐level foraging attributes of the world’s birds and mammals. Ecological Archives E095‐178. Ecology. 95:2027–2027.)
WilmanH, BelmakerJ, SimpsonJ, de la RosaC, RivadeneiraMM, JetzW 2014 EltonTraits 1.0: species‐level foraging attributes of the world’s birds and mammals. Ecological Archives E095‐178. Ecology. 95:2027–2027.WilmanH, BelmakerJ, SimpsonJ, de la RosaC, RivadeneiraMM, JetzW 2014 EltonTraits 1.0: species‐level foraging attributes of the world’s birds and mammals. Ecological Archives E095‐178. Ecology. 95:2027–2027., WilmanH, BelmakerJ, SimpsonJ, de la RosaC, RivadeneiraMM, JetzW 2014 EltonTraits 1.0: species‐level foraging attributes of the world’s birds and mammals. Ecological Archives E095‐178. Ecology. 95:2027–2027.
( JarmanP 1974 The social organisation of antelope in relation to their ecology. Behaviour. 48:215–267.)
JarmanP 1974 The social organisation of antelope in relation to their ecology. Behaviour. 48:215–267.JarmanP 1974 The social organisation of antelope in relation to their ecology. Behaviour. 48:215–267., JarmanP 1974 The social organisation of antelope in relation to their ecology. Behaviour. 48:215–267.
( AgnaniP, KauffmannC, HayesLD, SchradinC 2018 Intra-specific variation in social organization of Strepsirrhines. Am J Primatol. 80:e22758.29664134)
AgnaniP, KauffmannC, HayesLD, SchradinC 2018 Intra-specific variation in social organization of Strepsirrhines. Am J Primatol. 80:e22758.29664134AgnaniP, KauffmannC, HayesLD, SchradinC 2018 Intra-specific variation in social organization of Strepsirrhines. Am J Primatol. 80:e22758.29664134, AgnaniP, KauffmannC, HayesLD, SchradinC 2018 Intra-specific variation in social organization of Strepsirrhines. Am J Primatol. 80:e22758.29664134
( WrightPC 1999 Lemur traits and Madagascar ecology: coping with an island environment. Am J Phys Anthropol. (Suppl 29):31–72.10601983)
WrightPC 1999 Lemur traits and Madagascar ecology: coping with an island environment. Am J Phys Anthropol. (Suppl 29):31–72.10601983WrightPC 1999 Lemur traits and Madagascar ecology: coping with an island environment. Am J Phys Anthropol. (Suppl 29):31–72.10601983, WrightPC 1999 Lemur traits and Madagascar ecology: coping with an island environment. Am J Phys Anthropol. (Suppl 29):31–72.10601983
Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article/31/4/943/5830852 by DeepDyve user on 12 July 2022 applyparastyle "fig//caption/p[1]" parastyle "FigCapt" The official journal of the Behavioral ISBE Ecology International Society for Behavioral Ecology Behavioral Ecology (2020), 31(4), 943–949. doi:10.1093/beheco/araa039 Original Article Monotocy and the evolution of plural breeding in mammals a,b, a Dieter Lukas and Tim Clutton-Brock Department of Zoology, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, CB2 3EJ Cambridge, UK and Department of Human Behavior, Ecology and Culture, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Deutscher Platz 6, 04103 Leipzig, Germany Received 7 August 2019; revised 6 March 2020; editorial decision 3 April 2020; accepted 14 April 2020. In many mammals, breeding females are intolerant of each other and seldom associate closely but, in some, they aggregate in groups that vary in size, stability, and kinship structure. Aggregation frequently increases competition for food, and interspecific differences in female sociality among mammals are commonly attributed to contrasts in ecological parameters, including variation in activity timing, the distribution of resources, as well as the risk of predation. However, there is increasing indication that differences in female soci- ality are also associated with phylogenetic relationships and with contrasts in life-history parameters. We show here that evolutionary transitions from systems where breeding females usually occupy separate ranges (“singular breeding”) to systems where breeding females usually aggregate (“plural breeding”) have occurred more frequently in monotocous lineages where females produce single young than in polytocous ones where they produce litters. A likely explanation of this association is that competition between breeding females for resources is reduced where they produce single young and is more intense where they produce litters. Our findings rein- force evidence that variation in life-history parameters plays an important role in shaping the evolution of social behavior. Key words: life history, social competition, sociality, phylogenetic reconstruction. INTRODUCTION these systems predominate within some Orders, including ungu- lates, cetaceans, and primates (Clutton-Brock 2016; Ward and In many mammals, adult females compete for access to resources Webster 2016). In most “plural breeders,” groups usually consist necessary to raise young and occupy separate ranges or territo- of breeding females born in the same group, as well as a number ries during the breeding season—as in many rodents, insectivores, of nonbreeding natal males that will eventually disperse to breed and nocturnal carnivores (Eisenberg 1983; Wolff and Sherman elsewhere, together with one or more immigrant males that fa- 2007; Clutton-Brock 2016). The ranges of breeding females in ther most of the young born in the group. In a small number of these “singular breeders” overlap to varying extents with those of species, groups consist of immigrant breeding females together neighboring females, as well as with those of males: in some, fe- with one or more natal breeding males (Clutton-Brock 1989; males defend exclusive territories against members of both sexes, Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2011). Intraspecific variation in group whereas, in others, they share their territories with particular males size is common (Richard 1974; Lott 1991) and, in some cases, but exclude other mature females; in some, there is extensive range groups in some populations usually contain multiple breeding fe- overlap between neighboring females, whereas, in others, breeding males, whereas, in others, many contain a single breeding female females share their ranges with nonbreeding relatives and unrelated (Schradin 2013). In many cases, this variation appears to reflect breeding males but forage separately; finally, in a few, breeding fe- contrasts in population density, with larger numbers of breeding males associate with nonbreeding relatives of both sexes in cohe- females in groups where population densities are higher. sive groups as in the social mole-rats and some social mongooses Many comparative studies have explored the distribution of (Jarvis 1981; Clutton-Brock 2016). female group size in particular Orders of mammals and have In other species, breeding females are more tolerant of each shown that interspecific differences are related to variation in other and multiple breeding females share a common range and habitat use, feeding ecology, activity timing, and population den- form groups that include multiple individuals that breed regularly sity (Jarman 1974; Kaufmann 1974; Bradbury and Vehrencamp (Lewis and Pusey 1997; Clutton-Brock 2016). Although a rela- 1977; Clutton-Brock and Harvey 1977; Gittleman 1989; Wright tively small proportion of all mammals form groups of this kind, 1999; Fisher and Owens 2000; Ebensperger 2001), as well as to contrasts in body size, longevity, litter size, and juvenile develop- Address correspondence to D. Lukas. E-mail: [email protected] ment (Eisenberg 1983; van Schaik and Kappeler 1993; Rubenstein © The Author(s) 2020. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article/31/4/943/5830852 by DeepDyve user on 12 July 2022 Behavioral Ecology and Abbot 2017; Smith et al. 2017). However, analyses of variation Here, we use comparative data for mammals and phylogenetic in female sociality seldom distinguish between groups that incor- reconstructions to investigate whether there is a consistent rela- porate several breeding females (plural breeders) and groups that tionship between the evolution of monotocy and the distribution include a single breeding female (singular breeders). The distinc- of plural breeding. Given that plural breeding is rarer and ap- tion between total group size and the number of breeding females parently the derived system, we investigated whether contem- that aggregate is important for it is the extent to which breeding porary mammals in which plural breeding has been observed at females aggregate that affects social and mating competition. When all are more likely to be monotocous, whereas species in which breeding females aggregate, the number of potential breeding part- only singular breeding occurs are more likely to be polytocous. ners that individual males can guard effectively is higher (Emlen To test the prediction that transitions to the production of single and Oring 1977), potentially increasing the intensity of sexual se- offspring remove constraints on aggregation in breeding females, lection on males (Kvarnemo and Ahnesjo 1996) and the extent we subsequently investigated whether any transitions to plural to which breeding males aggregate with each other (Andelman breeding have been more common in monotocous lineages than 1986). The number of breeding females in a group also affects so- in polytocous ones. cial interactions by influencing average levels of kinship between group members (Lukas et al. 2005; Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2018). Although contrasts in group size often reflect differences in the MATERIAL AND METHODS number of breeding females that associate with each other, the re- Data and classifications lationship between total group size and the number of breeding females per group is inconsistent (Rubenstein et al. 2016). For ex- We based the classification of the social system of mammals on our ample, some of the largest stable social groups found in mammals previous databases (Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2013, 2017a) and ex- occur in singular breeders, like naked mole-rats (Heterocephalus glaber) cluded the few mammalian species in which females produce eggs. where colonies can include several hundred individuals but only a We identified species as monotocous if median litter size was less single female breed in each group (Jarvis 1981; Braude 2000). than 1.5 and as polytocous if it was greater than 1.5. Data for litter Previous theories about the evolution of sociality among size (number of young per birth, central tendency across females in breeding females (plural breeding) have focused on its association wild populations), for adult body mass (female mass in grams, cen- with diurnal activity patterns and susceptibility to predation, as well tral tendency across females in both captive and wild populations), as with reliance on resources where direct competition between in- for diurnality (whether a species is strictly nocturnal or whether dividuals foraging in close proximity is not intense (Jarman 1974; part of its activity occurs during the day), and for diet (whether a Wrangham 1980). However, there are both theoretical and empir- species is a herbivore, carnivore, or omnivore) were obtained from ical reasons for expecting life-history parameters to also play an a combination of primary and secondary sources, including pub- important role. Lactating females experience substantial increases lished databases (Carey and Judge 2002; Ernest 2003; Bielby et al. in energetic requirements in all mammals and the energetic costs 2007; McCarthy et al. 2008; de Magalhaes and Costa 2009; Jones of raising young increase with litter size (Speakman 2008; Hamel et al. 2009; Wilman et al. 2014). If entries in these databases dif- et al. 2010; Hinde and Milligan 2011). As a result, competition be- fered slightly, we used the median value and, if entries were sub- tween coresident breeding females for resources necessary to raise stantially different, we referred to the primary literature to identify offspring is likely to be more intense in polytocous species (where the most likely value. females produce multiple young at once) than in monotocous ones We restricted species in our analysis to those for which field- (females produce single young). Comparative studies support this: based reports of social behavior were available (following Schradin for example, female infanticide appears to be more frequent in 2017; Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2017b). We identified species as polytocous than monotocous species (Lukas and Huchard 2019). In singular breeders if most females occupy separate home ranges addition, polytocy is usually associated with the production of rela- or territories during the breeding season (even if they share these tively altricial infants that need to be maintained in a nest or burrow with nonbreeding females or with males) and as plural breeders if (Eisenberg 1983; van Schaik and Kappeler 1993). As a result, many multiple breeding females share a common range and actively as- polytocous mammals are central place foragers (Stephens and sociate with each other (Ward and Webster 2016), forming groups Krebs 1986), which increases the energetic costs of aggregation in that usually include more than one individual that breeds regu- breeding females and so may constrain the evolution of sociality larly. Our definition of plural breeding includes fission–fusion spe- in breeding females (Kappeler 1998). Phylogenetic comparisons cies, such as chimpanzees where females may spend part of the also suggest that monotocy may facilitate the evolution of soci- day foraging independently but live in discrete communities that ality. Although singular breeding and polytocy appear to have been include multiple breeding females that share a common range. We the ancestral condition in many phylogenetic groups of mammals classified species that live in mixed sex pairs or in groups where (Leutenegger 1979; Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2013; Werneburg only a single female breeds regularly (like the social mole-rats, et al. 2016), many of the mammalian taxa where plural breeding callitrichid primates, and several social mongooses) as singular is common are monotocous—including the primates, ungulates, breeders. We focus our classification of sociality on the behavior and cetaceans (Eisenberg 1983; Clutton-Brock 2016). Transitions to of breeding females, those that are in the later stages of preg- monotocy from polytocy appear to be rare and to have occurred at nancy or have dependent young and do not consider the beha- a relatively early stage in many mammalian lineages (Leutenegger vior of juveniles, males, or adult females who are not breeding. 1979; Bordes et al. 2011; Werneburg et al. 2016; Battistella et al. In some species where breeding females commonly aggregate, 2019), whereas transitions in sociality appear to be more recent some breeding groups include a single breeding female, whereas (Blomberg et al. 2003; Kamilar and Cooper 2013), suggesting that others include several females that breed regularly (Nievergelt transitions to monotocy in these mammalian lineages might have et al. 2002; Dalerum 2007; Schradin et al. 2012; Weidt et al. 2014; removed constraints on the evolution of plural breeding. Valomy et al. 2015; Agnani et al. 2018; Miles et al. 2019). Where Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article/31/4/943/5830852 by DeepDyve user on 12 July 2022 Lukas and Clutton-Brock • Monotocy and female sociality in mammals intrapopulation variation of this kind was reported, we classified RESULTS species as plural breeders if, throughout the breeding season, most In the 1267 species in our sample, 44% were classified as breeding females are found in groups where several females breed monotocous and 56% as polytocous. In 54% of the mamma- regularly and as singular breeders if the majority of breeding fe- lian Orders represented in our sample, all species were either males were found in groups that included a single breeding female. monotocous or polytocous, whereas 46% of Orders included We used a majority rule to reduce risks of misclassification of rare both monotocous and polytocous species. Polytocy is predominant observations that are likely to be nonadaptive (following Schradin (more than 75%) in 10 of the 24 mammalian Orders represented et al. 2018). In the small number of species where studies of dif- in our sample, whereas monotocy predominates in another 10 (see ferent populations have shown that plural breeding predominates Figure 1). in some populations or at some times, whereas singular breeding Although 56% of contemporary mammals in our sample predominates in others, often in association with relatively low are polytocous, more than 85% of all species classified as plural population density (e.g., striped mice; Schradin et al. 2012), we breeders in our sample are monotocous and, in all seven Orders of classify them as plural breeders if plural breeding predominates mammals where plural breeding is common (artiodactyls, cetaceans, in either population as this is the rarer and derived system. It bats, diprotodons, perissodactyls, proboscidea, and primates), most has been suggested that, where multiple records of female group or all species are monotocous (see Figure 1). Compared with spe- size are available, they should be included as separate points (e.g. cies that we classified as singular breeders (and controlling for Miles et al. 2019). However, like most other comparative studies, phylogenetic effects), plural breeding mammals have significantly we preferred to include a single value for each species in order smaller litter sizes (mean effect of larger litter size: −118.0, 95% to avoid particular species weighting our analyses disproportion- CI: −168.7 to −76.5; 810 singular vs. 457 plural breeders): among ately. We also generally do not have matching data on litter size singular breeders, median litter size is 3 and more than 80% of spe- for each population, so we cannot attempt to explain the full var- cies produce two or more offspring per breeding attempt. In con- iation in sociality across populations; we do not know the history trast, among species classified as plural breeders, median litter size of the populations within a given species, so we cannot infer if and is one and only 14% of species produce two or more offspring per how often transitions in sociality might have occurred (Stone et al. breeding attempt. 2011). The number of species for which multiple records showing The influence of litter size on the distribution of plural breeding differences in female sociality are available is relatively low and is independent of whether a species is nocturnal or diurnal (effect whether they are classified as plural or singular breeding does not of larger litter size on occurrence of plural breeding: −25.4, 95% appear to affect the outcome of our analyses. CI: −33.1 to −16.5; effect of being nocturnal rather than diurnal: For our phylogenetic reconstructions, we relied on a mamma- −8.9, 95% CI: −13.2 to −4.7; 374 singular vs. 115 plural breeders) lian supertree (Rolland et al. 2014) and did not resolve polytomies and whether it is omnivorous, herbivorous, or carnivorous (effect or modify the branch lengths in any analyses. All data and sources of larger litter size on occurrence of plural breeding: −19.7, 95% are deposited at the Knowledge Network for Biocomplexity (doi: CI: −27.8 to −11.4; effect of being herbivore rather than carnivore 10.5063/F1P8497S). or omnivore: 7.7, 95% CI: 0.5 to 15.7; 409 singular vs. 333 plural breeders). In a combined model, litter size also has a stronger esti- Statistical approaches mated effect on the distribution of plural breeding than body mass To test whether singular and plural breeders differ in their life- when both factors were standardized by subtracting each species’ history parameters, we ran binomial regression models using value from the mean across species divided by the standard devi- MCMCglmm (Hadfield and Nakagawa 2010) in the statistical soft- ation (effect of larger litter size on occurrence of plural breeding: ware R (R Core Team 2019). We first investigated whether differ - −11.4, 95% CI: −16.0 to −7.6, effect of larger body size: 3.3, 95% ences in monotocy/polytocy are associated with the distribution of CI: 1.4 to 5.4; 793 singular vs. 457 plural breeders). plural breeding. We included the phylogenetic relationship between Phylogenetic reconstructions provide further evidence of an as- species as covariance matrix, set a flat prior (Hadfield 2010) and sociation between the evolution of plural breeding and monotocy. used 1 500 000 iterations, a burn-in of 500 000 and a thinning in- Our phylogenetic reconstructions support previous evidence that terval of 10. Each analysis was repeated three times and visually the ancestor of mammals was polytocous and singular breeding inspected for convergence. We report the 95% confidence intervals (Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2013; Werneburg et al. 2016). They (CIs) based on the Bayesian sample for all relationships to deter- show that the prevalence of monotocy or polytocy is comparatively mine whether an estimated effect is systematically different from stable within phylogenetic groups, with few evolutionary transitions 0. The proportion of the Bayesian sample that crosses 0 is similar between monotocy and polytocy necessary to explain the distribu- to a P-value estimate; in all our cases, the CI did not contain 0 sug- tion of monotocy among the species in our sample (phylogenetic gesting that the results are robust. signal: K = 0.74; lambda 0.98; both P < 0.001). In contrast, plural For the phylogenetic reconstruction, we first estimated the breeding is more labile and, in several phyla, appears to have ori- strength of the phylogenetic signal for polytocy/monotocy and ginated relatively recently (phylogenetic signal: K = 0.45; lambda singular/plural breeding using the function phylosig in PhyTools 0.95; both P < 0.001). (Revell 2012) in R to calculate the K-statistic and lambda, as- The evolution of plural breeding has occurred between 10 and sessing their significance by comparison to 10 000 simulations. 20 times more frequently in monotocous than in polytocous lin- To assess whether monotocy and plural breeding coevolved, eages (see Supplementary Material) and models assuming that we performed reconstructions using the function Discrete in monotocy and plural breeding evolved independently received BayesTraits V3 (Pagel et al. 2004). We ran models assuming either consistently less support than those indicating that the two traits an independent or a dependent evolution, estimating the param- are associated (median of log-likelihoods of independent models: eters using maximum likelihoods based on 100 tries per model −1105; median of log-likelihood of dependent models: −829). All specification. models that we explored suggest that evolutionary transitions from Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article/31/4/943/5830852 by DeepDyve user on 12 July 2022 Behavioral Ecology Monotocous Polytocous Plural breeder Singular breeder Plural breeder Singular breeder SIRENIA PROBOSCIDEA CETACEA PERISSODACTYLA TUBULIDENTATA PILOSA PHOLIDOTA HYRACOIDEA CHIROPTERA ARTIODACTYLA PRIMATES DIPROTODONTIA MACROSCELIDEA CINGULATA CARNIVORA AFROSORICIDA RODENTIA LAGOMORPHA SORICOMORPHA SCANDENTIA PERAMELEMORPHIA ERINACEOMORPHA DIDELPHIMORPHIA DASYUROMORPHIA 0% 20% 40% 60% 80%100% Figure 1 Proportion of species in mammalian Orders that records show to be either monotocous (in blue) or polytocous (in yellow) and either plural (dark shading) or singular breeders (plain colour). In Orders in which the majority of species are monotocous, such as cetaceans or primates, plural breeding is more likely than in Orders in which most species are polytocous, such as carnivores or rodents. polytocy to monotocy occur before transitions from singular to ancestral state to be monotocous and has a much lower likelihood plural breeding and that transitions to monotocy increase the prob- than the model assuming that transitions to plural breeding are ability of subsequent transitions from singular to plural breeding. more likely to occur in monotocous species. The most likely dependent model suggests that monotocy evolved in singular breeders and was only lost in relatively few instances DISCUSSION in plural breeders. Restricting the phylogenetic reconstructions to Our analysis supports the suggestion that interspecific contrasts in assume that the evolution of plural breeding was equally likely in sociality among breeding females are associated with variation in monotocous and polytocous lineages did not change the inference litter size and with the evolution of monotocy. Although 65% of that all monotocous plural breeders originated from monotocous all mammals are polytocous and polytocy is likely to have been the singular breeders: the model suggested that there have been no original ancestral condition for all live-bearing mammals (Myhrvold transitions to monotocy in plural breeders, with the only change et al. 2015; Werneburg et al. 2016), more than 85% of species in the model inference being a 10-fold increase in the rate at which where breeding units usually include more than one female that plural breeding would be lost in polytocous species. A model which breeds regularly are monotocous. Plural breeders produce smaller restricts both gains and losses of plural breeding to occur with litters than singular breeders and transitions from singular breeding equal probability in polytocous and monotocus species infers the Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article/31/4/943/5830852 by DeepDyve user on 12 July 2022 Lukas and Clutton-Brock • Monotocy and female sociality in mammals to plural breeding appear to have been substantially more frequent birds and insects, where females usually produce multiple eggs (de in monotocous lineages than in polytocous ones. Although plural Maghalaes and Costa 2009), cooperative and eusocial breeding ap- breeding and monotocy are both associated with diurnal activity, pears more common than plural breeding (Riehl 2013; Koenig and this association does not account for the association between plural Dickinson 2016; Rubenstein and Abbot 2017). breeding and monotocy, which persists when the effects of differ - The effects of phylogenetic contrasts in life-history patterns ences in activity timing are controlled. emphasize the need for comparative studies to focus at the most A likely reason for the association between plural breeding and appropriate taxonomic level. Where related species differ in their monotocy is that energy requirements of breeding females (espe- life-history parameters, analyses of the distributions of traits cially during lactation) are substantially higher in polytocous spe- within Orders can offer important insights into relationships be- cies, generating more intense competition between females for the tween contrasts in social behavior and variation in ecology and resources necessary to provision and raise offspring (Johnson et al. life-history parameters (Rubenstein 1989; Kappeler and Pereira 2001; Speakman 2008). Although female interference in breeding 2003). However, where all members of the same Order share sim- attempts by other females is not confined to polytocous species, it ilar life-history characteristics, comparisons may need to span dif- appears to be more common in polytocous than monotocous spe- ferent radiations in order to identify the extent to which life-history cies: for example, physiological suppression of fertility in subor- parameters facilitate or constrain the evolution of social behavior dinate and female infanticide have been recorded to occur more (Harvey and Pagel 1991; Rubenstein and Abbott 2017). For ex- commonly in polytocous species than in monotocous ones (Clutton- ample, although there have been multiple analyses of the distri- Brock 2016; Lukas and Huchard 2019). bution of sociality in higher primates (Clutton-Brock and Harvey The association between monotocy and plural breeding may also 1980; Wrangham 1980; Sterck et al. 1997; Shultz et al. 2011), the help to explain contrasts in social behavior between major taxa— importance of monotocy on the evolution of sociality has (appar- and the nature of social relationships in particular. Comparative ently) not been previously recognized because a high proportion of studies show that rates of competitive interactions among group species are both monotocous and plural breeding. members tend to be reduced in mammals where average levels Finally, our analysis shows how differences in ecology, life of kinship between group members are relatively high and more history, and phylogeny are likely to interact in their effects on common when average kinship is low. In addition, costly forms of breeding systems and social organization. Although it is some- asymmetrical or altruistic cooperation, such as provisioning young times suggested that phylogenetic relationships, rather than con- born to others, are largely confined to species with high levels of trasts in ecology, control contrasts in social behavior between kinship and rare where average kinship between group members is higher level taxa (Gittleman 1986; Shultz and Dunbar 2007), low (Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2018). contrasts in life-history parameters between major taxonomic The association between plural breeding and monotocy raises groups are likely to represent ecological adaptations and to be questions about the evolution of monotocy itself. Within mammals, maintained by selection (Williams 1966; Lack 1968; Stearns the evolution of monotocy often appears to be associated with the 2000). Relationships between ecological variation and contrasts need for precociality in infants—either because they need to cling in social organization and reproductive strategies may conse- to their mothers or to the substrate (as in primates and bats) be- quently be less direct and may vary more widely between taxa cause they need to be able to locomote independently within a few than was recognized in early comparative studies. However, this hours of birth (as in many ungulates and cetaceans) or because variation does not contradict the view that contrasts in ecology young are exposed to potentially high levels of predation and there play a central role in guiding the evolution of interspecific differ - is a need to minimize the duration of the period of early develop- ences in social behavior and breeding systems. ment (as in the pinnipeds) (Martin and MacLarnon 1985; Kappeler 1998; Hamilton et al. 2011). However, it is likely that the ecolog- SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL ical circumstances favoring monotocy differ between major animal groups (Promislow and Harvey 1990; Charnov 1991; Tökölyi et al. Supplementary data are available at Behavioral Ecology online. 2014) and this is too large a topic to consider in detail here. Our analysis illustrates the way in which contrasts in life-history FUNDING parameters can influence the evolution of sociality and the form of social behavior. Monotocy and the production of precocial This work was supported by the European Research Council (grant young may allow members of some species to occupy niches or 294494THCB2011). habitats where altricial young could not be reared but may, at the Conflict of interest: The authors have no financial or other conflicts of in- same time, preclude the evolution of some breeding systems. For terest to declare. example, among mammals, cooperative breeding systems, where group members other than their parents are principally respon- Data accessibility: Analyses reported in this article can be reproduced using sible for guarding and feeding infants, are restricted to polytocous the data provided by Lukas (2020). species, where average kinship between group members is rela- Handling editor: Michael Taborsky tively high (Lukas and Clutton-Brock 2012). This may be because, in monotocous species, helpers cannot generate large effects on the reproductive output of breeders or because monotocy reduces REFERENCES average kinship between group members to low levels, in partic- Agnani P, Kauffmann C, Hayes LD, Schradin C. 2018. Intra-specific ular in large groups, precluding the evolution of breeding systems variation in social organization of Strepsirrhines. Am J Primatol. involving costly forms of cooperation. The role of litter size in the 80:e22758. evolution of breeding systems may also help to explain differences Andelman SJ. 1986. Ecological and social determinants of cercopithecine in the distribution of breeding systems outside of mammals. In mating patterns. In: Rubenstein DI, Wrangham RW, editors. Ecological Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article/31/4/943/5830852 by DeepDyve user on 12 July 2022 Behavioral Ecology aspects of social evolution: birds and mammals. Princeton (NJ): Princeton Johnson MS, Thomson SC, Speakman JR. 2001. Limits to sustained energy University Press. p. 201–216. intake: II. Inter-relationships between resting metabolic rate, life-history Battistella T, Cerezer F, Bubadué J, Melo G, Graipel M, Cáceres N. 2019. traits and morphology in Mus musculus. J Exp Biol. 204:1937–1946. Litter size variation in didelphid marsupials: evidence of phylogenetic Jones KE, Bielby J, Cardillo M, Fritz SA, O’Dell J, Orme CDL, Safi K, constraints and adaptation. Biol J Linn Soc. 126:40–54. Sechrest W, Boakes EH, Carbone C, et al. 2009. PanTHERIA: a spe- Bielby J, Mace GM, Bininda-Emonds OR, Cardillo M, Gittleman JL, cies‐level database of life history, ecology, and geography of extant and Jones KE, Orme CD, Purvis A. 2007. The fast-slow continuum recently extinct mammals. Ecological Archives E090‐184. Ecology. in mammalian life history: an empirical reevaluation. Am Nat. 90:2648–2648. 169:748–757. Kamilar JM, Cooper N. 2013. Phylogenetic signal in primate behav- Blomberg SP, Garland T Jr, Ives AR. 2003. Testing for phylogenetic iour, ecology and life history. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. signal in comparative data: behavioral traits are more labile. Evolution. 368:20120341. 57:717–745. Kappeler PM. 1998. Nests, tree holes, and the evolution of primate life his- Bordes F, Guégan JF, Morand S. 2011. Microparasite species richness in ro- tories. Am J Primatol. 46:7–33. dents is higher at lower latitudes and is associated with reduced litter size. Kappeler PM, Pereira ME, editors. 2003. Primate life histories and socioe- Oikos. 120:1889–1896. cology. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press. Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL. 1977. Social organization and foraging in Kaufmann JH. 1974. Habitat use and social organization of nine sympatric emballonurid bats. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 2:1–17. species of macropodid marsupials. J Mammal. 55:66–80. Braude S. 2000. Dispersal and new colony formation in wild naked mole- Koenig WD, Dickinson JL, editors. 2016. Cooperative breeding in verte- rats: evidence against inbreeding as the system of mating. Behav Ecol. brates: studies of ecology, evolution, and behavior. Cambridge (UK): 11:7–12. Cambridge University Press. Carey JR, Judge DS. 2002. Longevity records: life spans of mammals, Kvarnemo C, Ahnesjo I. 1996. The dynamics of operational sex ratios and birds, amphibians, reptiles, and fish monographs on population aging. competition for mates. Trends Ecol Evol. 11:404–408. Monographs on population aging, 8. Odense (DK): Odense University Lack D. 1968. Factors influencing clutch size, egg size and the incubation Press. period in waterfowl and other freshwater aquatic birds. In: Lack D, editor. Charnov EL. 1991. Evolution of life history variation among female mam- Ecological adaptations for breeding in birds. London (UK): Methuen. p. mals. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 88:1134–1137. 222–237. Clutton-Brock TH. 1989. Female transfer and inbreeding avoidance in so- Leutenegger W. 1979. Evolution of litter size in primates. Am Naturalist. cial mammals. Nature. 337:70–72. 114:525–531. Clutton-Brock T. 2016. Mammal societies. Chichester (UK): John Wiley & Lewis SE, Pusey AE. 1997. Factors influencing the occurrence of communal Sons. care in plural breeding mammals. In: Solomon NG, French JA, editors. Clutton‐Brock TH, Harvey PH. 1977. Primate ecology and social organiza- Cooperative breeding in mammals. New York: Cambridge University tion. J Zool. 183:1–39. Press. p. 335–363. Clutton‐Brock TH, Harvey PH. 1980. Primates, brains and ecology. J Zool. Lott DF. 1991. Intraspecific variation in the social systems of wild verte- 190:309–323. brates. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press. Dalerum F. 2007. Phylogenetic reconstruction of carnivore social organiza- Lukas D. 2020. Data from: monotocy and the evolution of plural breeding tions. J Zool. 273:90–97. in mammals. Behav Ecol. doi: 10.5063/F1P8497S. Ebensperger LA. 2001. A review of the evolutionary causes of rodent Lukas D, Clutton-Brock TH. 2011. Group structure, kinship, inbreeding group-living. Acta Theriol. 46:115–144. risk and habitual female dispersal in plural-breeding mammals. J Evol Eisenberg, JF. 1983. The mammalian radiations: an analysis of trends in ev- Biol. 24:2624–2630. olution, adaptation, and behaviour. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Lukas D, Clutton-Brock T. 2012. Cooperative breeding and monogamy in Press. mammalian societies. Proc Biol Sci. 279:2151–2156. Emlen ST, Oring LW. 1977. Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of Lukas D, Clutton-Brock TH. 2013. The evolution of social monogamy in mating systems. Science. 197:215–223. mammals. Science. 341:526–530. Ernest SM. 2003. Life history characteristics of placental nonvolant mam- Lukas D, Clutton-Brock T. 2017a. Climate and the distribution of coopera- mals. Ecological Archives E084‐093. Ecology. 84:3402–3402. tive breeding in mammals. R Soc Open Sci. 4:160897. Fisher DO, Owens IP. 2000. Female home range size and the evolu- Lukas D, Clutton-Brock T. 2017b. Comparative studies need to rely both tion of social organization in macropod marsupials. J Anim Ecol. on sound natural history data and on excellent statistical analysis. R Soc 69:1083–1098. Open Sci. 4:171211. Gittleman JL. 1986. Carnivore brain size, behavioral ecology, and phy- Lukas D, Clutton-Brock T. 2018. Social complexity and kinship in animal logeny. J Mammal. 67:23–36. societies. Ecol Lett. 21:1129–1134. Gittleman JL. 1989. Carnivore group living: comparative trends. In: Lukas D, Huchard E. 2019. The evolution of infanticide by females in Gittleman JL, editor. Carnivore behavior, ecology, and evolution. Boston: mammals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 374:20180075. Springer. p. 183–207. Lukas D, Reynolds V, Boesch C, Vigilant L. 2005. To what extent does Hadfield JD. 2010. MCMCglmm: Markov chain Monte Carlo methods for living in a group mean living with kin? Mol Ecol. 14:2181–2196. generalized linear mixed models. Available from: cran.uvigo.es/web/ de Magalhães JP, Costa J. 2009. A database of vertebrate longevity records packages/MCMCglmm/vignettes/Tutorial.pdf (accessed 26 March and their relation to other life-history traits. J Evol Biol. 22:1770–1774. 2015). Martin RD, MacLarnon AM. 1985. Gestation period, neonatal size and Hadfield JD, Nakagawa S. 2010. General quantitative genetic methods for maternal investment in placental mammals. Nature. 313:220–223. comparative biology: phylogenies, taxonomies and multi-trait models for McCarthy MA, Citroen R, McCall SC. 2008. Allometric scaling and continuous and categorical characters. J Evol Biol. 23:494–508. Bayesian priors for annual survival of birds and mammals. Am Nat. Hamel S, Gaillard JM, Yoccoz NG, Loison A, Bonenfant C, Descamps S. 172:216–222. 2010. Fitness costs of reproduction depend on life speed: empirical evi- Miles MI, Jaeggi AV, Festa-Bianchet M, Schradin C, Hayes LD. 2019. dence from mammalian populations. Ecol Lett. 13:915–935. Intraspecific variation in Artiodactyla social organisation: a Bayesian phy- Hamilton MJ, Davidson AD, Sibly RM, Brown JH. 2011. Universal logenetic multilevel analysis of detailed population-level data. bioRxiv scaling of production rates across mammalian lineages. Proc Biol Sci. 603399. doi: 10.1101/603399. 278:560–566. Myhrvold NP, Baldridge E, Chan B, Sivam D, Freeman DL, Ernest SKM. Harvey PH, Pagel MD. 1991. The comparative method in evolutionary bi- 2015. An amniote life-history database to perform comparative analyses ology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. with birds, mammals, and reptiles. Ecology. 96:3109. Hinde K, Milligan LA. 2011. Primate milk: proximate mechanisms and ul- Nievergelt CM, Mutschler T, Feistner AT, Woodruff DS. 2002. Social timate perspectives. Evol Anthropol. 20:9–23. system of the Alaotran gentle lemur (Hapalemur griseus alaotrensis): ge- Jarman P. 1974. The social organisation of antelope in relation to their netic characterization of group composition and mating system. Am J ecology. Behaviour. 48:215–267. Primatol. 57:157–176. Jarvis JU. 1981. Eusociality in a mammal: cooperative breeding in naked Pagel M, Meade A, Barker D. 2004. Bayesian estimation of ancestral char- mole-rat colonies. Science. 212:571–573. acter states on phylogenies. Syst Biol. 53:673–684. Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/article/31/4/943/5830852 by DeepDyve user on 12 July 2022 Lukas and Clutton-Brock • Monotocy and female sociality in mammals Promislow DE, Harvey PH. 1990. Living fast and dying young: a com- Shultz S, Opie C, Atkinson QD. 2011. Stepwise evolution of stable sociality parative analysis of life‐history variation among mammals. J Zool. in primates. Nature. 479:219–222. 220:417–437. Smith JE, Lacey EA, Hayes LD. 2017. Sociality in non-primate mammals. R Core Team. 2019. R: a language and environment for statistical com- In: Rubenstein DR, Abbot P, editors. Comparative social evolution. puting. Vienna (Austria): R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press. p. 284–319. Available from: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed 03 May 2019). Speakman JR. 2008. The physiological costs of reproduction in small mam- Revell LJ. 2012. phytools: an R package for phylogenetic comparative bi- mals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 363:375–398. ology (and other things). Methods Ecol Evol. 3:217–223. Stearns SC. 2000. Life history evolution: successes, limitations, and pro- Richard A. 1974. Intra-specific variation in the social organization and spects. Naturwissenschaften. 87:476–486. ecology of Propithecus verreauxi. Folia Primatol (Basel). 22:178–207. Stephens DW, Krebs JR. 1986. Foraging theory. Princeton (NJ): Princeton Riehl C. 2013. Evolutionary routes to non-kin cooperative breeding in University Press. birds. Proc Biol Sci. 280:20132245. Sterck EH, Watts DP, van Schaik CP. 1997. The evolution of female Rolland J, Condamine FL, Jiguet F, Morlon H. 2014. Faster speciation and social relationships in nonhuman primates. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. reduced extinction in the tropics contribute to the Mammalian latitudinal 41:291–309. diversity gradient. PLoS Biol. 12:e1001775. Stone GN, Nee S, Felsenstein J. 2011. Controlling for non-independence in Rubenstein DI. 1989. Life history and social organization in arid adapted comparative analysis of patterns across populations within species. Philos ungulates. J Arid Environ. 17:145–156. Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 366:1410–1424. Rubenstein DR, Abbot P. 2017. Conclusion: social synthesis: opportunities Tökölyi J, Schmidt J, Barta Z. 2014. Climate and mammalian life histories. for comparative social evolution. In: Rubenstein DR, Abbot P, editors. Biol J Linn Soc. 111:719–736. Comparative social evolution. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Valomy M, Hayes LD, Schradin C. 2015. Social organization in Press. p. 284–319. Eulipotyphla: evidence for a social shrew. Biol Lett. 11:20150825. Rubenstein DR, Botero CA, Lacey EA. 2016. Discrete but variable struc- Ward A, Webster M. 2016. Sociality: the behaviour of group-living animals. ture of animal societies leads to the false perception of a social con- Berlin (Germany): Springer. tinuum. R Soc Open Sci. 3:160147. Weidt A, Lindholm AK, König B. 2014. Communal nursing in wild van Schaik CP, Kappeler PM. 1993. Life history, activity period and lemur house mice is not a by-product of group living: females choose. social systems. In: Ganzhorn J, Kappeler PM, editors. Lemur social sys- Naturwissenschaften. 101:73–76. tems and their ecological basis. Boston: Springer. p. 241–260. Werneburg I, Laurin M, Koyabu D, Sánchez-Villagra MR. 2016. Evolution Schradin C. 2013. Intraspecific variation in social organization by genetic of organogenesis and the origin of altriciality in mammals. Evol Dev. variation, developmental plasticity, social flexibility or entirely extrinsic 18:229–244. factors. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 368:20120346. Williams GC. 1966. Natural selection, the costs of reproduction, and a re- Schradin C. 2017. Comparative studies need to rely both on sound nat- finement of Lack’s principle. Am Naturalist. 100:687–690. ural history data and on excellent statistical analysis. R Soc Open Sci. Wilman H, Belmaker J, Simpson J, de la Rosa C, Rivadeneira MM, 4:170346. Jetz W. 2014. EltonTraits 1.0: species‐level foraging attributes of the Schradin C, Hayes LD, Pillay N, Bertelsmeier C. 2018. The evolution of in- world’s birds and mammals. Ecological Archives E095‐178. Ecology. traspecific variation in social organization. Ethology. 124:527–536. 95:2027–2027. Schradin C, Lindholm AK, Johannesen J, Schoepf I, Yuen CH, König B, Wolff JO, Sherman PW. 2007. Rodent societies: an ecological and evolu- Pillay N. 2012. Social flexibility and social evolution in mammals: a tionary perspective. Chicago (IL): University of Chicago Press. case study of the African striped mouse (Rhabdomys pumilio). Mol Ecol. Wrangham RW. 1980. An ecological model of female-bonded primate 21:541–553. groups. Behaviour. 75(3–4):262–300. Shultz S, Dunbar RI. 2007. The evolution of the social brain: anthropoid Wright PC. 1999. Lemur traits and Madagascar ecology: coping with an primates contrast with other vertebrates. Proc Biol Sci. 274:2429–2436. island environment. Am J Phys Anthropol. (Suppl 29):31–72.
Behavioral Ecology – Oxford University Press
Published: Jul 29, 2020
You can share this free article with as many people as you like with the url below! We hope you enjoy this feature!
Read and print from thousands of top scholarly journals.
Already have an account? Log in
Bookmark this article. You can see your Bookmarks on your DeepDyve Library.
To save an article, log in first, or sign up for a DeepDyve account if you don’t already have one.
Copy and paste the desired citation format or use the link below to download a file formatted for EndNote
Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
All DeepDyve websites use cookies to improve your online experience. They were placed on your computer when you launched this website. You can change your cookie settings through your browser.