Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 7-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support

Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support Key Points Question Do seriously ill patients’ IMPORTANCE Patients with serious illnesses are often encouraged to actively deliberate about the decisions about life support desirability of life support. Yet it is unknown whether deliberation changes the substance or quality interventions differ when made of such decisions. intuitively vs deliberatively? Findings In a randomized clinical trial of OBJECTIVE To identify differences in decisions about life support interventions and goals of care 199 hospitalized patients aged 60 years made intuitively vs deliberatively by patients with serious illnesses. and older with serious oncologic, cardiac, and pulmonary illnesses, DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS Randomized clinical trial in which patients were asked to encouraging patients to deliberate on express treatment preferences in a series of clinical scenarios. Participants were 199 hospitalized end-of-life decisions did not change the patients aged 60 years and older with serious oncologic, cardiac, and pulmonary illnesses treated in content or improve the quality of those a large, urban academic hospital from July 1, 2015, through March 15, 2016. decisions. INTERVENTIONS Patients in the intuitive group were subjected to a cognitive load and instructed Meaning It is important to evaluate to answer each question immediately based on gut instinct. Patients in the deliberative group were whether commonly advocated decision not cognitively loaded, were instructed to think carefully about their answers, and were required to aids and structured communication explain their answers. interventions improve seriously ill patients’ choices. MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Choices regarding life support (4 scenarios) and goals of care (1 scenario), concordance of these choices with patients’ valuations of health states that could follow Invited Commentary from them, and decisional uncertainty. Supplemental content RESULTS Of 199 patients, 132 (66%) were male and the mean (SD) age was 67.2 (5.0) years. Similar Author affiliations and article information are proportions of patients in the intuitive group (n = 97) and the deliberative group (n = 102) said they listed at the end of this article. would accept a feeding tube for chronic aspiration (42% vs 44%, respectively; difference, −2%; 95% CI, −16% to 12%; P = .79), antibiotics for life-threatening infection in the event of terminal illness (39% vs 43%, respectively; difference, −4%; 95% CI, −18% to 10%; P = .57), a trial of mechanical ventilation (59% vs 60%, respectively; difference,−1%; 95% CI, −15% to 13%; P = .88), and a tracheostomy tube (37% vs 41%, respectively; difference, −4%; 95% CI, −22% to 13%; P = .64). Patients in the deliberative group were slightly more likely than patients in the intuitive group to choose a palliative approach to treatment in the event of serious illness (45% vs 30%, respectively; difference, 15%; 95% CI, 1%-29%; P = .04). Across scenarios, decisional uncertainty was similar between the 2 groups (all P > .05), and intuitive decisions were either equally or more closely aligned with patients’ health state valuations than deliberative decisions. CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE In this study, encouraging hospitalized patients with serious illnesses to deliberate on end-of-life decisions did not change the content or improve the quality of (continued) Open Access. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 1/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support Abstract (continued) these decisions. It is important to evaluate whether decision aids and structured communication interventions improve seriously ill patients’ choices. TRIAL REGISTRATION ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02487810 JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 Introduction Patients with serious illnesses are often asked to articulate their preferences regarding treatment at the end of life. Such decisions include preferences for receiving specific medical interventions such as mechanical ventilation, and more general preferences for care designed to maximize longevity or 1,2 comfort if these goals were to conflict. The widespread promotion of shared decision making and burgeoning development of formal decision aids represent efforts to foster careful deliberation 3-6 about such decisions. The implicit assumption is that active deliberation will help patients choose care that is most consistent with their underlying values. This norm of promoting deliberation is also supported by basic research showing that human cognition involves 2 different but interrelated modes of processing: one controlled by an intuitive system, which is fast and associative, and the other controlled by a deliberative system, which is 7-9 slower, rule based, and analytic. A dominant view holds that the intuitive mode relies on multiple heuristics that can lead to systemic errors in judgment, whereas the deliberative system is commonly thought to help prevent such errors. However, a contrasting view holds that decisions made automatically can more faithfully integrate existing values and lead to normatively optimal judgments, particularly when making 11-13 complex decisions. Other work has suggested that complex decisions are neither improved nor 14,15 worsened by responding immediately and that deliberation may serve primarily to justify people’s original beliefs (a form of confirmation bias). A final view is that the mode of processing that will optimize any given decision depends on how closely the mode of thought matches the demands and features of the task. 18,19 Although the mode of processing has been shown to influence peoples’ moral reasoning, 20 21 22,23 depth of religiosity, exercise of self-control, and inclinations toward social cooperation, it is uncertain which mode ought to be promoted when engaging patients in making choices about end-of-life treatments. To inform the common practice of asking patients to deliberate about desired end-of-life treatment intensity, we conducted a randomized clinical trial in which we required hospitalized patients with serious illnesses to respond intuitively or deliberatively to a series of decisions regarding life support and general goals of care. We sought to identify differences in the aggressiveness of care chosen intuitively vs deliberatively and to determine which decision-making approach yielded choices that were more concordant with patients’ underlying valuations of health states that could foreseeably follow from those choices. Methods Patient Recruitment We recruited patients who were hospitalized at the Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania from July 1, 2015, through March 15, 2016 (Figure 1). The trial protocol is included in Supplement 1. Eligible patients were aged 60 years or older and had advanced stage solid malignant neoplasm, advanced stage lymphoma, acute myeloid leukemia, New York Heart Association class III or IV congestive heart failure, or severe obstructive or restrictive lung disease. Patients with earlier-stage malignant neoplasms or class II heart failure were also eligible if they had been hospitalized previously that year (eFigure 1 and eFigure 2 in Supplement 2). JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 2/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support One of us (E.B.R.) reviewed the electronic medical record to identify eligible patients, and 2 others (A.E.B. and A.A.M.) screened the patient’s primary nurse to verify that patients had no exclusion criteria. The latter investigators then approached patients for written informed consent and conducted interviews using a standardized survey instrument on an electronic tablet that we had previously pilot tested with 20 patients to ensure facile comprehension. The University of Pennsylvania institutional review board approved this study. This study followed the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) reporting guideline. Intervention Just prior to starting the interview, consenting patients were randomized using the Qualtrics Randomizer tool to either an intuitive or deliberative decision-making group. Prior to beginning the interview, patients in the intuitive group were instructed to answer each of 5 questions about treatment preferences in different clinical scenarios immediately based on instinct (Table 1). Prior to each scenario, these patients were given a new 5-digit number and were instructed to remember it while hearing the scenario and to repeat it after answering each question. Imposing a cognitive load Figure 1. CONSORT Flow Diagram of Recruitment, Enrollment, and Data Analysis 1255 Hospitalized patients assessed for eligibility 683 Excluded 301 Did not meet diagnostic criteria 112 Code status limitation 81 Altered mental status 65 Uncontrolled symptoms 58 New diagnosis 25 Non-English speaking 23 Transplant evaluation 16 Tracheostomy or feeding tube 2 Incarcerated 572 Determined to be eligible by medical record review and nursing screening 372 Excluded 162 Declined to participate 210 Missed 200 Randomized 98 Randomized to intuitive group 102 Randomized to deliberative group 98 Received intervention 102 Received intervention Missed patients were those who met eligibility 97 Included in primary analysis 102 Included in primary analysis criteria but were discharged from the hospital before 1 Revoked consent after randomization they could be approached for consent. Table 1. Instructions Provided to Patients in the Intuitive and Deliberative Groups Intuitive Group Deliberative Group It is very important that you answer each question immediately based on your gut It is very important that you take your time in thinking about these scenarios and consider instinct, giving the first answer that comes into your head. each situation very carefully before you answer. You can take as much time as you need, but I want you to wait at least 1 full minute after I read each scenario before giving me your answer. Try not to let your gut feelings and emotions get in the way of making the best possible decision. Before I describe each scenario, I am going to give you a string of 5 numbers. I am While I ask you these questions, I am going to ask you to remember a single-digit number. going to ask you to keep that string of numbers in your head while you are When I am done with each question, I will ask if you remember the number. considering the scenario and making your decision. After you have made your decision for that scenario, I will ask you to try to repeat those numbers back to me. The most important thing is that you try as hard as possible to keep the string of After you have answered each question, I will ask you to say a couple of sentences about numbers in mind while you are hearing and answering the question. And remember why you made the decision you did. to answer each question immediately with the first answer that comes into your head. JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 3/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support by asking people to focus on such a secondary task is commonly used in psychology to limit 25-27 deliberation by occupying working memory. Patients in the deliberative group were instructed to carefully consider all of their options and to wait at least 1 minute after hearing each scenario before answering the accompanying question (Table 1). They were told that they would be asked to explain why they answered as they did and were prompted for such explanations following each question. To promote structural balance of the interviews across groups without cognitively loading patients in the deliberative group, these patients were asked to remember 1 single-digit number throughout the entire exercise and to state that number following their answer to each question. Data Collection Prior to hearing the clinical scenarios, and under the conditions for their assigned group described previously, all patients were presented with a single syllogistic reasoning task that had a logically valid but intuitively implausible conclusion (Premise 1: All mammals walk. Premise 2: Whales are mammals. Conclusion: Whales walk.). Patients were asked whether the conclusion followed logically from the 2 premises (the correct answer being no). Syllogistic reasoning tasks are commonly used to test whether interventions intended to promote intuitive responses were effective, as evidenced by 20,28 more incorrect responses. Patients were given basic educational information about feeding tubes, endotracheal intubation, and tracheostomy. They were then presented, under the conditions for their assigned group, with 4 scenarios involving specific interventions: a feeding tube for chronic aspiration when there was a likelihood of dying sooner without the feeding tube; antibiotics for serious infection with life expectancy of a few months or likelihood of dying within 1 to 2 weeks without antibiotics; intubation for respiratory failure with estimated 50% chance of survival or certain death without intervention; and tracheostomy for prolonged respiratory failure or certain death without intervention (eFigure 3 in Supplement 2). The antibiotics and tracheostomy scenarios specified that, if the patient survived, he or she would live in a nursing home or other medical facility and would be dependent on others for activities of daily living. After the first 20 patients had completed the study without demonstrating decisional fatigue, we added a fifth scenario in which patients were asked to indicate a preferred general approach to treatment in the event of critical illness: low intensity with focus on symptom management; moderate intensity with initial trial of aggressive care including life support, but no prolonged life support; or high intensity with all possible measures to prolong life (eFigure 3 in Supplement 2). After stating a preference regarding each intervention, patients indicated on a 5-point Likert scale how strongly they agreed with the 3 statements in the uncertainty subscale of the Decisional Conflict Scale: (1) I am clear about the best choice for me; (2) I feel sure about what to choose; and (3) this decision is easy for me to make. After responding to all the scenarios, patients were asked to rate on a scale from 0 to 10 how much mental effort it took for them to remember the numbers while 30,31 answering the questions about medical treatment. Next, without any cognitive load or specific instructions about deliberation, patients were asked to rate various single-dimension states of physical and cognitive debility on a 5-point Likert scale (worse than death, neither better nor worse than death, little better than death, somewhat better than death, much better than death). The raw distributions of these responses have been reported previously. Many of the health states reflected foreseeable outcomes of 1 or more hypothetical scenarios examined in the present study, including being confined to bed, needing constant care, living in a nursing home, or relying on a feeding tube or breathing machine. This portion of the interview was also added after the first 20 patients completed the study. Finally, patients provided demographic information and indicated whether they had a living will and whether they had 2 or more conversations in the last year regarding their end-of-life treatment preferences. JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 4/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support Outcomes The primary outcomes were patients’ decisions about life support and goals of care in the 5 scenarios. Prespecified secondary outcomes were (1) decisional uncertainty, defined by the uncertainty subscale of the Decisional Conflict Scale; and (2) value-concordant choices, defined as refusal of forms of life support in scenarios in which acceptance would result in health states rated by the patient as the same as or worse than death. We hypothesized that patients in the deliberative group would make choices that were more concordant with their evaluation of underlying health states. Statistical Analysis We used χ tests to compare choices about life support and overall care goals between groups and analyzed decisional uncertainty using 2-sample t tests. A priori, we hypothesized that patients who endorsed having living wills or having had 2 or more conversations in the last year about end-of-life preferences may be less strongly influenced by the deliberation intervention because such patients may have previously deliberated on similar choices. Thus, we prespecified 2 tests for effect modification by examining statistical interactions between the intervention and either the existence of a living will or the endorsement of prior goals-of-care conversations. Statistical significance of all tests was defined as a 2-sided P value less than .05. A Bayes factor, which is a statistical index that quantifies the evidence in favor of a null hypothesis as opposed to an alternative hypothesis, was calculated for each decision using R package BayesFactor. The Bayes factor as reported in R package BayesFactor is a ratio of the likelihood probability of the alternative hypothesis to the likelihood probability of the null hypothesis. A lower ratio therefore suggests a higher degree of evidence for the null hypothesis. We used Stata statistical software version 13.1 (StataCorp) to calculate sample size. With 100 patients in each group, the study would have 80% power to detect differences of 20% above or below baseline rates of 50% in the proportions of patients choosing each form of life support or goal of care with a type I error rate of 0.05. Power to detect the same 20% absolute effects would increase as baseline rates deviated away from 50%. It is difficult to identify a minimal clinically important difference in rates of selecting certain treatments or goals. But we reasoned that such 20% absolute differences, if found, would convincingly suggest that the decision-making strategies produced different results. Smaller differences may also be clinically important, yet may go undetected owing to low statistical power. However, providing Bayes factors to accompany each result enables readers to gauge the risk of such type II errors. Results Trial Population Among 362 eligible patients approached for this study, 200 (55%) consented and were randomized: 98 to the intuitive group and 102 to the deliberative group (Figure 1). All 200 patients completed the interview. One patient in the intuitive group withdrew consent after completing the interview and was excluded from analyses. Of 199 patients, 132 (66%) were male and the mean (SD) age was 67.2 (5.0) years. Patients’ baseline characteristics were similar in the 2 groups (Table 2). Effectiveness of Intervention The mean (SD) rating of mental effort was significantly higher in the intuitive group (5.5 [2.26] of 10) than in the deliberative group (0.23 [1.23] of 10) (difference, 5.27; 95% CI, 4.78 to 5.78; P < .001) (eFigure 4 in Supplement 2). Similarly, 0% of patients in the intuitive group rated the effort required for the number recall task as 0, whereas 93% of patients in the deliberative group did so (difference, −93%; 95% CI, −98% to −88%; P < .001). In the intuitive group, 57% of participants correctly answered the syllogism question, vs 66% in the deliberative group (difference, −9%; 95% CI, −22% to 6%; P = .14). JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 5/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support Care Choices Similar proportions of patients in the intuitive and deliberative groups said they would accept a feeding tube (42% vs 44%, respectively; difference, −2%; 95% CI, −16% to 12%; P = .79), antibiotics (39% vs 43%, respectively; difference, −4%; 95% CI, −18% to 10%; P = .57), intubation (59% vs 60%, respectively; difference,−1%; 95% CI, −15% to 13%; P = .88), and tracheostomy (37% vs 41%, respectively; difference, −4%; 95% CI, −22% to 13%; P = .64) (Figure 2A). The Bayes factors calculated using R package BayesFactor were as follows: feeding tube, 0.1802; antibiotics, 0.2029; intubation, 0.1747; and tracheostomy, 0.2453. These Bayes factors suggest that there is moderate evidence in support of the null hypothesis that the interventions do not produce different distributions of choices. In the scenario regarding general goals of care, patients in the deliberative group were slightly more likely to prefer low-intensity approaches to care (45% vs 30%, respectively; difference, 15%; 95% CI, 1%-29%; P = .04) (Figure 2B). The Bayes factor was 0.9861, suggesting that there is anecdotal evidence for the null hypothesis that the interventions do not produce a different distribution of choices. Patients who endorsed having living wills or having had 2 or more conversations in the last year about end-of-life preferences were generally less likely to choose aggressive approaches to care, but the corresponding interaction terms from regression models suggested that neither variable consistently modified the effect of the intuitive vs deliberative intervention (eTable 1 in Supplement 2). Table 2. Baseline Patient Characteristics Patients, No. (%) Total Sample Intuitive Group Deliberative Group Characteristic (N = 199) (n = 97) (n = 102) Demographic characteristics Age, mean (SD), y 67.2 (5.0) 66.5 (4.8) 67.9 (5.2) Male 132 (66) 64 (66) 68 (67) White 170 (85) 86 (89) 84 (82) Married or partnered 140 (70) 69 (71) 71 (70) Education >12 y 151 (76) 75 (77) 77 (76) Annual income ≥$100 000 67 (39) 29 (30) 38 (37) Christian 131 (66) 70 (72) 61 (60) Medical history Advanced solid malignant neoplasm 29 (15) 14 (14) 15 (15) Less advanced solid malignant 3 (2) 2 (2) 1 (1) neoplasm Acute leukemia 38 (19) 19 (20) 19 (19) Lymphoma 26 (13) 9 (9.3) 17 (17) Multiple myeloma 21 (11) 14 (14) 7 (7) Other hematologic malignant 8 (4) 5 (5) 3 (3) neoplasm Severe lung disease 23 (12) 11 (11) 12 (12) Congestive heart failure New York Heart Association class III 48 (24) 23 (24) 25 (25) or IV New York Heart Association class II 3 (2) 0 3 (3) Previous end-of-life discussions 116 (57) 55 (57) 61 (60) All characteristics showed nonsignificant differences Living will 112 (56) 54 (56) 58 (57) with P > .05. Current health rating fair or poor 116 (58) 57 (59) 59 (58) b Endorsed having had 2 or more conversations Prior hospitalization in intensive care 118 (59) 58 (60) 60 (59) regarding end-of-life treatment preferences in the unit last year. JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 6/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support Decisional Uncertainty Decisional uncertainty was low for all decisions and did not differ by group (eTable 2 in Supplement 2). On a scale from 0 (extremely certain about best choice) to 100 (extremely uncertain about best choice), mean (SD) uncertainty for the decision about tracheostomy was 29 (25) in the intuitive group and 24 (25) in the deliberative group (difference, 5; 95% CI, −3 to 15; P = .22). For all other scenarios, decisional uncertainty was lower, and the differences between the groups were smaller. Value-Concordant Care Choices For nearly every combination of specific intervention choices and resulting health states, a smaller proportion of patients in the intuitive group than in the deliberative group accepted interventions that would leave them in states they valued as equal to or worse than death (Table 3), but none of these results were significant using a Fisher exact test. For example, among patients who stated that needing care all the time would be a state equal to or worse than death, 10% in the intuitive group and 32% in the deliberative group said they would accept a tracheostomy (difference, −22%; −45% to −0.003%; P = .08). The corresponding Bayes factor was 1.475, providing anecdotal evidence that intuitive decision making more commonly produces value-concordant choices. The remaining Bayes factors suggested either anecdotal or moderate evidence for the null hypothesis, and none provided evidence that deliberative decision making increased the frequency of value-concordant choices. Discussion The task of making decisions about the intensity of medical treatment one desires at the end of life involves integrating personal values regarding the relative importance of quality and quantity of life with information about the nature and efficacy of medical interventions. Given this complex interplay of factors, the relative importance of intuition and deliberation in making optimal decisions about medical interventions at the end of life is unclear. However, decision aids and other strategies 4,34 designed to help patients deliberate on complex medical decisions are proliferating. This randomized clinical trial of intuitive vs deliberative approaches to choosing care options in states of advanced illness suggests that deliberation may be insufficient to improve choices about end-of-life care. Specifically, we found that encouraging hospitalized patients with a diverse array of serious illnesses to deliberate did not yield preferences regarding receipt of life-sustaining Figure 2. Acceptance of Specific Interventions and Preferred General Approach to Care Among Patients Making Decisions Intuitively and Deliberatively A Acceptance of specific interventions B Preferred general approach to care 100 100 Intuitive Deliberative Intuitive Deliberative 80 80 60 60 40 40 20 20 0 0 Feeding Tube Antibiotics Intubation Tracheostomy Low Intensity Moderate Intensity High Intensity Specific Medical Intervention Preferred Approach to Care A, In the intuitive group, n = 97 for all interventions except tracheostomy and n = 57 for interventions except tracheostomy and n = 61 for tracheostomy. B, In the intuitive group, tracheostomy (only patients who stated they would accept intubation were asked n = 87. In the deliberative group, n = 93. P values were calculated using χ test. whether they would accept tracheostomy). In the deliberative group, n = 102 for all JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 7/13 Patients Accepting, % Patients Endorsing, % JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support interventions across 4 clinical scenarios that differed from those revealed by patients who were forced to respond intuitively. Importantly, the patients we studied are precisely those who are likely to be encouraged to deliberate on such choices in the near term. Thus, these data extend the results of a prior study showing that deliberation also failed to change choices among healthy people asked to imagine being ill. There are several possible explanations for the similar preferences expressed by patients responding intuitively and deliberatively. First, it is possible that our interventions did not create adequate separation between how patients in the 2 groups made their decisions. It is unlikely that patients given the intuitive instructions could engage in significant deliberation because they were required to respond immediately. Furthermore, the enormous difference between groups in patients’ subjective ratings of mental effort suggests that patients in the intuitive group had more of their working memory occupied by the task, crowding out the capacity for deliberation. By contrast, patients in the deliberative group might have responded intuitively despite explicit instructions to deliberate. Similarly, a relatively brief period of deliberation in the context of an interview with a survey instrument may not yield different conclusions than intuitive decision making. A second possible explanation for the similarity in the groups’ distributions of decisions about life support is that patients with serious illnesses may already have strong preferences regarding end-of-life care. If so, patients may be able to express their true preferences spontaneously, making deliberation unnecessary. The present data cannot disprove this explanation, but it runs counter to prior evidence. For example, the observations that default options in advance directives dramatically influence seriously ill patients’ preferences for life support suggest that many patients do not hold 35,36 previously assembled preferences for such treatments. Third, deliberation may truly be ineffective as a means of influencing end-of-life decisions. We cannot be certain which of these mechanistic explanations is correct. However, all 3 possibilities yield the same conclusion that simply encouraging seriously ill patients to deliberate about specific decisions to receive mechanical ventilation, tracheostomy, feeding tubes, and antibiotics in states of advanced illness is unlikely to change their choices. Although failing to identify differences in the choices made does not necessarily mean that no differences exist, the Bayes factors reported in this article provide some assurance that the results do not represent type II errors. Table 3. Value-Discordant Decisions Among Patients Responding Intuitively or Deliberatively Decision Intuitive Group, % Deliberative Group, % Difference (95% CI), % P Value Accept tracheostomy Bed bound equal to or worse than death 15 24 −9 (−29 to 10) .53 (intuitive, n = 26; deliberative, n = 37) Need care all the time equal to or worse than death 10 32 −22 (−45 to −0.003) .08 (intuitive, n = 21; deliberative, n = 25) Live in a nursing home equal to or worse than death 22 19 3 (−21 to 27) >.99 (intuitive, n = 18; deliberative, n = 26) Rely on a breathing machine equal to or worse than death 16 22 −6 (−26 to 14) .74 (intuitive, n = 25; deliberative, n = 32) Accept antibiotics Bed bound equal to or worse than death 24 35 −11 (−28 to 5) .23 (intuitive, n = 50; deliberative, n = 68) Need care all the time equal to or worse than death 21 35 −14 (−32 to 3) .18 (intuitive, n = 43; deliberative, n = 54) Live in a nursing home equal to or worse than death 30 30 0 (−21 to 20) >.99 (intuitive, n = 33; deliberative, n = 46) Accept feeding tube Rely on a feeding tube equal to or worse than death 15 30 −15 (−30 to 2) .10 (intuitive, n = 46; deliberative, n = 54) This table illustrates the proportion of patients in each group who stated they would specifically provided that acceptance of the given intervention would result in a certain accept a specific intervention that would result in 1 or more health states considered by health state. P values were calculated using Fisher exact test. the patient to be worse than death. Comparisons were conducted only when a scenario JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 8/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support An important strength of this study is that it also sought to determine whether deliberation improved the quality of patients’ decisions. First, we explored rates at which patients’ treatment 37,38 decisions were concordant with their underlying valuations of resultant health states. Deliberation could improve rates of value-concordant choices even without altering the overall distributions of responses if it made some patients more likely to forgo interventions that would lead to intolerable health states and others more likely to accept interventions that would lead to tolerable states. However, we found no evidence of such benefits. Across all scenarios, patients in the intuitive group were as likely or more likely to appropriately forgo interventions that would foreseeably lead to health states they had personally valued as equal to or worse than death. Indeed, none of the Bayes factors suggested even weak evidence in favor of deliberation. We also gauged decision quality by measuring patients’ uncertainty regarding decisions made 25,39,40 intuitively or deliberatively. Deliberation has reduced decisional uncertainty in other contexts, although this may not reflect better decisions because the effect may be an artifact of confirmation bias. Regardless, we found similarly low levels of decisional uncertainty in both groups. Although encouraging deliberation neither changed nor improved the quality of choices for specific life-sustaining interventions, there was a signal that patients in the deliberative group may have been more likely to choose a low-intensity overall approach to treatment. It is plausible that careful reflection may have helped patients articulate their broader preferences without influencing their preferences for specific treatments. However, even for this analysis the Bayes factor provided anecdotal support for the null hypothesis that the interventions do not produce a difference in distribution of choices. Furthermore, this was the only question that had 3 answer choices, and the choices were ordered from low to high intensity for all patients. Thus, patients who were cognitively loaded may have more commonly relied on heuristics such as extremeness aversion (tendency to 42,43 44-46 pick middle options) and recency effects, both of which would bias toward more aggressive choices among patients responding intuitively. Limitations The results of this study should be interpreted in light of several limitations. First, the patients were predominantly male, white, and married or partnered. These factors, combined with the fact that patients were recruited from a single center, may limit generalizability. Second, although more than half of the seriously ill patients we approached enrolled in this trial, it is possible that patients who declined enrollment may have had systematically different preferences for end-of-life treatment. Third, we encouraged patients to deliberate for as long as they wished, but we did not test a prolonged period of deliberation or “facilitated deliberation,” as is advocated under shared decision making. Thus, our results indicate that simply encouraging deliberation is unlikely to improve choices, but it is uncertain whether actively facilitating deliberation, including providing prognostic information and describing a range of potential outcomes, might influence choices. Likewise, it is uncertain whether and how the inclusion of family members or other close contacts in deliberation would influence choices. Fourth, it is possible that participants misunderstood the scenarios presented, which could bias the results toward the null. However, we pilot tested the scenarios among 20 patients prior to launching the trial and observed good understanding. Fifth, more than half of the patients who did enroll in the study had engaged in prior deliberation about end-of-life treatments, as reflected by having a living will or having 2 or more conversations about desired end-of-life treatment in the last year (Table 2). We found no evidence that deliberation changed choices even among patients without these objective markers of prior deliberation, nor that prior deliberation modified the effects of the intervention. However, it is possible that many may have contemplated these choices previously without endorsing either objective marker of prior deliberation. Sixth, the sample size may have limited the study’s ability to detect true effects. In an effort to quantify the strength of the evidence in favor of the null hypothesis (ie, that the interventions did not alter the distributions of choices made), we calculated Bayes factors for the 4 primary decisions made JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 9/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support by the patients about specific interventions. These analyses provide moderate evidence in direct support of the null hypothesis. Conclusions In summary, this randomized clinical trial finds that encouraging seriously ill patients to deliberate before making choices regarding life support may not increase the likelihood that patients will make decisions that best promote their values. Although the scenarios were hypothetical, the findings are salient because the hospitalized patients participating in the study were at high risk of encountering the decisions and health states being studied. These findings may not generalize to other types of patients or decisions, or to contexts in which clinicians guide patients through a deliberative process. However, the results highlight the importance of testing the effects of deliberation on choices made in these other contexts, rather than assuming that deliberative approaches will necessarily improve patients’ decision-making processes. ARTICLE INFORMATION Accepted for Publication: December 7, 2018. Published: January 25, 2019. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2019 Rubin EB et al. JAMA Network Open. Corresponding Author: Emily B. Rubin, MD, JD, Massachusetts General Hospital, 55 Fruit St, Boston, MA 02114 ([email protected]). Author Affiliations: Division of Pulmonary and Critical Care Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston (Rubin); Fostering Improvement in End-of-Life Decision Science, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Rubin, Cooney, Gabler, Mante, Halpern); University of California, San Diego, School of Medicine, La Jolla (Buehler); Leonard Davis Institute of Health Economics, The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Cooney); The Palliative and Advanced Illness Research Center, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Cooney, Halpern); Division of Pulmonary, Allergy and Critical Care Medicine, Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Halpern); Department of Biostatistics, Epidemiology, and Informatics, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Halpern); Department of Medical Ethics and Health Policy, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Halpern). Author Contributions: Dr Rubin had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Concept and design: Rubin, Cooney, Halpern. Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors. Drafting of the manuscript: Rubin. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors. Statistical analysis: Rubin, Gabler. Obtained funding: Rubin, Halpern. Administrative, technical, or material support: Cooney, Mante, Halpern. Supervision: Cooney. Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported. Funding/Support: This research was funded in part by a grant from the Center for Health Incentives and Behavioral Economics at the University of Pennsylvania. Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The Center for Health Incentives and Behavioral Economics had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication. Disclaimer: The interpretation and reporting of these data are the sole responsibility of the authors. Data Sharing Statement: See Supplement 3. JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 10/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support REFERENCES 1. National Quality Forum (NQF). Safe practices for better healthcare—2010 update: a consensus report. https:// www.qualityforum.org/Publications/2010/04/Safe_Practices_for_Better_Healthcare_%E2%80%93_2010_ Update.aspx. Accessed December 28, 2018. 2. US Department of Health and Human Services National Institute on Aging. Advance care planning. https://www. nia.nih.gov/health/caregiving/advance-care-planning. Accessed December 28, 2018. 3. Snyder L; American College of Physicians Ethics, Professionalism, and Human Rights Committee. American College of Physicians Ethics Manual: sixth edition. Ann Intern Med. 2012;156(1, pt 2):73-104. doi:10.7326/0003- 4819-156-1-201201031-00001 4. Butler M, Ratner E, McCreedy E, Shippee N, Kane RL. Decision aids for advance care planning: an overview of the state of the science. Ann Intern Med. 2014;161(6):408-418. doi:10.7326/M14-0644 5. O’Connor AM, Wennberg JE, Legare F, et al. Toward the ‘tipping point’: decision aids and informed patient choice. Health Aff (Millwood). 2007;26(3):716-725. doi:10.1377/hlthaff.26.3.716 6. Oshima Lee E, Emanuel EJ. Shared decision making to improve care and reduce costs. N Engl J Med. 2013;368 (1):6-8. doi:10.1056/NEJMp1209500 7. Stanovich KE, West RF. Individual differences in reasoning: implications for the rationality debate? Behav Brain Sci. 2000;23(5):645-665. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003435 8. Kahneman D. A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. Am Psychol. 2003;58(9): 697-720. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697 9. Kahneman D, Frederick S. Frames and brains: elicitation and control of response tendencies. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(2):45-46. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.007 10. Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science. 1974;185(4157): 1124-1131. doi:10.1126/science.185.4157.1124 11. Usher M, Russo Z, Weyers M, Brauner R, Zakay D. The impact of the mode of thought in complex decisions: intuitive decisions are better. Front Psychol. 2011;2:37. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00037 12. Wilson TD, Schooler JW. Thinking too much: introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;60(2):181-192. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.181 13. Dijksterhuis A, Bos MW, Nordgren LF, van Baaren RB. On making the right choice: the deliberation-without- attention effect. Science. 2006;311(5763):1005-1007. doi:10.1126/science.1121629 14. Calvillo DP, Penaloza A. Are complex decisions better left to the unconscious? further failed replications of the deliberation-without-attention effect. Judgm Decis Mak. 2009;4(6):509-517. 15. Woolley A, Kostopoulou O, Delaney BC. Can medical diagnosis benefit from “unconscious thought”? Med Decis Making. 2016;36(4):541-549. doi:10.1177/0272989X15581352 16. Mercier H, Sperber D. Why do humans reason? arguments for an argumentative theory. Behav Brain Sci. 2011; 34(2):57-74. doi:10.1017/S0140525X10000968 17. Ayal S, Rusou Z, Zakay D, Hochman G. Determinants of judgment and decision making quality: the interplay between information processing style and situational factors. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1088. doi:10.3389/fpsyg. 2015.01088 18. Greene JD, Morelli SA, Lowenberg K, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition. 2008;107(3):1144-1154. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.004 19. Suter RS, Hertwig R. Time and moral judgment. Cognition. 2011;119(3):454-458. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011. 01.018 20. Gervais WM, Norenzayan A. Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. Science. 2012;336(6080): 493-496. doi:10.1126/science.1215647 21. Hinson JM, Jameson TL, Whitney P. Impulsive decision making and working memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2003;29(2):298-306. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.298 22. Rand DG, Greene JD, Nowak MA. Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature. 2012;489(7416): 427-430. doi:10.1038/nature11467 23. Rand DG, Epstein ZG. Risking your life without a second thought: intuitive decision-making and extreme altruism. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e109687. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0109687 24. Scherer LD, de Vries M, Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Witteman HO, Fagerlin A. Trust in deliberation: the consequences of deliberative decision strategies for medical decisions. Health Psychol. 2015;34(11):1090-1099. doi:10.1037/ hea0000203 JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 11/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support 25. Schulz JF, Fischbacher U, Thöni C, Utikal V. Affect and fairness: dictator games under cognitive load. J Econ Psychol. 2014;41:77-87. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2012.08.007 26. Filoteo JV, Lauritzen S, Maddox WT. Removing the frontal lobes: the effects of engaging executive functions on perceptual category learning. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(3):415-423. doi:10.1177/0956797610362646 27. Navon D, Miller J. Queuing or sharing? a critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion. Cogn Psychol. 2002;44(3):193-251. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0767 28. Alter AL, Oppenheimer DM, Epley N, Eyre RN. Overcoming intuition: metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2007;136(4):569-576. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.569 29. O’Connor AM. Validation of a decisional conflict scale. Med Decis Making. 1995;15(1):25-30. doi:10.1177/ 0272989X9501500105 30. Ayres P. Using subjective measures to detect variations of intrinsic cognitive load within problems. Learn Instr. 2006;16(5):389-400. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.09.001 31. Paas F, Tuovinen JE, Tabbers H, Van Gerven PWM. Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educ Psychol. 2003;38(1):63-71. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3801_8 32. Rubin EB, Buehler AE, Halpern SD. States worse than death among hospitalized patients with serious illnesses. JAMA Intern Med. 2016;176(10):1557-1559. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.4362 33. Lee MD, Wagenmakers EJ. Bayesian Cognitive Modeling: A Practical Course. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2014. 34. Stacey D, Légaré F, Lewis K, et al. Decision aids for people facing health treatment or screening decisions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2017;4:CD001431. 35. Halpern SD, Loewenstein G, Volpp KG, et al. Default options in advance directives influence how patients set goals for end-of-life care. Health Aff (Millwood). 2013;32(2):408-417. doi:10.1377/hlthaff.2012.0895 36. Kressel LM, Chapman GB, Leventhal E. The influence of default options on the expression of end-of-life treatment preferences in advance directives. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22(7):1007-1010. doi:10.1007/s11606-007- 0204-6 37. Sepucha K, Ozanne E, Silvia K, Partridge A, Mulley AG Jr. An approach to measuring the quality of breast cancer decisions. Patient Educ Couns. 2007;65(2):261-269. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2006.08.007 38. Ubel PA. Is information always a good thing? helping patients make “good” decisions. Med Care. 2002;40(9) (suppl):V39-V44. 39. Aldag RJ, Power DJ. An empirical assessment of computer-assisted decision analysis. Decis Sci. 1986;17(4): 572-588. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5915.1986.tb00243.x 40. Davis EL, McCaffery K, Mullan B, Juraskova I. An exploration of decision aid effectiveness: the impact of promoting affective vs. deliberative processing on a health-related decision. Health Expect. 2015;18(6): 2742-2752. doi:10.1111/hex.12248 41. Navajas J, Bahador B, Latham PE. Post-decisional accounts of biases in confidence. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2016; 11:55-60. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.005 42. Simonson I, Tversky A. Choice in context: tradeoff contrast and extremeness aversion. J Mark Res. 1992;29 (3):281-295. doi:10.1177/002224379202900301 43. Tversky A, Simonson I. Context-dependent preferences. Manage Sci. 1993;39(10):1179-1189. doi:10.1287/mnsc. 39.10.1179 44. Glanzer M. Storage mechanisms in recall. In: Bower GH, ed. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Vol 5. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1972:129-193. 45. Waugh NC, Norman DA. Primary memory. Psychol Rev. 1965;72(2):89-104. doi:10.1037/h0021797 46. Krosnick JA, Alwin DF. An evaluation of a cognitive theory of response-order effects in survey measurement. Public Opin Q. 1987;51(2):201-219. doi:10.1086/269029 SUPPLEMENT 1. Trial Protocol SUPPLEMENT 2. eFigure 1. Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria eFigure 2. Distribution of Primary Diagnoses Among Patients Enrolled in the Study eFigure 3. Scenarios Requiring Decision About Life-Sustaining Treatment eFigure 4. Distribution of Subjective Rating of Mental Effort in Intuitive Arm Versus Deliberative Arm JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 12/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support eTable 1. Interaction Between Selected Characteristics and Treatment Arm for the Outcome of Treatment Decisions eTable 2. Decisional Uncertainty by Arm SUPPLEMENT 3. Data Sharing Statement JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 13/13 P A A ) ) P ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P Supplementary Online Content Rubin EB, Buehler AE, Cooney E, Gabler NB, Mante AA, Halpern SD. Intuitive vs deliberative approaches to making decisions about life support: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 eFigure 1. Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria eFigure 2. Distribution of Primary Diagnoses Among Patients Enrolled in the Study eFigure 3. Scenarios Requiring Decision About Life-Sustaining Treatment eFigure 4. Distribution of Subjective Rating of Mental Effort in Intuitive Arm Versus Deliberative Arm eTable 1. Interaction Between Selected Characteristics and Treatment Arm for the Outcome of Treatment Decisions eTable 2. Decisional Uncertainty by Arm This supplementary material has been provided by the authors to give readers additional information about their work. JAMA Network Open. eFigure 1. Study inclusion and exclusion criteria Inclusion criteria: 1. Age 60 or older 2. Speaks and reads fluently in English 3. Currently an inpatient at the Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania 4. Stable vital signs 5. Listed as “full code” in the electronic medical record 6. Currently admitted to the oncology, cardiology or pulmonary service and has been admitted to the same service at least once in the last year; and/or has one or more of the following diagnoses: Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease with at least severe airflow obstruction on most recent spirometry and/or eligible for long-term oxygen therapy Incurable interstitial lung disease with at least severe restriction on most recent pulmonary function tests and/or eligible for long-term oxygen therapy Congestive heart failure with NYHA Class III or higher and current hospitalization related to heart failure Acute myeloid leukemia Stage IV lymphoma Stage IIIB or Stage IV non-small cell lung cancer, cholangiocarcinoma, renal cell carcinoma, breast cancer, uterine cancer, cervical cancer, ovarian cancer, colorectal cancer, gastric cancer, pancreatic cancer, prostate cancer, urothelial cancer Stage C or D hepatocellular carcinoma Mesothelioma or any malignancy metastatic to the pleura Exclusion criteria Cognitive impairment to the point where patient is unable to provide informed consent or to understand instructions Uncontrolled or poorly controlled pain, dyspnea or other symptoms on the day of recruitment Currently has feeding tube or tracheostomy tube Currently undergoing evaluation for solid organ transplantation First admission to the hospital following diagnosis of a serious illness JAMA Network Open. eFigure 2. Distribution of primary diagnoses among patients enrolled in the study CHF, NYHA class 2 Less advanced solid malignancy Hematological malignancy, other Multiple myeloma CHF, NYHA class 4 Severe pulmonary disease Advanced stage lymphoma CHF, NYHA class 3 Advanced solid malignancy Acute leukemia 0 5 10 15 20 Patients, % Abbreviations: NYHA, New York Heart Association “Advanced solid malignancy” was defined as Stage IIIB or Stage IV non-small cell lung cancer, cholangiocarcinoma, renal cell carcinoma, breast cancer, uterine cancer, cervical cancer, ovarian cancer, colorectal cancer, gastric cancer, pancreatic cancer, prostate cancer, urothelial cancer, stage C or D hepatocellular carcinoma, mesothelioma or any malignancy metastatic to the pleura. Lymphoma patients all had stage IV disease. JAMA Network Open. Diagnosis eFigure 3. Scenarios requiring decision about life-sustaining treatment Imagine that in the future you become very weak and develop difficulty swallowing. You are unable to eat or drink safely. When you do, small amounts of food and liquid go into your lungs and cause trouble with your breathing. Doctors think that this will be a permanent condition. The only way for you to get nutrition safely is to have a feeding tube inserted into your stomach. For the rest of your life, you will most likely have some trouble getting around, need some help with basic life activities like dressing yourself, and have mild pain or discomfort. Under these circumstances, would you choose: To have a feeding tube inserted into your stomach to give you food and fluids with the goal of living as long as possible; or Not to have a feeding tube inserted into your stomach and continue to eat and drink small amounts, understanding that you might die sooner than you would if you had a feeding tube inserted Imagine that in the future you become ill to the point that you are expected to die within the next several months. This means that you have some days when you drift in and out of awareness, have some discomfort that requires medication, are in bed most of the time due to weakness and need help getting dressed, bathing, and using the bathroom. You develop an infection. If you receive antibiotics, you might live another several months in a nursing home or similar facility in the condition I described above. If you don’t receive antibiotics, you likely will die within a week to two weeks. Under these circumstances, would you choose: To treat the infection with the goal of living as long as possible; or Not to treat the infection, receive medication to treat any uncomfortable symptoms, and die at home Imagine that in the future you develop a life-threatening illness with difficulty breathing. Doctors think there is a 50% chance you will survive this illness. In order to survive, you will likely need support from a breathing machine for at least two weeks. Doctors expect that if you survive and go through a long period of rehabilitation, you will be able to return home. If you survive, it is likely that you will have a lot of trouble getting around, will need some help with activities like getting dressed and using the bathroom, and will have mild pain or discomfort. Under these circumstances, would you choose: To have the breathing tube put in; or To be made comfortable with medications and die without a breathing tube Imagine that you were to become very sick in the near future and your loved ones and doctors were trying to decide how to best care for you. I am going to describe three general approaches to medical treatment in the event of serious illness. Please tell me which approach best describes how you would wish to be treated if you were to become very sick in the near future. If I were to become very sick, I would want the main focus of my care to be on treating any uncomfortable symptoms I might have and not on keeping me alive as long as possible. I would want to limit the amount of time I spent in the hospital and the number of tests and treatments I received. I would not wish to have any procedures that would cause me pain and I would not want to go to the intensive care unit. I would want life support measures only if they were absolutely necessary to keep me comfortable and would not want to be put or kept on life support with the goal of extending my life. I would like to be allowed to die naturally. If my heart were to stop, I would not want my medical team to take extraordinary measures to try to restart it. JAMA Network Open. If I were to become very sick, I would want the main focus of my care at first to be on keeping me alive as long as possible, even if that means that I would have to spend a lot of time in the hospital and have some procedures done that might cause me pain or discomfort. If I needed to go to the intensive care unit, I would want to do that. If I needed life support measures to keep me alive, I would want those for a little while. But I would not want to live for a long time on life support or in another condition where I would be unable to leave the hospital or another health care facility. If after a trial of life support, it looked very unlikely that I would get better and be able to go home, I would want the main focus of my care to shift to treating any uncomfortable symptoms I have and away from trying to keep me alive as long as possible. I would like to be allowed to die naturally. If my heart were to stop, I would not want my medical team to take extraordinary measures to try to restart it. If I were to become very sick, I would want the focus of my care to be on keeping me alive as long as possible, even if that means that I would have to spend a lot of time in the hospital and have some procedures done that might cause me pain or discomfort. If I needed to go to the intensive care unit, I would want to do that. If I needed life support measures to keep me alive, I would want to have them. I would want to have any medical treatment or procedure that would keep me alive. If my heart were to stop, I would want my medical team to take every possible measure to try to restart it. The figure depicts the distribution in the two arms of subjective ratings of mental effort required to remember the designated numbers while hearing the scenarios and answering questions about preferences (0 = very, very little; 10 = very, very much). 93% of patients in the deliberative arm rated the mental effort as 0, while 72% of patients in the intuitive arm rated the mental effort as 5 or greater. n=97 in the intuitive arm; n=102 in the deliberative arm. JAMA Network Open. eFigure 4. Distribution of subjective rating of mental effort in intuitive arm versus deliberative arm Intuitive Deliberative 0123456789 10 Subjective rating of mental effort 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Deliberative 93% 4% 1% 0% 0% 0% 0% 1% 0% 0% 1% Intuitive 0% 5% 8% 7% 7% 21% 14% 19% 12% 2% 4% JAMA Network Open. Patients, % a eTable 1. Interaction between selected characteristics and treatment arm for the outcome of treatment decisions Patients, (%) Intuitive Deliberative P value of Arm Arm interaction term Accept feeding tube .47 Living will yes 32 38 Living will no or don’t know 56 52 Accept antibiotics .10 Living will yes 24 38 Living will no or don’t know 58 52 Accept intubation .53 Living will yes 52 57 Living will no or don’t know 67 64 Accept tracheostomy .67 Living will yes 36 36 Living will no or don’t know 38 46 Preference for high intensity .53 treatment Low intensity Living will yes 35 53 Living will no or don’t know 24 34 Moderate intensity Living will yes 55 38 Living will no or don’t know 37 50 High intensity Living will yes 10 9 Living will no or don’t know 40 16 Accept feeding tube .03 Previous EOL discussions yes 47 36 Previous EOL discussions no 36 56 Accept antibiotics .94 Previous EOL discussions yes 38 43 Previous EOL discussions no 41 44 Accept intubation .97 Previous EOL discussions yes 56 57 Previous EOL discussions no 62 27 Accept tracheostomy .26 Previous EOL discussions yes 39 51 Previous EOL discussions no 35 27 Preference for high intensity .34 treatment Low intensity Previous EOL discussions yes 31 43 Previous EOL discussions no 28 49 Moderate intensity Previous EOL discussions yes 49 43 Previous EOL discussions n0 44 42 High intensity JAMA Network Open. Previous EOL discussions yes 20 13 Previous EOL discussions no 28 9 Abbreviation: EOL, end of life. This table reports treatment decisions by arm stratified by (a) endorsement of living will and (b) endorsement of having had at least two conversations in the last year about end of life treatment preferences. The decisions are reported as percentages. Sample sizes: Living will yes, n=54 in intuitive arm, n=58 in deliberative arm; Living will no or don’t know, n=43 in intuitive arm, n=44 in deliberative arm; previous EOL discussions yes, n=55 in intuitive arm, n=61 in deliberative arm; previous EOL discussions no, n=42 in intuitive arm, n=41 in deliberative arm JAMA Network Open. a eTable 2. Decisional uncertainty by arm Mean (SD) Scenario P value Total Sample Intuitive Arm Deliberative Arm (n=199) (n=97) (n=102) Feeding tube 21 (22) 21 (23) 21 (21) .86 Antibiotics 20 (21) 20 (22) 19 (21) .65 Intubation 23 (24) 26 (26) 21 (22) .11 Tracheostomy 26 (25) 29 (25) 24 (25) .22 Treatment approach 21 (22) 23 (22) 20 (21) .33 Decisional uncertainty was evaluated using the uncertainty subscale of the Decisional Conflict Scale. 0 = feels extremely certain about best choice, 100 = feels extremely uncertain about best choice. All values are reported as mean (standard deviation). JAMA Network Open. http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png JAMA Network Open American Medical Association

Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support

Loading next page...
 
/lp/american-medical-association/intuitive-vs-deliberative-approaches-to-making-decisions-about-life-o0OA0spKAe

References (121)

Publisher
American Medical Association
Copyright
Copyright 2019 Rubin EB et al. JAMA Network Open.
eISSN
2574-3805
DOI
10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

Key Points Question Do seriously ill patients’ IMPORTANCE Patients with serious illnesses are often encouraged to actively deliberate about the decisions about life support desirability of life support. Yet it is unknown whether deliberation changes the substance or quality interventions differ when made of such decisions. intuitively vs deliberatively? Findings In a randomized clinical trial of OBJECTIVE To identify differences in decisions about life support interventions and goals of care 199 hospitalized patients aged 60 years made intuitively vs deliberatively by patients with serious illnesses. and older with serious oncologic, cardiac, and pulmonary illnesses, DESIGN, SETTING, AND PARTICIPANTS Randomized clinical trial in which patients were asked to encouraging patients to deliberate on express treatment preferences in a series of clinical scenarios. Participants were 199 hospitalized end-of-life decisions did not change the patients aged 60 years and older with serious oncologic, cardiac, and pulmonary illnesses treated in content or improve the quality of those a large, urban academic hospital from July 1, 2015, through March 15, 2016. decisions. INTERVENTIONS Patients in the intuitive group were subjected to a cognitive load and instructed Meaning It is important to evaluate to answer each question immediately based on gut instinct. Patients in the deliberative group were whether commonly advocated decision not cognitively loaded, were instructed to think carefully about their answers, and were required to aids and structured communication explain their answers. interventions improve seriously ill patients’ choices. MAIN OUTCOMES AND MEASURES Choices regarding life support (4 scenarios) and goals of care (1 scenario), concordance of these choices with patients’ valuations of health states that could follow Invited Commentary from them, and decisional uncertainty. Supplemental content RESULTS Of 199 patients, 132 (66%) were male and the mean (SD) age was 67.2 (5.0) years. Similar Author affiliations and article information are proportions of patients in the intuitive group (n = 97) and the deliberative group (n = 102) said they listed at the end of this article. would accept a feeding tube for chronic aspiration (42% vs 44%, respectively; difference, −2%; 95% CI, −16% to 12%; P = .79), antibiotics for life-threatening infection in the event of terminal illness (39% vs 43%, respectively; difference, −4%; 95% CI, −18% to 10%; P = .57), a trial of mechanical ventilation (59% vs 60%, respectively; difference,−1%; 95% CI, −15% to 13%; P = .88), and a tracheostomy tube (37% vs 41%, respectively; difference, −4%; 95% CI, −22% to 13%; P = .64). Patients in the deliberative group were slightly more likely than patients in the intuitive group to choose a palliative approach to treatment in the event of serious illness (45% vs 30%, respectively; difference, 15%; 95% CI, 1%-29%; P = .04). Across scenarios, decisional uncertainty was similar between the 2 groups (all P > .05), and intuitive decisions were either equally or more closely aligned with patients’ health state valuations than deliberative decisions. CONCLUSIONS AND RELEVANCE In this study, encouraging hospitalized patients with serious illnesses to deliberate on end-of-life decisions did not change the content or improve the quality of (continued) Open Access. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 1/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support Abstract (continued) these decisions. It is important to evaluate whether decision aids and structured communication interventions improve seriously ill patients’ choices. TRIAL REGISTRATION ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT02487810 JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 Introduction Patients with serious illnesses are often asked to articulate their preferences regarding treatment at the end of life. Such decisions include preferences for receiving specific medical interventions such as mechanical ventilation, and more general preferences for care designed to maximize longevity or 1,2 comfort if these goals were to conflict. The widespread promotion of shared decision making and burgeoning development of formal decision aids represent efforts to foster careful deliberation 3-6 about such decisions. The implicit assumption is that active deliberation will help patients choose care that is most consistent with their underlying values. This norm of promoting deliberation is also supported by basic research showing that human cognition involves 2 different but interrelated modes of processing: one controlled by an intuitive system, which is fast and associative, and the other controlled by a deliberative system, which is 7-9 slower, rule based, and analytic. A dominant view holds that the intuitive mode relies on multiple heuristics that can lead to systemic errors in judgment, whereas the deliberative system is commonly thought to help prevent such errors. However, a contrasting view holds that decisions made automatically can more faithfully integrate existing values and lead to normatively optimal judgments, particularly when making 11-13 complex decisions. Other work has suggested that complex decisions are neither improved nor 14,15 worsened by responding immediately and that deliberation may serve primarily to justify people’s original beliefs (a form of confirmation bias). A final view is that the mode of processing that will optimize any given decision depends on how closely the mode of thought matches the demands and features of the task. 18,19 Although the mode of processing has been shown to influence peoples’ moral reasoning, 20 21 22,23 depth of religiosity, exercise of self-control, and inclinations toward social cooperation, it is uncertain which mode ought to be promoted when engaging patients in making choices about end-of-life treatments. To inform the common practice of asking patients to deliberate about desired end-of-life treatment intensity, we conducted a randomized clinical trial in which we required hospitalized patients with serious illnesses to respond intuitively or deliberatively to a series of decisions regarding life support and general goals of care. We sought to identify differences in the aggressiveness of care chosen intuitively vs deliberatively and to determine which decision-making approach yielded choices that were more concordant with patients’ underlying valuations of health states that could foreseeably follow from those choices. Methods Patient Recruitment We recruited patients who were hospitalized at the Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania from July 1, 2015, through March 15, 2016 (Figure 1). The trial protocol is included in Supplement 1. Eligible patients were aged 60 years or older and had advanced stage solid malignant neoplasm, advanced stage lymphoma, acute myeloid leukemia, New York Heart Association class III or IV congestive heart failure, or severe obstructive or restrictive lung disease. Patients with earlier-stage malignant neoplasms or class II heart failure were also eligible if they had been hospitalized previously that year (eFigure 1 and eFigure 2 in Supplement 2). JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 2/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support One of us (E.B.R.) reviewed the electronic medical record to identify eligible patients, and 2 others (A.E.B. and A.A.M.) screened the patient’s primary nurse to verify that patients had no exclusion criteria. The latter investigators then approached patients for written informed consent and conducted interviews using a standardized survey instrument on an electronic tablet that we had previously pilot tested with 20 patients to ensure facile comprehension. The University of Pennsylvania institutional review board approved this study. This study followed the Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) reporting guideline. Intervention Just prior to starting the interview, consenting patients were randomized using the Qualtrics Randomizer tool to either an intuitive or deliberative decision-making group. Prior to beginning the interview, patients in the intuitive group were instructed to answer each of 5 questions about treatment preferences in different clinical scenarios immediately based on instinct (Table 1). Prior to each scenario, these patients were given a new 5-digit number and were instructed to remember it while hearing the scenario and to repeat it after answering each question. Imposing a cognitive load Figure 1. CONSORT Flow Diagram of Recruitment, Enrollment, and Data Analysis 1255 Hospitalized patients assessed for eligibility 683 Excluded 301 Did not meet diagnostic criteria 112 Code status limitation 81 Altered mental status 65 Uncontrolled symptoms 58 New diagnosis 25 Non-English speaking 23 Transplant evaluation 16 Tracheostomy or feeding tube 2 Incarcerated 572 Determined to be eligible by medical record review and nursing screening 372 Excluded 162 Declined to participate 210 Missed 200 Randomized 98 Randomized to intuitive group 102 Randomized to deliberative group 98 Received intervention 102 Received intervention Missed patients were those who met eligibility 97 Included in primary analysis 102 Included in primary analysis criteria but were discharged from the hospital before 1 Revoked consent after randomization they could be approached for consent. Table 1. Instructions Provided to Patients in the Intuitive and Deliberative Groups Intuitive Group Deliberative Group It is very important that you answer each question immediately based on your gut It is very important that you take your time in thinking about these scenarios and consider instinct, giving the first answer that comes into your head. each situation very carefully before you answer. You can take as much time as you need, but I want you to wait at least 1 full minute after I read each scenario before giving me your answer. Try not to let your gut feelings and emotions get in the way of making the best possible decision. Before I describe each scenario, I am going to give you a string of 5 numbers. I am While I ask you these questions, I am going to ask you to remember a single-digit number. going to ask you to keep that string of numbers in your head while you are When I am done with each question, I will ask if you remember the number. considering the scenario and making your decision. After you have made your decision for that scenario, I will ask you to try to repeat those numbers back to me. The most important thing is that you try as hard as possible to keep the string of After you have answered each question, I will ask you to say a couple of sentences about numbers in mind while you are hearing and answering the question. And remember why you made the decision you did. to answer each question immediately with the first answer that comes into your head. JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 3/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support by asking people to focus on such a secondary task is commonly used in psychology to limit 25-27 deliberation by occupying working memory. Patients in the deliberative group were instructed to carefully consider all of their options and to wait at least 1 minute after hearing each scenario before answering the accompanying question (Table 1). They were told that they would be asked to explain why they answered as they did and were prompted for such explanations following each question. To promote structural balance of the interviews across groups without cognitively loading patients in the deliberative group, these patients were asked to remember 1 single-digit number throughout the entire exercise and to state that number following their answer to each question. Data Collection Prior to hearing the clinical scenarios, and under the conditions for their assigned group described previously, all patients were presented with a single syllogistic reasoning task that had a logically valid but intuitively implausible conclusion (Premise 1: All mammals walk. Premise 2: Whales are mammals. Conclusion: Whales walk.). Patients were asked whether the conclusion followed logically from the 2 premises (the correct answer being no). Syllogistic reasoning tasks are commonly used to test whether interventions intended to promote intuitive responses were effective, as evidenced by 20,28 more incorrect responses. Patients were given basic educational information about feeding tubes, endotracheal intubation, and tracheostomy. They were then presented, under the conditions for their assigned group, with 4 scenarios involving specific interventions: a feeding tube for chronic aspiration when there was a likelihood of dying sooner without the feeding tube; antibiotics for serious infection with life expectancy of a few months or likelihood of dying within 1 to 2 weeks without antibiotics; intubation for respiratory failure with estimated 50% chance of survival or certain death without intervention; and tracheostomy for prolonged respiratory failure or certain death without intervention (eFigure 3 in Supplement 2). The antibiotics and tracheostomy scenarios specified that, if the patient survived, he or she would live in a nursing home or other medical facility and would be dependent on others for activities of daily living. After the first 20 patients had completed the study without demonstrating decisional fatigue, we added a fifth scenario in which patients were asked to indicate a preferred general approach to treatment in the event of critical illness: low intensity with focus on symptom management; moderate intensity with initial trial of aggressive care including life support, but no prolonged life support; or high intensity with all possible measures to prolong life (eFigure 3 in Supplement 2). After stating a preference regarding each intervention, patients indicated on a 5-point Likert scale how strongly they agreed with the 3 statements in the uncertainty subscale of the Decisional Conflict Scale: (1) I am clear about the best choice for me; (2) I feel sure about what to choose; and (3) this decision is easy for me to make. After responding to all the scenarios, patients were asked to rate on a scale from 0 to 10 how much mental effort it took for them to remember the numbers while 30,31 answering the questions about medical treatment. Next, without any cognitive load or specific instructions about deliberation, patients were asked to rate various single-dimension states of physical and cognitive debility on a 5-point Likert scale (worse than death, neither better nor worse than death, little better than death, somewhat better than death, much better than death). The raw distributions of these responses have been reported previously. Many of the health states reflected foreseeable outcomes of 1 or more hypothetical scenarios examined in the present study, including being confined to bed, needing constant care, living in a nursing home, or relying on a feeding tube or breathing machine. This portion of the interview was also added after the first 20 patients completed the study. Finally, patients provided demographic information and indicated whether they had a living will and whether they had 2 or more conversations in the last year regarding their end-of-life treatment preferences. JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 4/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support Outcomes The primary outcomes were patients’ decisions about life support and goals of care in the 5 scenarios. Prespecified secondary outcomes were (1) decisional uncertainty, defined by the uncertainty subscale of the Decisional Conflict Scale; and (2) value-concordant choices, defined as refusal of forms of life support in scenarios in which acceptance would result in health states rated by the patient as the same as or worse than death. We hypothesized that patients in the deliberative group would make choices that were more concordant with their evaluation of underlying health states. Statistical Analysis We used χ tests to compare choices about life support and overall care goals between groups and analyzed decisional uncertainty using 2-sample t tests. A priori, we hypothesized that patients who endorsed having living wills or having had 2 or more conversations in the last year about end-of-life preferences may be less strongly influenced by the deliberation intervention because such patients may have previously deliberated on similar choices. Thus, we prespecified 2 tests for effect modification by examining statistical interactions between the intervention and either the existence of a living will or the endorsement of prior goals-of-care conversations. Statistical significance of all tests was defined as a 2-sided P value less than .05. A Bayes factor, which is a statistical index that quantifies the evidence in favor of a null hypothesis as opposed to an alternative hypothesis, was calculated for each decision using R package BayesFactor. The Bayes factor as reported in R package BayesFactor is a ratio of the likelihood probability of the alternative hypothesis to the likelihood probability of the null hypothesis. A lower ratio therefore suggests a higher degree of evidence for the null hypothesis. We used Stata statistical software version 13.1 (StataCorp) to calculate sample size. With 100 patients in each group, the study would have 80% power to detect differences of 20% above or below baseline rates of 50% in the proportions of patients choosing each form of life support or goal of care with a type I error rate of 0.05. Power to detect the same 20% absolute effects would increase as baseline rates deviated away from 50%. It is difficult to identify a minimal clinically important difference in rates of selecting certain treatments or goals. But we reasoned that such 20% absolute differences, if found, would convincingly suggest that the decision-making strategies produced different results. Smaller differences may also be clinically important, yet may go undetected owing to low statistical power. However, providing Bayes factors to accompany each result enables readers to gauge the risk of such type II errors. Results Trial Population Among 362 eligible patients approached for this study, 200 (55%) consented and were randomized: 98 to the intuitive group and 102 to the deliberative group (Figure 1). All 200 patients completed the interview. One patient in the intuitive group withdrew consent after completing the interview and was excluded from analyses. Of 199 patients, 132 (66%) were male and the mean (SD) age was 67.2 (5.0) years. Patients’ baseline characteristics were similar in the 2 groups (Table 2). Effectiveness of Intervention The mean (SD) rating of mental effort was significantly higher in the intuitive group (5.5 [2.26] of 10) than in the deliberative group (0.23 [1.23] of 10) (difference, 5.27; 95% CI, 4.78 to 5.78; P < .001) (eFigure 4 in Supplement 2). Similarly, 0% of patients in the intuitive group rated the effort required for the number recall task as 0, whereas 93% of patients in the deliberative group did so (difference, −93%; 95% CI, −98% to −88%; P < .001). In the intuitive group, 57% of participants correctly answered the syllogism question, vs 66% in the deliberative group (difference, −9%; 95% CI, −22% to 6%; P = .14). JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 5/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support Care Choices Similar proportions of patients in the intuitive and deliberative groups said they would accept a feeding tube (42% vs 44%, respectively; difference, −2%; 95% CI, −16% to 12%; P = .79), antibiotics (39% vs 43%, respectively; difference, −4%; 95% CI, −18% to 10%; P = .57), intubation (59% vs 60%, respectively; difference,−1%; 95% CI, −15% to 13%; P = .88), and tracheostomy (37% vs 41%, respectively; difference, −4%; 95% CI, −22% to 13%; P = .64) (Figure 2A). The Bayes factors calculated using R package BayesFactor were as follows: feeding tube, 0.1802; antibiotics, 0.2029; intubation, 0.1747; and tracheostomy, 0.2453. These Bayes factors suggest that there is moderate evidence in support of the null hypothesis that the interventions do not produce different distributions of choices. In the scenario regarding general goals of care, patients in the deliberative group were slightly more likely to prefer low-intensity approaches to care (45% vs 30%, respectively; difference, 15%; 95% CI, 1%-29%; P = .04) (Figure 2B). The Bayes factor was 0.9861, suggesting that there is anecdotal evidence for the null hypothesis that the interventions do not produce a different distribution of choices. Patients who endorsed having living wills or having had 2 or more conversations in the last year about end-of-life preferences were generally less likely to choose aggressive approaches to care, but the corresponding interaction terms from regression models suggested that neither variable consistently modified the effect of the intuitive vs deliberative intervention (eTable 1 in Supplement 2). Table 2. Baseline Patient Characteristics Patients, No. (%) Total Sample Intuitive Group Deliberative Group Characteristic (N = 199) (n = 97) (n = 102) Demographic characteristics Age, mean (SD), y 67.2 (5.0) 66.5 (4.8) 67.9 (5.2) Male 132 (66) 64 (66) 68 (67) White 170 (85) 86 (89) 84 (82) Married or partnered 140 (70) 69 (71) 71 (70) Education >12 y 151 (76) 75 (77) 77 (76) Annual income ≥$100 000 67 (39) 29 (30) 38 (37) Christian 131 (66) 70 (72) 61 (60) Medical history Advanced solid malignant neoplasm 29 (15) 14 (14) 15 (15) Less advanced solid malignant 3 (2) 2 (2) 1 (1) neoplasm Acute leukemia 38 (19) 19 (20) 19 (19) Lymphoma 26 (13) 9 (9.3) 17 (17) Multiple myeloma 21 (11) 14 (14) 7 (7) Other hematologic malignant 8 (4) 5 (5) 3 (3) neoplasm Severe lung disease 23 (12) 11 (11) 12 (12) Congestive heart failure New York Heart Association class III 48 (24) 23 (24) 25 (25) or IV New York Heart Association class II 3 (2) 0 3 (3) Previous end-of-life discussions 116 (57) 55 (57) 61 (60) All characteristics showed nonsignificant differences Living will 112 (56) 54 (56) 58 (57) with P > .05. Current health rating fair or poor 116 (58) 57 (59) 59 (58) b Endorsed having had 2 or more conversations Prior hospitalization in intensive care 118 (59) 58 (60) 60 (59) regarding end-of-life treatment preferences in the unit last year. JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 6/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support Decisional Uncertainty Decisional uncertainty was low for all decisions and did not differ by group (eTable 2 in Supplement 2). On a scale from 0 (extremely certain about best choice) to 100 (extremely uncertain about best choice), mean (SD) uncertainty for the decision about tracheostomy was 29 (25) in the intuitive group and 24 (25) in the deliberative group (difference, 5; 95% CI, −3 to 15; P = .22). For all other scenarios, decisional uncertainty was lower, and the differences between the groups were smaller. Value-Concordant Care Choices For nearly every combination of specific intervention choices and resulting health states, a smaller proportion of patients in the intuitive group than in the deliberative group accepted interventions that would leave them in states they valued as equal to or worse than death (Table 3), but none of these results were significant using a Fisher exact test. For example, among patients who stated that needing care all the time would be a state equal to or worse than death, 10% in the intuitive group and 32% in the deliberative group said they would accept a tracheostomy (difference, −22%; −45% to −0.003%; P = .08). The corresponding Bayes factor was 1.475, providing anecdotal evidence that intuitive decision making more commonly produces value-concordant choices. The remaining Bayes factors suggested either anecdotal or moderate evidence for the null hypothesis, and none provided evidence that deliberative decision making increased the frequency of value-concordant choices. Discussion The task of making decisions about the intensity of medical treatment one desires at the end of life involves integrating personal values regarding the relative importance of quality and quantity of life with information about the nature and efficacy of medical interventions. Given this complex interplay of factors, the relative importance of intuition and deliberation in making optimal decisions about medical interventions at the end of life is unclear. However, decision aids and other strategies 4,34 designed to help patients deliberate on complex medical decisions are proliferating. This randomized clinical trial of intuitive vs deliberative approaches to choosing care options in states of advanced illness suggests that deliberation may be insufficient to improve choices about end-of-life care. Specifically, we found that encouraging hospitalized patients with a diverse array of serious illnesses to deliberate did not yield preferences regarding receipt of life-sustaining Figure 2. Acceptance of Specific Interventions and Preferred General Approach to Care Among Patients Making Decisions Intuitively and Deliberatively A Acceptance of specific interventions B Preferred general approach to care 100 100 Intuitive Deliberative Intuitive Deliberative 80 80 60 60 40 40 20 20 0 0 Feeding Tube Antibiotics Intubation Tracheostomy Low Intensity Moderate Intensity High Intensity Specific Medical Intervention Preferred Approach to Care A, In the intuitive group, n = 97 for all interventions except tracheostomy and n = 57 for interventions except tracheostomy and n = 61 for tracheostomy. B, In the intuitive group, tracheostomy (only patients who stated they would accept intubation were asked n = 87. In the deliberative group, n = 93. P values were calculated using χ test. whether they would accept tracheostomy). In the deliberative group, n = 102 for all JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 7/13 Patients Accepting, % Patients Endorsing, % JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support interventions across 4 clinical scenarios that differed from those revealed by patients who were forced to respond intuitively. Importantly, the patients we studied are precisely those who are likely to be encouraged to deliberate on such choices in the near term. Thus, these data extend the results of a prior study showing that deliberation also failed to change choices among healthy people asked to imagine being ill. There are several possible explanations for the similar preferences expressed by patients responding intuitively and deliberatively. First, it is possible that our interventions did not create adequate separation between how patients in the 2 groups made their decisions. It is unlikely that patients given the intuitive instructions could engage in significant deliberation because they were required to respond immediately. Furthermore, the enormous difference between groups in patients’ subjective ratings of mental effort suggests that patients in the intuitive group had more of their working memory occupied by the task, crowding out the capacity for deliberation. By contrast, patients in the deliberative group might have responded intuitively despite explicit instructions to deliberate. Similarly, a relatively brief period of deliberation in the context of an interview with a survey instrument may not yield different conclusions than intuitive decision making. A second possible explanation for the similarity in the groups’ distributions of decisions about life support is that patients with serious illnesses may already have strong preferences regarding end-of-life care. If so, patients may be able to express their true preferences spontaneously, making deliberation unnecessary. The present data cannot disprove this explanation, but it runs counter to prior evidence. For example, the observations that default options in advance directives dramatically influence seriously ill patients’ preferences for life support suggest that many patients do not hold 35,36 previously assembled preferences for such treatments. Third, deliberation may truly be ineffective as a means of influencing end-of-life decisions. We cannot be certain which of these mechanistic explanations is correct. However, all 3 possibilities yield the same conclusion that simply encouraging seriously ill patients to deliberate about specific decisions to receive mechanical ventilation, tracheostomy, feeding tubes, and antibiotics in states of advanced illness is unlikely to change their choices. Although failing to identify differences in the choices made does not necessarily mean that no differences exist, the Bayes factors reported in this article provide some assurance that the results do not represent type II errors. Table 3. Value-Discordant Decisions Among Patients Responding Intuitively or Deliberatively Decision Intuitive Group, % Deliberative Group, % Difference (95% CI), % P Value Accept tracheostomy Bed bound equal to or worse than death 15 24 −9 (−29 to 10) .53 (intuitive, n = 26; deliberative, n = 37) Need care all the time equal to or worse than death 10 32 −22 (−45 to −0.003) .08 (intuitive, n = 21; deliberative, n = 25) Live in a nursing home equal to or worse than death 22 19 3 (−21 to 27) >.99 (intuitive, n = 18; deliberative, n = 26) Rely on a breathing machine equal to or worse than death 16 22 −6 (−26 to 14) .74 (intuitive, n = 25; deliberative, n = 32) Accept antibiotics Bed bound equal to or worse than death 24 35 −11 (−28 to 5) .23 (intuitive, n = 50; deliberative, n = 68) Need care all the time equal to or worse than death 21 35 −14 (−32 to 3) .18 (intuitive, n = 43; deliberative, n = 54) Live in a nursing home equal to or worse than death 30 30 0 (−21 to 20) >.99 (intuitive, n = 33; deliberative, n = 46) Accept feeding tube Rely on a feeding tube equal to or worse than death 15 30 −15 (−30 to 2) .10 (intuitive, n = 46; deliberative, n = 54) This table illustrates the proportion of patients in each group who stated they would specifically provided that acceptance of the given intervention would result in a certain accept a specific intervention that would result in 1 or more health states considered by health state. P values were calculated using Fisher exact test. the patient to be worse than death. Comparisons were conducted only when a scenario JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 8/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support An important strength of this study is that it also sought to determine whether deliberation improved the quality of patients’ decisions. First, we explored rates at which patients’ treatment 37,38 decisions were concordant with their underlying valuations of resultant health states. Deliberation could improve rates of value-concordant choices even without altering the overall distributions of responses if it made some patients more likely to forgo interventions that would lead to intolerable health states and others more likely to accept interventions that would lead to tolerable states. However, we found no evidence of such benefits. Across all scenarios, patients in the intuitive group were as likely or more likely to appropriately forgo interventions that would foreseeably lead to health states they had personally valued as equal to or worse than death. Indeed, none of the Bayes factors suggested even weak evidence in favor of deliberation. We also gauged decision quality by measuring patients’ uncertainty regarding decisions made 25,39,40 intuitively or deliberatively. Deliberation has reduced decisional uncertainty in other contexts, although this may not reflect better decisions because the effect may be an artifact of confirmation bias. Regardless, we found similarly low levels of decisional uncertainty in both groups. Although encouraging deliberation neither changed nor improved the quality of choices for specific life-sustaining interventions, there was a signal that patients in the deliberative group may have been more likely to choose a low-intensity overall approach to treatment. It is plausible that careful reflection may have helped patients articulate their broader preferences without influencing their preferences for specific treatments. However, even for this analysis the Bayes factor provided anecdotal support for the null hypothesis that the interventions do not produce a difference in distribution of choices. Furthermore, this was the only question that had 3 answer choices, and the choices were ordered from low to high intensity for all patients. Thus, patients who were cognitively loaded may have more commonly relied on heuristics such as extremeness aversion (tendency to 42,43 44-46 pick middle options) and recency effects, both of which would bias toward more aggressive choices among patients responding intuitively. Limitations The results of this study should be interpreted in light of several limitations. First, the patients were predominantly male, white, and married or partnered. These factors, combined with the fact that patients were recruited from a single center, may limit generalizability. Second, although more than half of the seriously ill patients we approached enrolled in this trial, it is possible that patients who declined enrollment may have had systematically different preferences for end-of-life treatment. Third, we encouraged patients to deliberate for as long as they wished, but we did not test a prolonged period of deliberation or “facilitated deliberation,” as is advocated under shared decision making. Thus, our results indicate that simply encouraging deliberation is unlikely to improve choices, but it is uncertain whether actively facilitating deliberation, including providing prognostic information and describing a range of potential outcomes, might influence choices. Likewise, it is uncertain whether and how the inclusion of family members or other close contacts in deliberation would influence choices. Fourth, it is possible that participants misunderstood the scenarios presented, which could bias the results toward the null. However, we pilot tested the scenarios among 20 patients prior to launching the trial and observed good understanding. Fifth, more than half of the patients who did enroll in the study had engaged in prior deliberation about end-of-life treatments, as reflected by having a living will or having 2 or more conversations about desired end-of-life treatment in the last year (Table 2). We found no evidence that deliberation changed choices even among patients without these objective markers of prior deliberation, nor that prior deliberation modified the effects of the intervention. However, it is possible that many may have contemplated these choices previously without endorsing either objective marker of prior deliberation. Sixth, the sample size may have limited the study’s ability to detect true effects. In an effort to quantify the strength of the evidence in favor of the null hypothesis (ie, that the interventions did not alter the distributions of choices made), we calculated Bayes factors for the 4 primary decisions made JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 9/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support by the patients about specific interventions. These analyses provide moderate evidence in direct support of the null hypothesis. Conclusions In summary, this randomized clinical trial finds that encouraging seriously ill patients to deliberate before making choices regarding life support may not increase the likelihood that patients will make decisions that best promote their values. Although the scenarios were hypothetical, the findings are salient because the hospitalized patients participating in the study were at high risk of encountering the decisions and health states being studied. These findings may not generalize to other types of patients or decisions, or to contexts in which clinicians guide patients through a deliberative process. However, the results highlight the importance of testing the effects of deliberation on choices made in these other contexts, rather than assuming that deliberative approaches will necessarily improve patients’ decision-making processes. ARTICLE INFORMATION Accepted for Publication: December 7, 2018. Published: January 25, 2019. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2019 Rubin EB et al. JAMA Network Open. Corresponding Author: Emily B. Rubin, MD, JD, Massachusetts General Hospital, 55 Fruit St, Boston, MA 02114 ([email protected]). Author Affiliations: Division of Pulmonary and Critical Care Medicine, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston (Rubin); Fostering Improvement in End-of-Life Decision Science, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Rubin, Cooney, Gabler, Mante, Halpern); University of California, San Diego, School of Medicine, La Jolla (Buehler); Leonard Davis Institute of Health Economics, The Wharton School, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Cooney); The Palliative and Advanced Illness Research Center, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Cooney, Halpern); Division of Pulmonary, Allergy and Critical Care Medicine, Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Halpern); Department of Biostatistics, Epidemiology, and Informatics, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Halpern); Department of Medical Ethics and Health Policy, Perelman School of Medicine, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia (Halpern). Author Contributions: Dr Rubin had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Concept and design: Rubin, Cooney, Halpern. Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors. Drafting of the manuscript: Rubin. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors. Statistical analysis: Rubin, Gabler. Obtained funding: Rubin, Halpern. Administrative, technical, or material support: Cooney, Mante, Halpern. Supervision: Cooney. Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported. Funding/Support: This research was funded in part by a grant from the Center for Health Incentives and Behavioral Economics at the University of Pennsylvania. Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The Center for Health Incentives and Behavioral Economics had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication. Disclaimer: The interpretation and reporting of these data are the sole responsibility of the authors. Data Sharing Statement: See Supplement 3. JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 10/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support REFERENCES 1. National Quality Forum (NQF). Safe practices for better healthcare—2010 update: a consensus report. https:// www.qualityforum.org/Publications/2010/04/Safe_Practices_for_Better_Healthcare_%E2%80%93_2010_ Update.aspx. Accessed December 28, 2018. 2. US Department of Health and Human Services National Institute on Aging. Advance care planning. https://www. nia.nih.gov/health/caregiving/advance-care-planning. Accessed December 28, 2018. 3. Snyder L; American College of Physicians Ethics, Professionalism, and Human Rights Committee. American College of Physicians Ethics Manual: sixth edition. Ann Intern Med. 2012;156(1, pt 2):73-104. doi:10.7326/0003- 4819-156-1-201201031-00001 4. Butler M, Ratner E, McCreedy E, Shippee N, Kane RL. Decision aids for advance care planning: an overview of the state of the science. Ann Intern Med. 2014;161(6):408-418. doi:10.7326/M14-0644 5. O’Connor AM, Wennberg JE, Legare F, et al. Toward the ‘tipping point’: decision aids and informed patient choice. Health Aff (Millwood). 2007;26(3):716-725. doi:10.1377/hlthaff.26.3.716 6. Oshima Lee E, Emanuel EJ. Shared decision making to improve care and reduce costs. N Engl J Med. 2013;368 (1):6-8. doi:10.1056/NEJMp1209500 7. Stanovich KE, West RF. Individual differences in reasoning: implications for the rationality debate? Behav Brain Sci. 2000;23(5):645-665. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003435 8. Kahneman D. A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. Am Psychol. 2003;58(9): 697-720. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.58.9.697 9. Kahneman D, Frederick S. Frames and brains: elicitation and control of response tendencies. Trends Cogn Sci. 2007;11(2):45-46. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.11.007 10. Tversky A, Kahneman D. Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science. 1974;185(4157): 1124-1131. doi:10.1126/science.185.4157.1124 11. Usher M, Russo Z, Weyers M, Brauner R, Zakay D. The impact of the mode of thought in complex decisions: intuitive decisions are better. Front Psychol. 2011;2:37. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00037 12. Wilson TD, Schooler JW. Thinking too much: introspection can reduce the quality of preferences and decisions. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991;60(2):181-192. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.181 13. Dijksterhuis A, Bos MW, Nordgren LF, van Baaren RB. On making the right choice: the deliberation-without- attention effect. Science. 2006;311(5763):1005-1007. doi:10.1126/science.1121629 14. Calvillo DP, Penaloza A. Are complex decisions better left to the unconscious? further failed replications of the deliberation-without-attention effect. Judgm Decis Mak. 2009;4(6):509-517. 15. Woolley A, Kostopoulou O, Delaney BC. Can medical diagnosis benefit from “unconscious thought”? Med Decis Making. 2016;36(4):541-549. doi:10.1177/0272989X15581352 16. Mercier H, Sperber D. Why do humans reason? arguments for an argumentative theory. Behav Brain Sci. 2011; 34(2):57-74. doi:10.1017/S0140525X10000968 17. Ayal S, Rusou Z, Zakay D, Hochman G. Determinants of judgment and decision making quality: the interplay between information processing style and situational factors. Front Psychol. 2015;6:1088. doi:10.3389/fpsyg. 2015.01088 18. Greene JD, Morelli SA, Lowenberg K, Nystrom LE, Cohen JD. Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. Cognition. 2008;107(3):1144-1154. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.11.004 19. Suter RS, Hertwig R. Time and moral judgment. Cognition. 2011;119(3):454-458. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011. 01.018 20. Gervais WM, Norenzayan A. Analytic thinking promotes religious disbelief. Science. 2012;336(6080): 493-496. doi:10.1126/science.1215647 21. Hinson JM, Jameson TL, Whitney P. Impulsive decision making and working memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2003;29(2):298-306. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.298 22. Rand DG, Greene JD, Nowak MA. Spontaneous giving and calculated greed. Nature. 2012;489(7416): 427-430. doi:10.1038/nature11467 23. Rand DG, Epstein ZG. Risking your life without a second thought: intuitive decision-making and extreme altruism. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e109687. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0109687 24. Scherer LD, de Vries M, Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Witteman HO, Fagerlin A. Trust in deliberation: the consequences of deliberative decision strategies for medical decisions. Health Psychol. 2015;34(11):1090-1099. doi:10.1037/ hea0000203 JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 11/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support 25. Schulz JF, Fischbacher U, Thöni C, Utikal V. Affect and fairness: dictator games under cognitive load. J Econ Psychol. 2014;41:77-87. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2012.08.007 26. Filoteo JV, Lauritzen S, Maddox WT. Removing the frontal lobes: the effects of engaging executive functions on perceptual category learning. Psychol Sci. 2010;21(3):415-423. doi:10.1177/0956797610362646 27. Navon D, Miller J. Queuing or sharing? a critical evaluation of the single-bottleneck notion. Cogn Psychol. 2002;44(3):193-251. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0767 28. Alter AL, Oppenheimer DM, Epley N, Eyre RN. Overcoming intuition: metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2007;136(4):569-576. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.569 29. O’Connor AM. Validation of a decisional conflict scale. Med Decis Making. 1995;15(1):25-30. doi:10.1177/ 0272989X9501500105 30. Ayres P. Using subjective measures to detect variations of intrinsic cognitive load within problems. Learn Instr. 2006;16(5):389-400. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.09.001 31. Paas F, Tuovinen JE, Tabbers H, Van Gerven PWM. Cognitive load measurement as a means to advance cognitive load theory. Educ Psychol. 2003;38(1):63-71. doi:10.1207/S15326985EP3801_8 32. Rubin EB, Buehler AE, Halpern SD. States worse than death among hospitalized patients with serious illnesses. JAMA Intern Med. 2016;176(10):1557-1559. doi:10.1001/jamainternmed.2016.4362 33. Lee MD, Wagenmakers EJ. Bayesian Cognitive Modeling: A Practical Course. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press; 2014. 34. Stacey D, Légaré F, Lewis K, et al. Decision aids for people facing health treatment or screening decisions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2017;4:CD001431. 35. Halpern SD, Loewenstein G, Volpp KG, et al. Default options in advance directives influence how patients set goals for end-of-life care. Health Aff (Millwood). 2013;32(2):408-417. doi:10.1377/hlthaff.2012.0895 36. Kressel LM, Chapman GB, Leventhal E. The influence of default options on the expression of end-of-life treatment preferences in advance directives. J Gen Intern Med. 2007;22(7):1007-1010. doi:10.1007/s11606-007- 0204-6 37. Sepucha K, Ozanne E, Silvia K, Partridge A, Mulley AG Jr. An approach to measuring the quality of breast cancer decisions. Patient Educ Couns. 2007;65(2):261-269. doi:10.1016/j.pec.2006.08.007 38. Ubel PA. Is information always a good thing? helping patients make “good” decisions. Med Care. 2002;40(9) (suppl):V39-V44. 39. Aldag RJ, Power DJ. An empirical assessment of computer-assisted decision analysis. Decis Sci. 1986;17(4): 572-588. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5915.1986.tb00243.x 40. Davis EL, McCaffery K, Mullan B, Juraskova I. An exploration of decision aid effectiveness: the impact of promoting affective vs. deliberative processing on a health-related decision. Health Expect. 2015;18(6): 2742-2752. doi:10.1111/hex.12248 41. Navajas J, Bahador B, Latham PE. Post-decisional accounts of biases in confidence. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2016; 11:55-60. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.005 42. Simonson I, Tversky A. Choice in context: tradeoff contrast and extremeness aversion. J Mark Res. 1992;29 (3):281-295. doi:10.1177/002224379202900301 43. Tversky A, Simonson I. Context-dependent preferences. Manage Sci. 1993;39(10):1179-1189. doi:10.1287/mnsc. 39.10.1179 44. Glanzer M. Storage mechanisms in recall. In: Bower GH, ed. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Advances in Research and Theory. Vol 5. New York, NY: Academic Press; 1972:129-193. 45. Waugh NC, Norman DA. Primary memory. Psychol Rev. 1965;72(2):89-104. doi:10.1037/h0021797 46. Krosnick JA, Alwin DF. An evaluation of a cognitive theory of response-order effects in survey measurement. Public Opin Q. 1987;51(2):201-219. doi:10.1086/269029 SUPPLEMENT 1. Trial Protocol SUPPLEMENT 2. eFigure 1. Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria eFigure 2. Distribution of Primary Diagnoses Among Patients Enrolled in the Study eFigure 3. Scenarios Requiring Decision About Life-Sustaining Treatment eFigure 4. Distribution of Subjective Rating of Mental Effort in Intuitive Arm Versus Deliberative Arm JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 12/13 JAMA Network Open | Critical Care Medicine Intuitive vs Deliberative Approaches to Making Decisions About Life Support eTable 1. Interaction Between Selected Characteristics and Treatment Arm for the Outcome of Treatment Decisions eTable 2. Decisional Uncertainty by Arm SUPPLEMENT 3. Data Sharing Statement JAMA Network Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 (Reprinted) January 25, 2019 13/13 P A A ) ) P ) ) ) ) ) ) ) ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P ) P Supplementary Online Content Rubin EB, Buehler AE, Cooney E, Gabler NB, Mante AA, Halpern SD. Intuitive vs deliberative approaches to making decisions about life support: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Netw Open. 2019;2(1):e187851. doi:10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2018.7851 eFigure 1. Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria eFigure 2. Distribution of Primary Diagnoses Among Patients Enrolled in the Study eFigure 3. Scenarios Requiring Decision About Life-Sustaining Treatment eFigure 4. Distribution of Subjective Rating of Mental Effort in Intuitive Arm Versus Deliberative Arm eTable 1. Interaction Between Selected Characteristics and Treatment Arm for the Outcome of Treatment Decisions eTable 2. Decisional Uncertainty by Arm This supplementary material has been provided by the authors to give readers additional information about their work. JAMA Network Open. eFigure 1. Study inclusion and exclusion criteria Inclusion criteria: 1. Age 60 or older 2. Speaks and reads fluently in English 3. Currently an inpatient at the Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania 4. Stable vital signs 5. Listed as “full code” in the electronic medical record 6. Currently admitted to the oncology, cardiology or pulmonary service and has been admitted to the same service at least once in the last year; and/or has one or more of the following diagnoses: Chronic obstructive pulmonary disease with at least severe airflow obstruction on most recent spirometry and/or eligible for long-term oxygen therapy Incurable interstitial lung disease with at least severe restriction on most recent pulmonary function tests and/or eligible for long-term oxygen therapy Congestive heart failure with NYHA Class III or higher and current hospitalization related to heart failure Acute myeloid leukemia Stage IV lymphoma Stage IIIB or Stage IV non-small cell lung cancer, cholangiocarcinoma, renal cell carcinoma, breast cancer, uterine cancer, cervical cancer, ovarian cancer, colorectal cancer, gastric cancer, pancreatic cancer, prostate cancer, urothelial cancer Stage C or D hepatocellular carcinoma Mesothelioma or any malignancy metastatic to the pleura Exclusion criteria Cognitive impairment to the point where patient is unable to provide informed consent or to understand instructions Uncontrolled or poorly controlled pain, dyspnea or other symptoms on the day of recruitment Currently has feeding tube or tracheostomy tube Currently undergoing evaluation for solid organ transplantation First admission to the hospital following diagnosis of a serious illness JAMA Network Open. eFigure 2. Distribution of primary diagnoses among patients enrolled in the study CHF, NYHA class 2 Less advanced solid malignancy Hematological malignancy, other Multiple myeloma CHF, NYHA class 4 Severe pulmonary disease Advanced stage lymphoma CHF, NYHA class 3 Advanced solid malignancy Acute leukemia 0 5 10 15 20 Patients, % Abbreviations: NYHA, New York Heart Association “Advanced solid malignancy” was defined as Stage IIIB or Stage IV non-small cell lung cancer, cholangiocarcinoma, renal cell carcinoma, breast cancer, uterine cancer, cervical cancer, ovarian cancer, colorectal cancer, gastric cancer, pancreatic cancer, prostate cancer, urothelial cancer, stage C or D hepatocellular carcinoma, mesothelioma or any malignancy metastatic to the pleura. Lymphoma patients all had stage IV disease. JAMA Network Open. Diagnosis eFigure 3. Scenarios requiring decision about life-sustaining treatment Imagine that in the future you become very weak and develop difficulty swallowing. You are unable to eat or drink safely. When you do, small amounts of food and liquid go into your lungs and cause trouble with your breathing. Doctors think that this will be a permanent condition. The only way for you to get nutrition safely is to have a feeding tube inserted into your stomach. For the rest of your life, you will most likely have some trouble getting around, need some help with basic life activities like dressing yourself, and have mild pain or discomfort. Under these circumstances, would you choose: To have a feeding tube inserted into your stomach to give you food and fluids with the goal of living as long as possible; or Not to have a feeding tube inserted into your stomach and continue to eat and drink small amounts, understanding that you might die sooner than you would if you had a feeding tube inserted Imagine that in the future you become ill to the point that you are expected to die within the next several months. This means that you have some days when you drift in and out of awareness, have some discomfort that requires medication, are in bed most of the time due to weakness and need help getting dressed, bathing, and using the bathroom. You develop an infection. If you receive antibiotics, you might live another several months in a nursing home or similar facility in the condition I described above. If you don’t receive antibiotics, you likely will die within a week to two weeks. Under these circumstances, would you choose: To treat the infection with the goal of living as long as possible; or Not to treat the infection, receive medication to treat any uncomfortable symptoms, and die at home Imagine that in the future you develop a life-threatening illness with difficulty breathing. Doctors think there is a 50% chance you will survive this illness. In order to survive, you will likely need support from a breathing machine for at least two weeks. Doctors expect that if you survive and go through a long period of rehabilitation, you will be able to return home. If you survive, it is likely that you will have a lot of trouble getting around, will need some help with activities like getting dressed and using the bathroom, and will have mild pain or discomfort. Under these circumstances, would you choose: To have the breathing tube put in; or To be made comfortable with medications and die without a breathing tube Imagine that you were to become very sick in the near future and your loved ones and doctors were trying to decide how to best care for you. I am going to describe three general approaches to medical treatment in the event of serious illness. Please tell me which approach best describes how you would wish to be treated if you were to become very sick in the near future. If I were to become very sick, I would want the main focus of my care to be on treating any uncomfortable symptoms I might have and not on keeping me alive as long as possible. I would want to limit the amount of time I spent in the hospital and the number of tests and treatments I received. I would not wish to have any procedures that would cause me pain and I would not want to go to the intensive care unit. I would want life support measures only if they were absolutely necessary to keep me comfortable and would not want to be put or kept on life support with the goal of extending my life. I would like to be allowed to die naturally. If my heart were to stop, I would not want my medical team to take extraordinary measures to try to restart it. JAMA Network Open. If I were to become very sick, I would want the main focus of my care at first to be on keeping me alive as long as possible, even if that means that I would have to spend a lot of time in the hospital and have some procedures done that might cause me pain or discomfort. If I needed to go to the intensive care unit, I would want to do that. If I needed life support measures to keep me alive, I would want those for a little while. But I would not want to live for a long time on life support or in another condition where I would be unable to leave the hospital or another health care facility. If after a trial of life support, it looked very unlikely that I would get better and be able to go home, I would want the main focus of my care to shift to treating any uncomfortable symptoms I have and away from trying to keep me alive as long as possible. I would like to be allowed to die naturally. If my heart were to stop, I would not want my medical team to take extraordinary measures to try to restart it. If I were to become very sick, I would want the focus of my care to be on keeping me alive as long as possible, even if that means that I would have to spend a lot of time in the hospital and have some procedures done that might cause me pain or discomfort. If I needed to go to the intensive care unit, I would want to do that. If I needed life support measures to keep me alive, I would want to have them. I would want to have any medical treatment or procedure that would keep me alive. If my heart were to stop, I would want my medical team to take every possible measure to try to restart it. The figure depicts the distribution in the two arms of subjective ratings of mental effort required to remember the designated numbers while hearing the scenarios and answering questions about preferences (0 = very, very little; 10 = very, very much). 93% of patients in the deliberative arm rated the mental effort as 0, while 72% of patients in the intuitive arm rated the mental effort as 5 or greater. n=97 in the intuitive arm; n=102 in the deliberative arm. JAMA Network Open. eFigure 4. Distribution of subjective rating of mental effort in intuitive arm versus deliberative arm Intuitive Deliberative 0123456789 10 Subjective rating of mental effort 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 Deliberative 93% 4% 1% 0% 0% 0% 0% 1% 0% 0% 1% Intuitive 0% 5% 8% 7% 7% 21% 14% 19% 12% 2% 4% JAMA Network Open. Patients, % a eTable 1. Interaction between selected characteristics and treatment arm for the outcome of treatment decisions Patients, (%) Intuitive Deliberative P value of Arm Arm interaction term Accept feeding tube .47 Living will yes 32 38 Living will no or don’t know 56 52 Accept antibiotics .10 Living will yes 24 38 Living will no or don’t know 58 52 Accept intubation .53 Living will yes 52 57 Living will no or don’t know 67 64 Accept tracheostomy .67 Living will yes 36 36 Living will no or don’t know 38 46 Preference for high intensity .53 treatment Low intensity Living will yes 35 53 Living will no or don’t know 24 34 Moderate intensity Living will yes 55 38 Living will no or don’t know 37 50 High intensity Living will yes 10 9 Living will no or don’t know 40 16 Accept feeding tube .03 Previous EOL discussions yes 47 36 Previous EOL discussions no 36 56 Accept antibiotics .94 Previous EOL discussions yes 38 43 Previous EOL discussions no 41 44 Accept intubation .97 Previous EOL discussions yes 56 57 Previous EOL discussions no 62 27 Accept tracheostomy .26 Previous EOL discussions yes 39 51 Previous EOL discussions no 35 27 Preference for high intensity .34 treatment Low intensity Previous EOL discussions yes 31 43 Previous EOL discussions no 28 49 Moderate intensity Previous EOL discussions yes 49 43 Previous EOL discussions n0 44 42 High intensity JAMA Network Open. Previous EOL discussions yes 20 13 Previous EOL discussions no 28 9 Abbreviation: EOL, end of life. This table reports treatment decisions by arm stratified by (a) endorsement of living will and (b) endorsement of having had at least two conversations in the last year about end of life treatment preferences. The decisions are reported as percentages. Sample sizes: Living will yes, n=54 in intuitive arm, n=58 in deliberative arm; Living will no or don’t know, n=43 in intuitive arm, n=44 in deliberative arm; previous EOL discussions yes, n=55 in intuitive arm, n=61 in deliberative arm; previous EOL discussions no, n=42 in intuitive arm, n=41 in deliberative arm JAMA Network Open. a eTable 2. Decisional uncertainty by arm Mean (SD) Scenario P value Total Sample Intuitive Arm Deliberative Arm (n=199) (n=97) (n=102) Feeding tube 21 (22) 21 (23) 21 (21) .86 Antibiotics 20 (21) 20 (22) 19 (21) .65 Intubation 23 (24) 26 (26) 21 (22) .11 Tracheostomy 26 (25) 29 (25) 24 (25) .22 Treatment approach 21 (22) 23 (22) 20 (21) .33 Decisional uncertainty was evaluated using the uncertainty subscale of the Decisional Conflict Scale. 0 = feels extremely certain about best choice, 100 = feels extremely uncertain about best choice. All values are reported as mean (standard deviation). JAMA Network Open.

Journal

JAMA Network OpenAmerican Medical Association

Published: Jan 25, 2019

There are no references for this article.