Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 7-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

Modeling the Association between 43 Different Clinical and Pathological Variables and the Severity of Cognitive Impairment in a Large Autopsy Cohort of Elderly Persons

Modeling the Association between 43 Different Clinical and Pathological Variables and the... INTRODUCTION Cognitive impairment linked to aging (CILA), a leading cause of morbidity and mortality, spans a continuum from subtle cognitive changes to end‐stage dementia. We used the term “CILA” to include cognitive impairment in an aging population regardless of diagnoses or of predicted eventual clinical course. Multiple etiologies contribute to CILA at a population level and coincident pathologies are frequently observed in aged individuals (11, 18, 87, 89, 109) . As prospects for specific brain disease therapies improve (45, 50) , it is increasingly important to know the relative weights of the distinct contributing factors in CILA. Rubrics are lacking to relate the severity of CILA to the presence and/or extent of most brain diseases. Alzheimer's disease (AD) has a relatively well‐documented association between the amount of neuropathology and the severity of ante–mortem cognitive impairment (6, 10, 14, 20, 48, 52, 85) , yet controversy remains about the contribution(s) of amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) to clinical deterioration (24, 76, 87) . Pathological processes also linked to CILA include cerebrovascular disease (CVD), dementia with Lewy bodies, argyrophilic grains (AGs), frontotemporal dementias (FTDs) and hippocampal sclerosis (HS) (9, 11, 14, 24, 66, 72, 74, 78, 86, 87, 91, 108, 109, 120) . In addition, there are clinical comorbidities that may contribute to CILA despite the absence of definite neuropathological hallmarks to support these associations (9, 74, 90) . Examples of clinical features that have been associated with worsened CILA include diabetes (40, 41, 54) , coronary artery bypass graft (17, 44, 97) , and “extreme old age” (63, 65, 125) . In contrast, there is an association with improved cognition, or decreased CILA, in persons with high levels of education and/or early linguistic ability (74, 82, 96, 108) . How can we weigh the importance of each disease mechanism in CILA? Despite great scientific progress in recent years, both normal and disease mechanisms cannot be modeled with certainty outside the aging human brain. Hence, careful clinicopathological correlations must be performed on relatively large human cohorts to tease out the importance of the factors involved in CILA. This is a formidable challenge. Precise correlation of ante–mortem cognitive measures, clinical variables and post‐mortem neuropathology requires a long‐term, prospective approach involving a multidisciplinary team. Patients must be recruited, tested longitudinally at regular intervals with thorough collection of demographic and clinical data, evaluated proximal to death, and then, upon autopsy, assessed systematically for neuropathology. A large number of patients must be studied to enable adequate statistical testing. Finally, there must be statistical tools to analyze how the ante–mortem parameters correlate to the neuropathology observed after autopsy, to produce data that are clinically and biologically relevant. The University of Kentucky Alzheimer's Disease Center (UK ADC) studies older persons with a full gamut of cognitive impairment (annual target n = 700 subjects). Along with thorough longitudinal mental status evaluations, many clinical and demographic data are obtained on every clinical visit. All subjects agreed to brain donation following death. To date, the autopsy rate on this cohort is over 90%. To evaluate the contributions of clinical, demographic and neuropathological factors to CILA, we utilized a broad strategy to assess quantitatively which factors are most directly associated with CILA in this large autopsy convenience cohort. Methodology included statistical regression modeling of 43 different clinical and pathological variables in relation to the severity of ante–mortem cognitive impairment. This approach provides the basis for some unforeseen conclusions. MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects Patients who had come to autopsy from the UK ADC cohorts (total n = 501) were the basis for the study. Research protocols were approved by the University of Kentucky Institutional Review Board. Details of the UK ADC recruitment were described previously (78, 87) . Initially non‐demented subjects (“initially normal” cohort) comprised individuals (n = 212) from the UK Biologically Resilient Adults in Neurological Studies (BRAiNS) cohort (101) . The “dementia clinic” group (n = 122) derived from the patients followed longitudinally in the UK dementia clinic, who had presented with cognitive symptoms. Characteristics of these groups are shown in Table 1 . 1 Cohort demographics and numbers. Data from two different research clinics show distinct demographic profiles in the groups. Although some Alzheimer's disease‐type pathology was found in many cases from both clinics, other concomitant neuropathological features including infarcts and Lewy bodies (LBs) were also quite common. Abbreviations: ApoE = apolipoprotein E; MMSE = mini‐mental status examination. Research clinic n Sex (% F) Formal education, years ApoE allele 2/3/4% Age at death Final MMSE score Years since final MMSE % with infarcts (all types) % with cortical LBs Initially normal 212 58 16.0 8/77/15 85.9 26.3 0.82 55.2 8.0 (standard deviation) — — ±2.4 — ±7.7 ±5.4 ±0.6 — — Dementia clinic 122 61 12.9 3/64/33 80.8 10.4 1.55 35.2 26.2 (standard deviation) — — ±3.1 — ±7.0 ±7.4 ±1.2 — — Combined/total 334 59 14.8 6/71/23 84.0 20.4 1.09 48.0 14.7 Criteria for exclusion involved either inadequate clinical documentation (see further discussion) or the presence of pathology for which statistical power were lacking to model their impact on cognition ( Table 2 ); that is, low patient numbers for rare or idiosyncratic diseases. Thus, the following were excluded: primary or metastatic neoplasm in brain parenchyma including pituitary adenomas; >2 cm contusions in the brain parenchyma; hydrocephalus (except ex vacuo type); infectious agents identified in the brain parenchyma; and a history of left‐sided frontal and/or temporal/parietal infarct with clinically documented aphasia that would seriously confound the results of standard mental status evaluation. Only patients who had MMSE scores within 4 years of death were used in this project [as in (92) ]. Inadequate or non‐recent MMSE data (n = 120 cases) were the criteria that most frequently led to exclusion from this study. Our sample had insufficient patients with FTDs and/or tauopathies (corticobasal degeneration, Pick's disease, FTD‐motor neuron disease, progressive supranuclear palsy, other tauopathies and dementia lacking distinct histopathology; n = 17 total) to provide statistical power for a linear regression‐based study. 2 Exclusion criteria and the numbers excluded from each research clinic according to the reason for exclusion. From the total cases that came to autopsy at the University of Kentucky Alzheimer's Disease Center (n = 501), a total of 167 were excluded according to the criteria shown. By far the most frequent criterion for exclusion were undocumented mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) scores (n = 94) or cases for whom MMSE scores were documented >4 years prior to death (n = 17), which were most often seen in the high‐dementia clinic. Abbreviations: FTD = frontotemporal dementia. Reason for exclusion from study n (cohort) Total Initially normal Dementia clinic Chronic infection 2 0 2 Subdural/chronic hematoma 3 2 5 Contusions 1 4 5 Tumor (including adenomas) 9 1 10 Hydrocephalus (not ex vacuo ) 1 1 2 Infarcts + aphasia 2 1 3 Undocumented MMSE scores 1 93 94 >4 years since final MMSE score 2 27 29 FTDs, all subtypes 2 15 17 Total 23 144 167 Testing All initially normal individuals were contacted at 6‐month intervals, had detailed mental status testing, and we attempted to conduct neurological and physical examinations annually. For persons with clinical symptoms of dementia, we attempted to obtain mental status testing and neurological examinations annually. Mental status testing of our subjects has been described previously (78, 87) . The present study focuses on the MMSE conducted closest to the date of death as the “severity metric” for cognition. MMSE was chosen as it was the most consistently available measure obtained from both normal and demented subjects over the course of their evaluations (and see Discussion). Neuropathology Most aspects of pathological assessments were as described in detail previously (78, 87) . Briefly, a total of at least 25 sections were taken from each brain that included the middle frontal gyrus (MFG; area 9), superior and middle temporal gyri (SMT; areas 21 and 22), inferior parietal lobule (IPL; areas 39 and 40) and the occipital lobe including primary visual area (Occ; areas 17 and 18). Amyloid plaques were separated into plaques without neurites (DPs) and plaques with degenerating neurites (NPs) in each region as described previously (87) . An arithmetic mean was calculated from the count of the five most involved fields for DPs (number of DPs per 2.35 mm 2 ), NPs (number of NPs per 2.35 mm 2 ), and NFTs (number of NFTs per 0.586 mm 2 ) for each region. TDP‐43 immunohistochemistry (IHC) was performed on hippocampal and superior and middle temporal (SMT) gyri sections from a subset of cases ( n = 49). We hypothesized that aberrant TDP‐43 IHC (abTDP‐43) might be found disproportionately in patients whose final MMSE scores were lower than predicted by the statistical model because the pathology may contribute to their lower MMSE scores. We selected cases at random, except that we included 10 cases with the largest deviation from the mean such that (Predicted MMSE‐Actual MMSE) was greatest. Sections were cut on to Probe‐On slides at 5 micron thickness. Rabbit polyclonal anti‐TDP‐43 (1:500; ProteinTech, Chicago, IL, USA) was used following antigen retrieval (Trilogy pH 8.0, Cell Marque, Austin, TX, USA) in a Decloaking Chamber (Biocare Medical, Concord, CA, USA) and then formic acid pretreatment (3 minutes). Secondary antibody reaction employed the Vectastain ABC kit (Vector Labs, Burlingame, CA, USA). TDP‐43 IHC was evaluated by a neuropathologist blinded to the case identifications, and graded semiquantitively (see Analyses section and Supporting Information Table S1). Analyses The UK ADC database was built from in‐depth patient interviews, formal longitudinal cognitive and capacity assessments, pre‐ and post‐mortem chart reviews, extensive neuropathological investigation that includes periodic audits, and back‐maintenance on clinical and pathological data that extend from 1984. Unknown medical history items were assumed to indicate absence only after rigorous data extraction from clinical and research charts (otherwise, data were imputed as described in the following discussion). The earliest autopsy used in this series was performed in 1988. Forty‐three different clinical and pathological variables were abstracted, to encompass most of the prevalent factors that have shown associations with CILA ( Table 3 ). Variables were operationalized to determine the relative weight of many potential clinical and pathological contributors to CILA. 3 The 43 clinical and pathological variables used in the multiple regression algorithm to find which indices are associated with variability in mini‐mental status examination scores proximal to death. Items are operationalized as described in the Supporting Information. Most of the clinical variables are dichotomous (1/0), whereas most of the pathological variables were scored on a continuum from zero to one by graded increments. “Advanced coronary artery disease” is defined by having documented myocardial infarction, coronary artery bypass graft and/or coronary artery angioplasty. Abbreviations: ApoE = apolipoprotein E; AFib = atrial fibrillation. Clinical indices Pathological indices Age at death Sex Macroinfarct(s)—cortical Education Cerebral amyloid angiopathy ApoE alleles Arteriolosclerosis AFib/other cardiac arrhythmia * Number of microinfarcts Hypertension * Number of pale infarcts Body mass index Number of hemorrhagic infarcts Transient ischemic attack(s) * Number of lacunar infarcts Head trauma * Subcortical non‐lacunar infarcts Diabetes/takes insulin * Argyrophilic grains Seizures/epilepsy * Lewy bodies—isocortical Smoking * Lewy bodies—brainstem Peripheral vascular disease * Lewy bodies—medial temporal lobe Advanced coronary artery disease * Lewy bodies—amygdala only * Cancer * Isocortical neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) Anxiety * Isocortical neuritic plaques (NPs) Depression/antidepressant meds * Isocortical diffuse plaques (DPs) Number of documented drugs Mesial temporal lobe NFTs Opiates * Mesial temporal lobe NPs Antipsychotics * Mesial temporal lobe DPs Barbiturates * Hippocampal sclerosis (HS) unilateral * Benzodiazepenes * HS bilateral * * Dichotomous (1 or 0). Each variable was rendered along a severity continuum from zero to one in a manner hypothesized to be biologically and clinically relevant. “Isocortical” pathology refers to pathology in the MFG, IPL, SMT, and Occ cortices as described earlier. Medial temporal pathology is from amygdala plus CA1, subiculum and entorhinal cortex of the hippocampal formation. The criteria and methods for each variable operationalization are outlined in Supporting Information Table S1. Briefly, some of the variables were dichotomous (0 or 1; for example, history of seizures) whereas for others the severity continua was textured. For example, operationalization of AGs was graded from 0 to 1 proportional to a scoring system wherein AGs were scored semiquantitatively (0–3) from three areas: entorhinal cortex; subiculum; and cornu ammonis fields. AGs have been posited to develop first in the ambient gyrus (99, 100) ; however, sampling the ambient gyrus dependably (particularly in cases of end‐stage dementia) is a challenge, and the immediately adjacent entorhinal cortex is a reliable area for detecting early‐stage AGs (21) . We treated apolipoprotein E (ApoE) genotype as ApoE 2/2 (reference, 0 point), ApoE 2/3 (0.25 point), ApoE 2/4 or 3/3 (0.5 point), ApoE 3/4 (0.75 point) or ApoE 4/4 (1 point). A subset of 54 cases (14.9% of the cohort overall) had a documentation of demographic information, and neuropathology was performed, but medical records were not available. Missing items for these cases were multiply imputed using the logistic regression method in SAS 9.1.3 ® PROC MI (Chicago, IL, USA). Covariates were used to predict the missing items include age at death, sex, number of infarcts and number of prescription drugs taken. Grading of arteriolosclerosis severity at autopsy was missing in 28% of cases. These missing data were multiply imputed using the logistic regression method in PROC MI. Covariates were used to predict the missing items include age at death, total number of macro infarcts and total number of micro infarcts from the other cases where records were complete. Four additional items were also multiply imputed: mesial temporal cortex Lewy bodies (0.3%); amygdala‐only Lewy bodies (2.2%); ApoE (4.96%); and body mass index (27.0%). These items were multiply imputed using the MCMC method in PROC MI. The fully imputed data were analyzed using PROC REG, and the results were combined using PROC MIANALYZE. Imputation and modeling were performed with SAS 9.1.3 ® . For additional information about SAS and also about the results of model‐checking procedures, including delta–betas and backward selection, please see Supporting Information Table S2. Stratified analyses were conducted based on initial group membership (“initially normal” or “dementia clinic” cohorts). Linear models were fitted with adjusted MMSE as the dependent variable, and all the demographic, clinical and pathological variables as predictors to find the negative regressors ( Table 4 ). Stepwise model selection was used to determine the subset of regressors that best predicts the final MMSE at the 0.10 level of significance. Because multiple imputation methods for binary data may produce biased estimates, models for the “dementia clinic” group were fit both excluding the 54 cases where all clinical data were missing and including the imputed data. Because the results did not appreciably differ, the fully imputed data were used in the final analyses that are presented. 4 Results of multiple variable linear regression analyses using all clinical and pathological variables for each research cohort separately. The research cohorts were analyzed separately first. Note that the linear regression found large differences between the cohorts in the y ‐intercept, and in each case the isocortical NFTs was the largest parameter estimate. Other regressors differed, which may indicate biological differences in the parameters that affect relatively low‐CILA and high‐CILA individuals. Regressors with positive parameter estimates were not listed on this table. There are no such regressors in the “initially normal” group but ApoE and presence of diabetes were positive parameter estimates in the “dementia clinic” group. Abbreviations: ApoE = apolipoprotein E; CILA = cognitive impairment linked to aging; HS = hippocampal sclerosis; LB = Lewy body; NFT = neurofibrillary tangle; TIA = transient ischemic attack. Parameter Parameter estimate Standard error Pr > |t| “Initially normal” group n = 212 Intercept 29.49 0.31 <0.0001 NFTs, isocortex −24.93 4.30 <0.0001 NFTs, mesial temporal −16.10 2.09 <0.0001 HS—unilateral −12.53 1.48 <0.0001 HS—bilateral −5.50 1.65 0.001 LBs, mesial temporal −5.61 1.56 0.0004 LBs, amygdala only −3.73 1.51 0.0146 History of TIA −1.55 0.72 0.0334 “Dementia clinic” group, n = 122 Intercept 10.47 2.05 <0.0001 NFTs, isocortex −16.42 2.42 <0.0001 LBs, neocortex −5.07 2.10 0.0177 Antipsychotics −4.61 2.01 0.0237 Seizures −4.31 1.91 0.0257 Cancer −2.84 1.40 0.0447 To assess the effect of the clinical predictors on final MMSE scores without the neuropathological predictors, and vice versa, stepwise models using only clinical or pathological variable predictors were fit using the fully imputed, combined data ( Table 5 ). Lastly, in addition to the stratified analyses, a model was fit combining both groups using the pool of significant negative regressors ( Table 5 ). Severity of isocortical NPs and lacunar infarcts were included because they were significantly associated with CILA in the pathology‐only regression. ApoE alleles were included in the regression because they were significantly associated with CILA in the clinical‐only regression. 5 Regression modeling of parameters from entire research cohort (n = 334)—results of multiple variable linear regression analyses using entire cohort. Some of the included parameters are shown only to demonstrate their lack of independent association, but were also present in prior analyses. Note, in the combined model, ApoE parameter estimates are opposite in the different cohorts (positive in “dementia clinic”, negative in “initially normal”). For the overall group, ApoE did not have a significant effect. If the different cohort‐specific effects were taken into account, then, NPs, isocortex, would lose statistical significance but all the others are roughly the same. This is discussed in the Supporting Information. Abbreviations: ApoE = apolipoprotein E; HS = hippocampal sclerosis; LB = Lewy body; NFT = neurofibrillary tangle; NP = neuritic plaques. Parameter Parameter Estimate Standard error Pr > |t| Clinical parameters only Intercept 10.86 2.06 <0.0001 ApoE alleles −8.73 2.45 0.0004 Pathology parameters Intercept 28.50 0.47 <0.0001 NFTs, isocortex −21.78 2.30 <0.0001 NFTs, mesial temporal −14.48 1.87 <0.0001 LBs, isocortex −13.95 1.67 <0.0001 NPs, isocortex −8.81 1.87 <0.0001 HS—bilateral −7.81 2.10 0.0244 HS—unilateral −6.33 2.82 0.0256 Macroinfarcts −6.09 2.83 0.0323 Lacunar infarcts −4.81 2.51 0.0559 All significant parameters combined Intercept 24.32 1.07 <0.0001 NFTs, isocortex −19.66 2.27 <0.0001 LBs, isocortex −12.47 1.67 <0.0001 HS—unilateral −8.58 1.82 <0.0001 NFTs, mesial temporal −12.44 1.87 <0.0001 HS—bilateral −7.84 2.04 0.0001 NPs, isocortex −6.61 2.71 0.0152 Lacunar infarcts −5.15 2.46 0.037 Age >0.1 LBs, amygdala only >0.1 Sex >0.1 The average number of years between the final MMSE test and death for all cases was 1.09 years and the median was 0.82 years. Thirteen cases in the dataset had intervals between 3.5 and 4 years, and these were predominantly end‐stage dementia patients. As patients' MMSE scores would have declined in the interim presumably (23, 81, 82, 95, 111) , we also performed the linear regression model using an MMSE correction. In this model, for patients with final MMSE scores less than 28, 2.5 points were deducted from the MMSE score for every 12‐month duration between the final MMSE evaluation and death. For the average patient in this study, this involved a “correction” of less than 2.5 MMSE points (median 1.1; range 0–12.1). In this model, the findings were broadly similar to the data reported without the MMSE correction (data not shown). Following the derivation of parameter estimates, we used the regression equation and the covariates to predict the MMSE score. These were arrayed on a Microsoft Excel spreadsheet. Using this database we modeled the contribution of the different clinical and pathological variables to CILA over our entire cohort (n = 334). This also allowed for comparison between the actual final MMSE scores vs. “predicted” MMSE scores based on the parameter estimates. RESULTS Based on the inclusion criteria described earlier, 334 patients from the UK ADC autopsy series served as the research cohort for a detailed analysis of the clinical and pathological parameters that underlie variations in ante–mortem MMSE scores. As shown in the demographics in Table 1 , this cohort comprises predominantly highly educated elderly persons. All cases except one were Caucasian. Over half of the patients had some form of cerebrovascular pathology; of patients beyond 80 years of age, more than 66% had some cerebrovascular pathology. Many of the patients died with minimal CILA, or none, as detected by the MMSE scores. In all, 11% of the cohort had final MMSE scores of 29 or 30. In the first level of analyses, the total cohort was stratified by the group in which the subjects had initially enrolled. The demographics of these two groups differed in some aspects from each other ( Table 1 ). Stratified statistical analyses identified the determinants of MMSE variability with R 2 correlation coefficient of >0.70. In addition to running the multiple linear regression with the parameters mentioned earlier, additional analyses were ran using only the clinical and demographic variables (n = 22), or only the pathological variables (n = 21). When only clinical variables were queried, the impact of ApoE ( P = 0.0004) was manifest. Another model was fit using the data combined from both groups. All the parameters were included from the stratified models, and this final model serves as the source of the results described in the following discussion. Clinical conditions, neuropathology and CILA Parameter estimates provide a numerically weighted indicator of how each clinical and pathological variable correlates to interindividual differences in MMSE scores in our sample. The extent of interpersonal differences in MMSE scores can be predicted referent to each parameter and for every patient. For example, all other variables being equal, the presence of HS is associated with eight fewer MMSE score points. Evaluating parameter estimates and frequencies reveals that more than two‐thirds of the MMSE variability in our study sample was associated with the amount of Alzheimer‐type pathology, NFTs and NPs. This held true whether in the context of “pure” or mixed‐type pathology (for example, with cortical Lewy bodies or cerebrovascular pathology). The AD‐related pathologies associated with CILA were isocortical and mesial temporal NFTs and isocortical NPs [the latter only statistically significant when the whole cohort (n = 334) was analyzed]. The contribution to CILA from AD‐type pathology appeared to differ based on disease severity. In patients with higher MMSE scores, most of the CILA was associated with mesial temporal NFTs and isocortical NPs, whereas with more severe CILA, the isocortical NFTs accounted for a much larger proportion of the cognitive impairment ( Figure 1 ). 1 The association of particular subtypes and anatomical areas of Alzheimer's disease (AD) pathology with cognitive impairment linked to aging (CILA) severity changes according to the different stages of cognitive impairment . This chart shows data from modeling on the entire cohort using the statistically significant parameter coefficients as shown in Table 5 . Cases are binned by mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) scores: MMSE 26–28 (n = 83); MMSE 20–26 (n = 38); MMSE 10–20 (n = 65); and MMSE 0–10 (n = 62). Note that as cognition declines, the apparent impact of isocortical neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) becomes greater, and the relative importance of mesial temporal lobe NFTs and isocortical NPs declines. Nonetheless, each subtype of AD‐type pathology is associated independently with significant amount of CILA. Isocortical LBs tended to be associated with lower MMSE scores ( Table 5 ). However, even advanced LB pathology alone tended not to be associated with end‐stage dementia. The presence of isocortical‐pattern dementia with Lewy bodies sometimes coincided with AD pathology and these pathological processes appear to have an additive clinical effect ( Table 6 ). On average, isocortical LBs correlated with lower MMSE scores by approximately 10 points. These results bear out the regression modeling where the parameter estimate for isocortical LBs in the most severe cases (maximally severe involvement in all four isocortical regions tested) correlate with an approximately 13‐point loss in MMSE score ( Table 5 ). 6 Final mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) scores proximal to death in patients with extensive isocortical Lewy bodies (LBs) or no isocortical LBs, stratified by Braak stages. Moderate‐to‐severe alpha‐synuclein pathology in neocortex is an additive pathology in the University of Kentucky Alzheimer's Disease Center research cohorts. In these patients (“extensive isocortical LBs”); the frequency of concomitant Alzheimer's disease (AD) was approximately the same as in cases without isocortical LBs. Isocortical LB and AD pathologies appear additive with respect to clinical symptoms. Stage for stage, the presence of isocortical LBs appears to correlate with a decrease of approximately 10 points in MMSE scores, consistent with the statistically derived parameter estimates ( Tables 4 and 5 ). The few cases with “pure” isocortical LBs (minimal Alzheimer‐type pathology or infarcts) did not have end‐stage dementia. Braak stages—average MMSE (n) 0–II III, IV V, VI Cases with extensive isocortical LBs, n = 28 17.1 (10) 11.8 (4) 7.1 (14) Cases without isocortical LBs, n = 281 27.6 (106) 25.3 (65) 13.3 (110) The majority of the 43 clinical and pathological variables showed no statistically significant contribution to CILA in our cohort. The stepwise linear regression algorithm flags those variables most directly correlating to changes in MMSE scores. It is presumed that those variables that contribute to cognitive impairment through stimulating other pathological processes would not distinguish themselves as long as the “downstream” factor was also evaluated. A presumptive example of this phenomenon is ApoE. ApoE alleles were not associated with significant independent contribution to MMSE variability unless the pathological variables were not present also as regressors. AGs by pathology were not associated statistically with the severity of CILA in our sample. This was not an artifact of having FTD cases excluded from analyses, because only three FTD cases had minimal AGs, and none of these cases had sufficient amounts to merit the diagnosis of argyrophilic grain disease. Seventy‐five out of 334 (22%) cases had some AGs as assessed using Gallyas silver impregnation. P ‐values for the association of AGs pathology to CILA severity were always >0.5. The operationalization technique allowed a graded score of AGs reflecting the distribution and severity (see Analysis section). Of the seven cases with the highest score of AGs, four had final MMSE scores of 29 or higher, and none had MMSE scores less than 24. These additional analyses ( Figure 2 ) again provide some validation for the statistical modeling data. 2 Statistical methods indicated that argyrophilic grains (AGs) are not associated with CILA severity in our research cohorts . To evaluate these data, we studied separately the 75 cases with AGs in our sample. A chart ( A ) shows how the actual pre‐mortem mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) scores for patients correlated to the amount of AG pathology. The AG score is proportional to the severity of AGs in entorhinal cortex, subiculum and cornu ammonis hippocampal fields. Note that there is no decrease in MMSE scores as AG pathology worsens. A photomicrograph of Gallyas‐stained cornu ammonis ( B ) from the individual in our autopsy cohort with the most severe AG pathology, a 91‐year old woman who died without any cognitive impairment (MMSE score = 30 less than 8 months prior to death). Scale bar = 50 µm. A subset of cases with aberrant TDP‐43 IHC To capitalize on strengths of the statistical data, a preliminary subset of cases (n = 49) was evaluated further using TDP‐43 IHC ( Table 7 , Figures 3 and 4 ) to test the hypothesis that TDP‐43 proteinopathy may help to account for the CILA in some cases. Patients with actual MMSE more than 10 points lower than predicted MMSE (using our statistical model) were included, as were patients with actual MMSE higher than the actual MMSE. Thus, this group included cases with the highest degree of “unaccounted‐for” CILA and the rest were chosen at random for this preliminary study. TDP‐43 IHC was performed on superior and middle temporal gyri as well as hippocampal formation to encompass areas where TDP proteinopathy has been described (3) . IHC‐positive lesions (abTDP‐43) were scored blindly and included either aberrant TDP‐43(+) NFT‐like structures with neuropil thread‐like profiles ( Figure 4 ) or TDP‐43(+) inclusions in dentate granule cells ( Figure 4C ). As shown in Figure 4 , TDP‐43 immunoreactivity could be a challenge to interpret, because the large majority of immunoreactive cells are probably not associated with pathology per se. Further, some staining irregularities may have only reflected the impact of antigen retrieval on tissue as “regularly irregular” staining. As a group, cases with abTDP‐43 showed a tendency to have a lower actual MMSE; all other things being equal (intercepts differ by linear regression at P = 0.04). These data indicate that there is an association between the presence of TDP‐43 pathology and the severity of CILA independent of other known clinical or pathological variables. These analyses represented a pilot study that we will follow up on in a separate report. 7 A subset of cases evaluated for TDP‐43 immunohistochemistry (IHC): demographics and mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) data (actual and predicted results). Demographics and MMSE data from the 49 cases stained for TDP‐43 IHC. Cases with abTPD‐43 had aberrant IHC in dentate granule cells (n = 18) and/or neurofibrillary tangle (NFT)‐like and neuropil thread‐like TDP‐43 immunoreactive profiles in the hippocampal formation (n = 21). Note that the actual MMSE score proximal to death was lower for the cases with abTDP‐43(+), and was even lower than the MMSE scores predicted using the regression algorithm that includes the known clinical and pathological risk factor for cognitive impairment linked to aging. n Formal education, years Braak stage Age at death, years Final MMSE score Final predicted MMSE score Cases with abTDP‐43 IHC 25 15.6 4.7 85.0 9.1 16.0 Cases without abTDP‐43 IHC 24 15.9 4.2 85.0 17.0 20.7 3 The multiple variable linear regression algorithm allows the prediction of mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) for each case based on the parameter coefficients . The algorithm was applied using the parameter coefficients from the combined cohort as shown in Table 5 for the 49 cases stained for TDP‐43 immunohistochemistry. The results indicate that the abTDP‐43(+) show a significant downward shift such that the “actual” MMSE score was lower than the abTDP‐43(−) cases (difference of intercept P = 0.04), indicating that abTDP‐43 is associated with cognitive impairment linked to aging severity independent of other known risk factors such as Alzheimer's disease or hippocampal sclerosis pathology. 4 Photomicrographs of TDP‐43 immunohistochemistry (IHC) show abnormal ( A,C ) and normal ( B,D ) staining, which can be a challenge to discriminate. Unlike most neurodegeneration‐related immunostains, TDP‐43 IHC is strongly positive in normal tissues. In some end‐stage AD cases we interpreted to be abTDP‐43(−) there were “regularly irregular” staining in the dentate granule cells ( B ) and hyperchromatic cells ( D , horizontal arrows) that were different from the normal‐appearing cells (vertical arrow) but distinct also from the aberrant intracellular inclusions ( C ). AbTDP‐43 staining in CA1 ( C ) showing the neurofibrillary tangle‐like (arrow) and neuritic thread‐like structures (arrows). Immunopositivity was scored blindly. Scale bars = 30 µm ( A,B ) or 50 µm ( C,D ). DISCUSSION Data from a large autopsy series were used to model the association of 43 clinical and pathological variables with CILA. Our results underscore the fundamental importance of AD pathology in our research cohorts, and indicate that many clinical variables (including but not limited to ApoE allele subtype) probably influence dementia if at all through altering “downstream” pathology such as NFTs. Most of the remainder of CILA in our cohort is associated with the presence of synucleinopathy, HS and cerebrovascular pathology. Our strategy provides insights into disease mechanisms that are associated with cognitive impairment in older persons. It has been known for some time that dementia is a syndrome with many contributing pathologies. However, there have been few studies that involve weighing the direct associations (rather than relative risks) of clinical and pathological variables with cognitive impairment in patients who died encompassing a full range of cognitive abilities. Three goals motivated our analytic strategy: (i) Provide a framework to know how adequately our current understanding of aging‐related brain diseases correlates with actual cognitive decline in humans. With the caveat that “correlation is not causation,” we highlight a handful of clinical and pathological variables that are associated with a significant majority of the variability of MMSE scores in our cohort. (ii) Highlight the specific clinical and pathological variables that are most directly associated with CILA in a quantitative, statistical framework. Specifically, AD‐related pathologies are by far most important (at least in this cohort), whereas some other variables such as AGs are not associated with CILA severity. (iii) Identify groups of individuals that are apparent “discrepancies” or outliers. These cases may either have unrecognized clinical contributors to CILA, or have pathological processes that are not currently recognized. In some patients with more CILA than predicted by other clinical and pathological indices, abTDP‐43 pathology is observed disproportionately. The clinical and pathological indices most strongly associated with CILA in the present study are compatible with prior research but also provide new insights. The data underscore the central importance of AD pathology in CILA, which has been described by a number of researchers previously (5–8, 10, 14–16, 20, 47, 48, 59, 63, 87, 89, 119) . Of the clinical and pathological variables, AD‐type pathology was associated with greater than two‐thirds of the variability in MMSE scores in our research group. In addition, the data had implications about the pathological contribution of the different subtypes and the neuroanatomical distribution of AD‐type pathology. Our statistical model supports an independent role for the contribution of NFTs in the isocortex (sampled six‐layered cerebral cortex comprising Brodmann areas 9, 17, 18, 21, 22, 39 and 40), NPs in the isocortex and NFTs in the mesial temporal lobe structures (CA1, subiculum and entorhinal cortex of the hippocampal formation, and amygdala). In persons with milder CILA, the predominant contribution to MMSE loss was NFTs in the mesial temporal lobe; however, in more demented patients, the isocortical NFTs contributed most profoundly. These findings are compatible with Braak staging (19, 20) . We (87) and others [for example, (6, 10, 85) ] have previously interpreted clinicopathological data to argue against the importance of amyloid plaques in contributing directly to cognitive impairment, focusing instead on isocortical NFTs. This interpretation derived from the relatively poor associations between cerebral cortical NP densities found at autopsy and the severity of ante–mortem cognitive impairment (6, 87) . However, the present study involves a larger cohort than our prior study and reflects a new approach that allows relatively unbiased modeling of the association of multiple variables with CILA. These data show an independent association between the density of isocortical NPs and CILA severity, although this only became statistically significant after both research cohorts were combined. The parameter coefficient for isocortical NP pathology is less than that for NFTs and these data should be interpreted with caution. However, the overall apparent contribution of isocortical NPs to CILA was statistically significant, accounting for approximately 20% of the total MMSE score variability in the sample. These data support the hypothesis that NPs themselves can contribute directly to cognitive impairment. Thus, isocortical NPs and medial temporal lobe NFTs—although their densities correlate poorly at death with ante–mortem cognitive impairment—nonetheless may contribute significantly to CILA. This is true especially at the earlier stages of the disease and in patients with mixed pathology. This theoretical point, which is harmonious with prior literature (56, 115) , is illustrated in Figure 5 . By contrast, our data suggest that ApoE alleles do not contribute directly to CILA. Rather, ApoE alleles probably act by differentially triggering the formation of NPs and NFTs in AD as suggested by Bennett et al (12) . The interactions between ApoE and plaque formation is underscored by the fact that when ApoE is included in the combined model, the statistically significant association between isocortical NPs and MMSE scores becomes non‐significant; no other parameters are likewise affected. It should also be noted that other clinical parameters may also impact AD via NPs and NFTs, but these indirect interactions or “upstream” modifiers would not be detected in the present study. 5 A theoretical model that harmonizes data from the present study and prior clinico‐pathological correlation research . Because isocortical neuritic plaques (NPs) and hippocampal neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) have significant “floor” and “ceiling” effects in relationship to the course of dementia, they are not strongly related to the cognitive impairment linked to aging (CILA) severity prior to autopsy (87) . However, they may still play a strong pathogenetic role and contribute directly to CILA particularly in the earliest stages of Alzheimer's disease. Multivariable correlation also allowed for a delineation of the associative impact of HS on CILA. In our sample, the presence of bilateral or of unilateral HS were associated, on average, with patients whose final MMSE scores were ∼8 points lower. These data are important for a number of reasons. First, we confirm the biological importance of HS as reported previously (27, 30, 38, 83, 91) , and we assessed quantitatively the associative impact of HS in cognitive impairment. Second, as HS is associated with TDP‐43 proteinopathy (2, 3, 28, 93) , TDP‐43 pathology is apparently an important contributor to CILA in human populations through multiple means. Previous studies have shown that TDP‐43 proteinopathy is also present in relatively rare subtypes of FTD (3, 4, 32, 77, 88, 105, 110, 127) . It has also been shown previously that a subset of AD patients have abTDP‐43, and cases with abTDP‐43 may have a faster disease course and more hippocampal atrophy (67) . A subset of patients in our study had a degree of cognitive impairment that is not “explained” by an association with any of the clinical or pathological variables, and abTDP‐43 is present in a significant proportion of these. This is the first formal demonstration of evidence for a role for TDP‐43 proteinopathy in CILA outside of the studies mentioned earlier. In contrast to AD‐type pathology and HS, some of the factors that have been associated with CILA in the past were not associated with lower MMSE scores in our cohort. For example, the presence of brain AGs was not associated with CILA. Prior studies have suggested that AGs may not associate strongly with CILA (68, 70) . In our cohort there was not even a weak association between the presence of AGs and CILA. This finding was not because of our exclusion of FTDs from the analyses (see Results section). It is possible that the presence of AGs represent a phase of pathological development such that in later stages of dementia the AGs regress. It is also possible that had we used anti‐Tau 4R antibodies we may have had greater sensitivity for AGs (116, 117, 126) , although in some cases with late‐stage AD pathology some AGs could be seen. Nonetheless, persons in our cohort did not show end‐stage dementia in the form of pure AG disease. We also confirm prior studies about the lack of impact on CILA by amygdala Lewy bodies (121) . Other factors that have been previously shown to be correlated to CILA were also found not to be in our cohort. These included the number of years of formal education; whether or not patients had undergone coronary artery bypass grafts; and dozens of other clinical and pathological variables (see Table 3 ). Because of the strong environmental risk factors, cerebrovascular‐related morbidity and mortality is a source for potential clinical and pathological differences between populations (87, 123, 124) . Although more than two‐thirds of individuals aged 80 years and above had CVD, our sample is probably biased against high‐infarct cases [cases of infarct‐associated aphasia (n = 3) were excluded from the statistical study]. Interestingly, in our sample microinfarcts did not constitute a parameter that associated with MMSE score variability, unlike other studies (109, 123) . This may be partly because other clinical and pathological variables linked to microinfarcts “washed out” the effect in our regression model. By contrast, and in agreement with prior studies (49, 104) , we did find a positive impact from lacunar infarcts in the model. These data highlight the need for a clinically relevant, precise, collectively agreed‐upon classification scheme for smaller infarcts. The presence on autopsy of large infarcts were associated with lower MMSE scores in the context of the pathology parameters alone, and history of transient ischemic attacks was linked to lower MMSE scores in the “initially normal” cohort. Our current model will be applied in the future to separate cohorts with even higher CVD incidence to enhance our understanding of clinicopathological correlation in CVD. Aside from the inherent difficulty with studying CVD, there were other limitations and potential confounds relevant to our study. The studied group represents a convenience sample rather than an epidemiological or population‐based cohort. These UK ADC research cohorts are biased toward aged, Caucasian and highly educated individuals ( Table 1 ). There also is a relative lack of movement disorders such as “pure” Parkinson's disease in our cohort. Although there are drawbacks to the demographic and pathological homogeneity, there are also benefits from the dampening of variability in our cohort. Athough perhaps less able to generalize conclusions to other populations, we are able to better establish relationships between the types of disease mechanisms and the severity of clinical deterioration seen in these individuals. In the present study, MMSE scores were used as the sole indicator of cognitive impairment severity. This is an imperfect metric for a number of reasons. Perhaps most importantly, a linear regression algorithm was applied although MMSE and other tests of cognition (and indeed, “cognition” itself) do not decline in a perfectly linear fashion during brain disease (29, 82, 84, 94) . This discrepancy would theoretically be reflected by artificially decreasing the degree to which accurate parameter coefficients could be established. Non‐linear regression models are available; however, they tend to have the disadvantage of providing less information about multivariable correlations. MMSE, like other cognitive assessments, also has “ceiling” and “floor” effects (39, 46, 61) . Other measures such as Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale—cognitive subscale assessments are more sensitive tools for detecting cognitive impairment (102) . Hence our analyses are not able to fully take into account subtle cognitive changes representative of “preclinical” cognitive impairment. The many patients in our cohort that achieved perfect MMSE scores of 30 prior to death may be misleading, given that our “predicted” MMSE (using our derived parameter estimates) was somewhat lower. Despite the imperfections of using MMSE scores, there are also compelling reasons to use this tool in the particular context of multivariable clinicopathological correlation. For a variety of reasons, MMSE scores (1, 8, 22, 28, 31, 33, 35, 37, 51, 60, 62, 64, 68, 71, 73, 84, 87, 98, 112, 114, 115, 122) , or the even less finely graded Clinical Dementia Rating scale (14, 15, 48, 57, 103, 107) , have been used as metrics of dementia severity in many informative clinicopathological correlation studies. MMSE scores offer good sensitivity in predicting disease (53, 69, 75) , with well‐known normative data. The test evaluates a number of cognitive domains, including orientation, language, attention/calculation, short‐term memory/recall and praxis, although not executive or psychiatric domains. MMSE data can be used for cross‐study comparisons and for biologically informative statistical rendering (25, 36, 43, 55, 106, 113, 115, 118) . Although MMSE scores are an imperfect metric of cognition (26, 81) , relatively small MMSE score changes (>2–4 points) have been found to be statistically reliable indicators of cognitive impairment (42, 58) even in many mild cognitive impairment cases (13, 34) . In the present study, the statistically significant parameter estimates tended to be greater than four MMSE score points. Some short‐term, memory‐intensive cognitive tests are less accurate in the detection of cognitive changes in late‐stage cases (102) . Moreover, other cognitive assessment tools have undergone repeated “fine‐tuning” over time, and—as such—cannot be used for longitudinal or retrospective comparisons over many years. In sum, MMSE scores were the best option in this context as a robust metric for multiple cognitive domains that we have documented in a consistent fashion between 1984 and 2008. Some aspects of the statistical modeling also deserve comment. We are studying variability in MMSE scores as a surrogate for CILA severity; however, we are using a cross‐sectional approach instead of comparing the trajectory of cognitive changes in individuals. It would be desirable to have a larger sample for the statistical modeling to assess with higher resolution the contribution of each parameter in association with MMSE score variability. The present approach is tailored to finding the relatively large, more direct contributing parameters that are associated with differences seen in final MMSE scores proximal to death. Inevitably there are both clinical and pathological variables left out of this study. For example, we do not measure synaptic density although others have found that synaptic density correlates well with CILA in AD brains (79, 80) . Instead, we concentrated on the clinical and pathological variables that were available in our dataset and were then considered mostly likely to relate to CILA. Ultimately, our statistical modeling approach is helpful only to the extent that it allows one to address clinically and biologically interesting questions. It is necessary to show that the clinicopathological correlations are not merely relevant to one population group. The present study is only the first step in developing parameter estimates for clinical and pathological variables that can be applied with greater confidence to more heterogeneous populations. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We are deeply grateful to all of the participants in our longitudinal aging study and to the patients with AD in our Alzheimer's Disease Center's research clinic. We thank Ann Tudor, Paula Thomason, Dr. Huaichen Liu and Sonya Anderson for the technical support, and Gregory Cooper, MD, PhD, Nancy Stiles, MD and Allison Caban‐Holt, PhD for the clinical evaluations. http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png Brain Pathology Wiley

Modeling the Association between 43 Different Clinical and Pathological Variables and the Severity of Cognitive Impairment in a Large Autopsy Cohort of Elderly Persons

Loading next page...
 
/lp/wiley/modeling-the-association-between-43-different-clinical-and-h0D01jn7ZH

References (132)

Publisher
Wiley
Copyright
© 2008 The Authors. Journal Compilation © 2008 International Society of Neuropathology
ISSN
1015-6305
eISSN
1750-3639
DOI
10.1111/j.1750-3639.2008.00244.x
pmid
19021630
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

INTRODUCTION Cognitive impairment linked to aging (CILA), a leading cause of morbidity and mortality, spans a continuum from subtle cognitive changes to end‐stage dementia. We used the term “CILA” to include cognitive impairment in an aging population regardless of diagnoses or of predicted eventual clinical course. Multiple etiologies contribute to CILA at a population level and coincident pathologies are frequently observed in aged individuals (11, 18, 87, 89, 109) . As prospects for specific brain disease therapies improve (45, 50) , it is increasingly important to know the relative weights of the distinct contributing factors in CILA. Rubrics are lacking to relate the severity of CILA to the presence and/or extent of most brain diseases. Alzheimer's disease (AD) has a relatively well‐documented association between the amount of neuropathology and the severity of ante–mortem cognitive impairment (6, 10, 14, 20, 48, 52, 85) , yet controversy remains about the contribution(s) of amyloid plaques and neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) to clinical deterioration (24, 76, 87) . Pathological processes also linked to CILA include cerebrovascular disease (CVD), dementia with Lewy bodies, argyrophilic grains (AGs), frontotemporal dementias (FTDs) and hippocampal sclerosis (HS) (9, 11, 14, 24, 66, 72, 74, 78, 86, 87, 91, 108, 109, 120) . In addition, there are clinical comorbidities that may contribute to CILA despite the absence of definite neuropathological hallmarks to support these associations (9, 74, 90) . Examples of clinical features that have been associated with worsened CILA include diabetes (40, 41, 54) , coronary artery bypass graft (17, 44, 97) , and “extreme old age” (63, 65, 125) . In contrast, there is an association with improved cognition, or decreased CILA, in persons with high levels of education and/or early linguistic ability (74, 82, 96, 108) . How can we weigh the importance of each disease mechanism in CILA? Despite great scientific progress in recent years, both normal and disease mechanisms cannot be modeled with certainty outside the aging human brain. Hence, careful clinicopathological correlations must be performed on relatively large human cohorts to tease out the importance of the factors involved in CILA. This is a formidable challenge. Precise correlation of ante–mortem cognitive measures, clinical variables and post‐mortem neuropathology requires a long‐term, prospective approach involving a multidisciplinary team. Patients must be recruited, tested longitudinally at regular intervals with thorough collection of demographic and clinical data, evaluated proximal to death, and then, upon autopsy, assessed systematically for neuropathology. A large number of patients must be studied to enable adequate statistical testing. Finally, there must be statistical tools to analyze how the ante–mortem parameters correlate to the neuropathology observed after autopsy, to produce data that are clinically and biologically relevant. The University of Kentucky Alzheimer's Disease Center (UK ADC) studies older persons with a full gamut of cognitive impairment (annual target n = 700 subjects). Along with thorough longitudinal mental status evaluations, many clinical and demographic data are obtained on every clinical visit. All subjects agreed to brain donation following death. To date, the autopsy rate on this cohort is over 90%. To evaluate the contributions of clinical, demographic and neuropathological factors to CILA, we utilized a broad strategy to assess quantitatively which factors are most directly associated with CILA in this large autopsy convenience cohort. Methodology included statistical regression modeling of 43 different clinical and pathological variables in relation to the severity of ante–mortem cognitive impairment. This approach provides the basis for some unforeseen conclusions. MATERIALS AND METHODS Subjects Patients who had come to autopsy from the UK ADC cohorts (total n = 501) were the basis for the study. Research protocols were approved by the University of Kentucky Institutional Review Board. Details of the UK ADC recruitment were described previously (78, 87) . Initially non‐demented subjects (“initially normal” cohort) comprised individuals (n = 212) from the UK Biologically Resilient Adults in Neurological Studies (BRAiNS) cohort (101) . The “dementia clinic” group (n = 122) derived from the patients followed longitudinally in the UK dementia clinic, who had presented with cognitive symptoms. Characteristics of these groups are shown in Table 1 . 1 Cohort demographics and numbers. Data from two different research clinics show distinct demographic profiles in the groups. Although some Alzheimer's disease‐type pathology was found in many cases from both clinics, other concomitant neuropathological features including infarcts and Lewy bodies (LBs) were also quite common. Abbreviations: ApoE = apolipoprotein E; MMSE = mini‐mental status examination. Research clinic n Sex (% F) Formal education, years ApoE allele 2/3/4% Age at death Final MMSE score Years since final MMSE % with infarcts (all types) % with cortical LBs Initially normal 212 58 16.0 8/77/15 85.9 26.3 0.82 55.2 8.0 (standard deviation) — — ±2.4 — ±7.7 ±5.4 ±0.6 — — Dementia clinic 122 61 12.9 3/64/33 80.8 10.4 1.55 35.2 26.2 (standard deviation) — — ±3.1 — ±7.0 ±7.4 ±1.2 — — Combined/total 334 59 14.8 6/71/23 84.0 20.4 1.09 48.0 14.7 Criteria for exclusion involved either inadequate clinical documentation (see further discussion) or the presence of pathology for which statistical power were lacking to model their impact on cognition ( Table 2 ); that is, low patient numbers for rare or idiosyncratic diseases. Thus, the following were excluded: primary or metastatic neoplasm in brain parenchyma including pituitary adenomas; >2 cm contusions in the brain parenchyma; hydrocephalus (except ex vacuo type); infectious agents identified in the brain parenchyma; and a history of left‐sided frontal and/or temporal/parietal infarct with clinically documented aphasia that would seriously confound the results of standard mental status evaluation. Only patients who had MMSE scores within 4 years of death were used in this project [as in (92) ]. Inadequate or non‐recent MMSE data (n = 120 cases) were the criteria that most frequently led to exclusion from this study. Our sample had insufficient patients with FTDs and/or tauopathies (corticobasal degeneration, Pick's disease, FTD‐motor neuron disease, progressive supranuclear palsy, other tauopathies and dementia lacking distinct histopathology; n = 17 total) to provide statistical power for a linear regression‐based study. 2 Exclusion criteria and the numbers excluded from each research clinic according to the reason for exclusion. From the total cases that came to autopsy at the University of Kentucky Alzheimer's Disease Center (n = 501), a total of 167 were excluded according to the criteria shown. By far the most frequent criterion for exclusion were undocumented mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) scores (n = 94) or cases for whom MMSE scores were documented >4 years prior to death (n = 17), which were most often seen in the high‐dementia clinic. Abbreviations: FTD = frontotemporal dementia. Reason for exclusion from study n (cohort) Total Initially normal Dementia clinic Chronic infection 2 0 2 Subdural/chronic hematoma 3 2 5 Contusions 1 4 5 Tumor (including adenomas) 9 1 10 Hydrocephalus (not ex vacuo ) 1 1 2 Infarcts + aphasia 2 1 3 Undocumented MMSE scores 1 93 94 >4 years since final MMSE score 2 27 29 FTDs, all subtypes 2 15 17 Total 23 144 167 Testing All initially normal individuals were contacted at 6‐month intervals, had detailed mental status testing, and we attempted to conduct neurological and physical examinations annually. For persons with clinical symptoms of dementia, we attempted to obtain mental status testing and neurological examinations annually. Mental status testing of our subjects has been described previously (78, 87) . The present study focuses on the MMSE conducted closest to the date of death as the “severity metric” for cognition. MMSE was chosen as it was the most consistently available measure obtained from both normal and demented subjects over the course of their evaluations (and see Discussion). Neuropathology Most aspects of pathological assessments were as described in detail previously (78, 87) . Briefly, a total of at least 25 sections were taken from each brain that included the middle frontal gyrus (MFG; area 9), superior and middle temporal gyri (SMT; areas 21 and 22), inferior parietal lobule (IPL; areas 39 and 40) and the occipital lobe including primary visual area (Occ; areas 17 and 18). Amyloid plaques were separated into plaques without neurites (DPs) and plaques with degenerating neurites (NPs) in each region as described previously (87) . An arithmetic mean was calculated from the count of the five most involved fields for DPs (number of DPs per 2.35 mm 2 ), NPs (number of NPs per 2.35 mm 2 ), and NFTs (number of NFTs per 0.586 mm 2 ) for each region. TDP‐43 immunohistochemistry (IHC) was performed on hippocampal and superior and middle temporal (SMT) gyri sections from a subset of cases ( n = 49). We hypothesized that aberrant TDP‐43 IHC (abTDP‐43) might be found disproportionately in patients whose final MMSE scores were lower than predicted by the statistical model because the pathology may contribute to their lower MMSE scores. We selected cases at random, except that we included 10 cases with the largest deviation from the mean such that (Predicted MMSE‐Actual MMSE) was greatest. Sections were cut on to Probe‐On slides at 5 micron thickness. Rabbit polyclonal anti‐TDP‐43 (1:500; ProteinTech, Chicago, IL, USA) was used following antigen retrieval (Trilogy pH 8.0, Cell Marque, Austin, TX, USA) in a Decloaking Chamber (Biocare Medical, Concord, CA, USA) and then formic acid pretreatment (3 minutes). Secondary antibody reaction employed the Vectastain ABC kit (Vector Labs, Burlingame, CA, USA). TDP‐43 IHC was evaluated by a neuropathologist blinded to the case identifications, and graded semiquantitively (see Analyses section and Supporting Information Table S1). Analyses The UK ADC database was built from in‐depth patient interviews, formal longitudinal cognitive and capacity assessments, pre‐ and post‐mortem chart reviews, extensive neuropathological investigation that includes periodic audits, and back‐maintenance on clinical and pathological data that extend from 1984. Unknown medical history items were assumed to indicate absence only after rigorous data extraction from clinical and research charts (otherwise, data were imputed as described in the following discussion). The earliest autopsy used in this series was performed in 1988. Forty‐three different clinical and pathological variables were abstracted, to encompass most of the prevalent factors that have shown associations with CILA ( Table 3 ). Variables were operationalized to determine the relative weight of many potential clinical and pathological contributors to CILA. 3 The 43 clinical and pathological variables used in the multiple regression algorithm to find which indices are associated with variability in mini‐mental status examination scores proximal to death. Items are operationalized as described in the Supporting Information. Most of the clinical variables are dichotomous (1/0), whereas most of the pathological variables were scored on a continuum from zero to one by graded increments. “Advanced coronary artery disease” is defined by having documented myocardial infarction, coronary artery bypass graft and/or coronary artery angioplasty. Abbreviations: ApoE = apolipoprotein E; AFib = atrial fibrillation. Clinical indices Pathological indices Age at death Sex Macroinfarct(s)—cortical Education Cerebral amyloid angiopathy ApoE alleles Arteriolosclerosis AFib/other cardiac arrhythmia * Number of microinfarcts Hypertension * Number of pale infarcts Body mass index Number of hemorrhagic infarcts Transient ischemic attack(s) * Number of lacunar infarcts Head trauma * Subcortical non‐lacunar infarcts Diabetes/takes insulin * Argyrophilic grains Seizures/epilepsy * Lewy bodies—isocortical Smoking * Lewy bodies—brainstem Peripheral vascular disease * Lewy bodies—medial temporal lobe Advanced coronary artery disease * Lewy bodies—amygdala only * Cancer * Isocortical neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) Anxiety * Isocortical neuritic plaques (NPs) Depression/antidepressant meds * Isocortical diffuse plaques (DPs) Number of documented drugs Mesial temporal lobe NFTs Opiates * Mesial temporal lobe NPs Antipsychotics * Mesial temporal lobe DPs Barbiturates * Hippocampal sclerosis (HS) unilateral * Benzodiazepenes * HS bilateral * * Dichotomous (1 or 0). Each variable was rendered along a severity continuum from zero to one in a manner hypothesized to be biologically and clinically relevant. “Isocortical” pathology refers to pathology in the MFG, IPL, SMT, and Occ cortices as described earlier. Medial temporal pathology is from amygdala plus CA1, subiculum and entorhinal cortex of the hippocampal formation. The criteria and methods for each variable operationalization are outlined in Supporting Information Table S1. Briefly, some of the variables were dichotomous (0 or 1; for example, history of seizures) whereas for others the severity continua was textured. For example, operationalization of AGs was graded from 0 to 1 proportional to a scoring system wherein AGs were scored semiquantitatively (0–3) from three areas: entorhinal cortex; subiculum; and cornu ammonis fields. AGs have been posited to develop first in the ambient gyrus (99, 100) ; however, sampling the ambient gyrus dependably (particularly in cases of end‐stage dementia) is a challenge, and the immediately adjacent entorhinal cortex is a reliable area for detecting early‐stage AGs (21) . We treated apolipoprotein E (ApoE) genotype as ApoE 2/2 (reference, 0 point), ApoE 2/3 (0.25 point), ApoE 2/4 or 3/3 (0.5 point), ApoE 3/4 (0.75 point) or ApoE 4/4 (1 point). A subset of 54 cases (14.9% of the cohort overall) had a documentation of demographic information, and neuropathology was performed, but medical records were not available. Missing items for these cases were multiply imputed using the logistic regression method in SAS 9.1.3 ® PROC MI (Chicago, IL, USA). Covariates were used to predict the missing items include age at death, sex, number of infarcts and number of prescription drugs taken. Grading of arteriolosclerosis severity at autopsy was missing in 28% of cases. These missing data were multiply imputed using the logistic regression method in PROC MI. Covariates were used to predict the missing items include age at death, total number of macro infarcts and total number of micro infarcts from the other cases where records were complete. Four additional items were also multiply imputed: mesial temporal cortex Lewy bodies (0.3%); amygdala‐only Lewy bodies (2.2%); ApoE (4.96%); and body mass index (27.0%). These items were multiply imputed using the MCMC method in PROC MI. The fully imputed data were analyzed using PROC REG, and the results were combined using PROC MIANALYZE. Imputation and modeling were performed with SAS 9.1.3 ® . For additional information about SAS and also about the results of model‐checking procedures, including delta–betas and backward selection, please see Supporting Information Table S2. Stratified analyses were conducted based on initial group membership (“initially normal” or “dementia clinic” cohorts). Linear models were fitted with adjusted MMSE as the dependent variable, and all the demographic, clinical and pathological variables as predictors to find the negative regressors ( Table 4 ). Stepwise model selection was used to determine the subset of regressors that best predicts the final MMSE at the 0.10 level of significance. Because multiple imputation methods for binary data may produce biased estimates, models for the “dementia clinic” group were fit both excluding the 54 cases where all clinical data were missing and including the imputed data. Because the results did not appreciably differ, the fully imputed data were used in the final analyses that are presented. 4 Results of multiple variable linear regression analyses using all clinical and pathological variables for each research cohort separately. The research cohorts were analyzed separately first. Note that the linear regression found large differences between the cohorts in the y ‐intercept, and in each case the isocortical NFTs was the largest parameter estimate. Other regressors differed, which may indicate biological differences in the parameters that affect relatively low‐CILA and high‐CILA individuals. Regressors with positive parameter estimates were not listed on this table. There are no such regressors in the “initially normal” group but ApoE and presence of diabetes were positive parameter estimates in the “dementia clinic” group. Abbreviations: ApoE = apolipoprotein E; CILA = cognitive impairment linked to aging; HS = hippocampal sclerosis; LB = Lewy body; NFT = neurofibrillary tangle; TIA = transient ischemic attack. Parameter Parameter estimate Standard error Pr > |t| “Initially normal” group n = 212 Intercept 29.49 0.31 <0.0001 NFTs, isocortex −24.93 4.30 <0.0001 NFTs, mesial temporal −16.10 2.09 <0.0001 HS—unilateral −12.53 1.48 <0.0001 HS—bilateral −5.50 1.65 0.001 LBs, mesial temporal −5.61 1.56 0.0004 LBs, amygdala only −3.73 1.51 0.0146 History of TIA −1.55 0.72 0.0334 “Dementia clinic” group, n = 122 Intercept 10.47 2.05 <0.0001 NFTs, isocortex −16.42 2.42 <0.0001 LBs, neocortex −5.07 2.10 0.0177 Antipsychotics −4.61 2.01 0.0237 Seizures −4.31 1.91 0.0257 Cancer −2.84 1.40 0.0447 To assess the effect of the clinical predictors on final MMSE scores without the neuropathological predictors, and vice versa, stepwise models using only clinical or pathological variable predictors were fit using the fully imputed, combined data ( Table 5 ). Lastly, in addition to the stratified analyses, a model was fit combining both groups using the pool of significant negative regressors ( Table 5 ). Severity of isocortical NPs and lacunar infarcts were included because they were significantly associated with CILA in the pathology‐only regression. ApoE alleles were included in the regression because they were significantly associated with CILA in the clinical‐only regression. 5 Regression modeling of parameters from entire research cohort (n = 334)—results of multiple variable linear regression analyses using entire cohort. Some of the included parameters are shown only to demonstrate their lack of independent association, but were also present in prior analyses. Note, in the combined model, ApoE parameter estimates are opposite in the different cohorts (positive in “dementia clinic”, negative in “initially normal”). For the overall group, ApoE did not have a significant effect. If the different cohort‐specific effects were taken into account, then, NPs, isocortex, would lose statistical significance but all the others are roughly the same. This is discussed in the Supporting Information. Abbreviations: ApoE = apolipoprotein E; HS = hippocampal sclerosis; LB = Lewy body; NFT = neurofibrillary tangle; NP = neuritic plaques. Parameter Parameter Estimate Standard error Pr > |t| Clinical parameters only Intercept 10.86 2.06 <0.0001 ApoE alleles −8.73 2.45 0.0004 Pathology parameters Intercept 28.50 0.47 <0.0001 NFTs, isocortex −21.78 2.30 <0.0001 NFTs, mesial temporal −14.48 1.87 <0.0001 LBs, isocortex −13.95 1.67 <0.0001 NPs, isocortex −8.81 1.87 <0.0001 HS—bilateral −7.81 2.10 0.0244 HS—unilateral −6.33 2.82 0.0256 Macroinfarcts −6.09 2.83 0.0323 Lacunar infarcts −4.81 2.51 0.0559 All significant parameters combined Intercept 24.32 1.07 <0.0001 NFTs, isocortex −19.66 2.27 <0.0001 LBs, isocortex −12.47 1.67 <0.0001 HS—unilateral −8.58 1.82 <0.0001 NFTs, mesial temporal −12.44 1.87 <0.0001 HS—bilateral −7.84 2.04 0.0001 NPs, isocortex −6.61 2.71 0.0152 Lacunar infarcts −5.15 2.46 0.037 Age >0.1 LBs, amygdala only >0.1 Sex >0.1 The average number of years between the final MMSE test and death for all cases was 1.09 years and the median was 0.82 years. Thirteen cases in the dataset had intervals between 3.5 and 4 years, and these were predominantly end‐stage dementia patients. As patients' MMSE scores would have declined in the interim presumably (23, 81, 82, 95, 111) , we also performed the linear regression model using an MMSE correction. In this model, for patients with final MMSE scores less than 28, 2.5 points were deducted from the MMSE score for every 12‐month duration between the final MMSE evaluation and death. For the average patient in this study, this involved a “correction” of less than 2.5 MMSE points (median 1.1; range 0–12.1). In this model, the findings were broadly similar to the data reported without the MMSE correction (data not shown). Following the derivation of parameter estimates, we used the regression equation and the covariates to predict the MMSE score. These were arrayed on a Microsoft Excel spreadsheet. Using this database we modeled the contribution of the different clinical and pathological variables to CILA over our entire cohort (n = 334). This also allowed for comparison between the actual final MMSE scores vs. “predicted” MMSE scores based on the parameter estimates. RESULTS Based on the inclusion criteria described earlier, 334 patients from the UK ADC autopsy series served as the research cohort for a detailed analysis of the clinical and pathological parameters that underlie variations in ante–mortem MMSE scores. As shown in the demographics in Table 1 , this cohort comprises predominantly highly educated elderly persons. All cases except one were Caucasian. Over half of the patients had some form of cerebrovascular pathology; of patients beyond 80 years of age, more than 66% had some cerebrovascular pathology. Many of the patients died with minimal CILA, or none, as detected by the MMSE scores. In all, 11% of the cohort had final MMSE scores of 29 or 30. In the first level of analyses, the total cohort was stratified by the group in which the subjects had initially enrolled. The demographics of these two groups differed in some aspects from each other ( Table 1 ). Stratified statistical analyses identified the determinants of MMSE variability with R 2 correlation coefficient of >0.70. In addition to running the multiple linear regression with the parameters mentioned earlier, additional analyses were ran using only the clinical and demographic variables (n = 22), or only the pathological variables (n = 21). When only clinical variables were queried, the impact of ApoE ( P = 0.0004) was manifest. Another model was fit using the data combined from both groups. All the parameters were included from the stratified models, and this final model serves as the source of the results described in the following discussion. Clinical conditions, neuropathology and CILA Parameter estimates provide a numerically weighted indicator of how each clinical and pathological variable correlates to interindividual differences in MMSE scores in our sample. The extent of interpersonal differences in MMSE scores can be predicted referent to each parameter and for every patient. For example, all other variables being equal, the presence of HS is associated with eight fewer MMSE score points. Evaluating parameter estimates and frequencies reveals that more than two‐thirds of the MMSE variability in our study sample was associated with the amount of Alzheimer‐type pathology, NFTs and NPs. This held true whether in the context of “pure” or mixed‐type pathology (for example, with cortical Lewy bodies or cerebrovascular pathology). The AD‐related pathologies associated with CILA were isocortical and mesial temporal NFTs and isocortical NPs [the latter only statistically significant when the whole cohort (n = 334) was analyzed]. The contribution to CILA from AD‐type pathology appeared to differ based on disease severity. In patients with higher MMSE scores, most of the CILA was associated with mesial temporal NFTs and isocortical NPs, whereas with more severe CILA, the isocortical NFTs accounted for a much larger proportion of the cognitive impairment ( Figure 1 ). 1 The association of particular subtypes and anatomical areas of Alzheimer's disease (AD) pathology with cognitive impairment linked to aging (CILA) severity changes according to the different stages of cognitive impairment . This chart shows data from modeling on the entire cohort using the statistically significant parameter coefficients as shown in Table 5 . Cases are binned by mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) scores: MMSE 26–28 (n = 83); MMSE 20–26 (n = 38); MMSE 10–20 (n = 65); and MMSE 0–10 (n = 62). Note that as cognition declines, the apparent impact of isocortical neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) becomes greater, and the relative importance of mesial temporal lobe NFTs and isocortical NPs declines. Nonetheless, each subtype of AD‐type pathology is associated independently with significant amount of CILA. Isocortical LBs tended to be associated with lower MMSE scores ( Table 5 ). However, even advanced LB pathology alone tended not to be associated with end‐stage dementia. The presence of isocortical‐pattern dementia with Lewy bodies sometimes coincided with AD pathology and these pathological processes appear to have an additive clinical effect ( Table 6 ). On average, isocortical LBs correlated with lower MMSE scores by approximately 10 points. These results bear out the regression modeling where the parameter estimate for isocortical LBs in the most severe cases (maximally severe involvement in all four isocortical regions tested) correlate with an approximately 13‐point loss in MMSE score ( Table 5 ). 6 Final mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) scores proximal to death in patients with extensive isocortical Lewy bodies (LBs) or no isocortical LBs, stratified by Braak stages. Moderate‐to‐severe alpha‐synuclein pathology in neocortex is an additive pathology in the University of Kentucky Alzheimer's Disease Center research cohorts. In these patients (“extensive isocortical LBs”); the frequency of concomitant Alzheimer's disease (AD) was approximately the same as in cases without isocortical LBs. Isocortical LB and AD pathologies appear additive with respect to clinical symptoms. Stage for stage, the presence of isocortical LBs appears to correlate with a decrease of approximately 10 points in MMSE scores, consistent with the statistically derived parameter estimates ( Tables 4 and 5 ). The few cases with “pure” isocortical LBs (minimal Alzheimer‐type pathology or infarcts) did not have end‐stage dementia. Braak stages—average MMSE (n) 0–II III, IV V, VI Cases with extensive isocortical LBs, n = 28 17.1 (10) 11.8 (4) 7.1 (14) Cases without isocortical LBs, n = 281 27.6 (106) 25.3 (65) 13.3 (110) The majority of the 43 clinical and pathological variables showed no statistically significant contribution to CILA in our cohort. The stepwise linear regression algorithm flags those variables most directly correlating to changes in MMSE scores. It is presumed that those variables that contribute to cognitive impairment through stimulating other pathological processes would not distinguish themselves as long as the “downstream” factor was also evaluated. A presumptive example of this phenomenon is ApoE. ApoE alleles were not associated with significant independent contribution to MMSE variability unless the pathological variables were not present also as regressors. AGs by pathology were not associated statistically with the severity of CILA in our sample. This was not an artifact of having FTD cases excluded from analyses, because only three FTD cases had minimal AGs, and none of these cases had sufficient amounts to merit the diagnosis of argyrophilic grain disease. Seventy‐five out of 334 (22%) cases had some AGs as assessed using Gallyas silver impregnation. P ‐values for the association of AGs pathology to CILA severity were always >0.5. The operationalization technique allowed a graded score of AGs reflecting the distribution and severity (see Analysis section). Of the seven cases with the highest score of AGs, four had final MMSE scores of 29 or higher, and none had MMSE scores less than 24. These additional analyses ( Figure 2 ) again provide some validation for the statistical modeling data. 2 Statistical methods indicated that argyrophilic grains (AGs) are not associated with CILA severity in our research cohorts . To evaluate these data, we studied separately the 75 cases with AGs in our sample. A chart ( A ) shows how the actual pre‐mortem mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) scores for patients correlated to the amount of AG pathology. The AG score is proportional to the severity of AGs in entorhinal cortex, subiculum and cornu ammonis hippocampal fields. Note that there is no decrease in MMSE scores as AG pathology worsens. A photomicrograph of Gallyas‐stained cornu ammonis ( B ) from the individual in our autopsy cohort with the most severe AG pathology, a 91‐year old woman who died without any cognitive impairment (MMSE score = 30 less than 8 months prior to death). Scale bar = 50 µm. A subset of cases with aberrant TDP‐43 IHC To capitalize on strengths of the statistical data, a preliminary subset of cases (n = 49) was evaluated further using TDP‐43 IHC ( Table 7 , Figures 3 and 4 ) to test the hypothesis that TDP‐43 proteinopathy may help to account for the CILA in some cases. Patients with actual MMSE more than 10 points lower than predicted MMSE (using our statistical model) were included, as were patients with actual MMSE higher than the actual MMSE. Thus, this group included cases with the highest degree of “unaccounted‐for” CILA and the rest were chosen at random for this preliminary study. TDP‐43 IHC was performed on superior and middle temporal gyri as well as hippocampal formation to encompass areas where TDP proteinopathy has been described (3) . IHC‐positive lesions (abTDP‐43) were scored blindly and included either aberrant TDP‐43(+) NFT‐like structures with neuropil thread‐like profiles ( Figure 4 ) or TDP‐43(+) inclusions in dentate granule cells ( Figure 4C ). As shown in Figure 4 , TDP‐43 immunoreactivity could be a challenge to interpret, because the large majority of immunoreactive cells are probably not associated with pathology per se. Further, some staining irregularities may have only reflected the impact of antigen retrieval on tissue as “regularly irregular” staining. As a group, cases with abTDP‐43 showed a tendency to have a lower actual MMSE; all other things being equal (intercepts differ by linear regression at P = 0.04). These data indicate that there is an association between the presence of TDP‐43 pathology and the severity of CILA independent of other known clinical or pathological variables. These analyses represented a pilot study that we will follow up on in a separate report. 7 A subset of cases evaluated for TDP‐43 immunohistochemistry (IHC): demographics and mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) data (actual and predicted results). Demographics and MMSE data from the 49 cases stained for TDP‐43 IHC. Cases with abTPD‐43 had aberrant IHC in dentate granule cells (n = 18) and/or neurofibrillary tangle (NFT)‐like and neuropil thread‐like TDP‐43 immunoreactive profiles in the hippocampal formation (n = 21). Note that the actual MMSE score proximal to death was lower for the cases with abTDP‐43(+), and was even lower than the MMSE scores predicted using the regression algorithm that includes the known clinical and pathological risk factor for cognitive impairment linked to aging. n Formal education, years Braak stage Age at death, years Final MMSE score Final predicted MMSE score Cases with abTDP‐43 IHC 25 15.6 4.7 85.0 9.1 16.0 Cases without abTDP‐43 IHC 24 15.9 4.2 85.0 17.0 20.7 3 The multiple variable linear regression algorithm allows the prediction of mini‐mental status examination (MMSE) for each case based on the parameter coefficients . The algorithm was applied using the parameter coefficients from the combined cohort as shown in Table 5 for the 49 cases stained for TDP‐43 immunohistochemistry. The results indicate that the abTDP‐43(+) show a significant downward shift such that the “actual” MMSE score was lower than the abTDP‐43(−) cases (difference of intercept P = 0.04), indicating that abTDP‐43 is associated with cognitive impairment linked to aging severity independent of other known risk factors such as Alzheimer's disease or hippocampal sclerosis pathology. 4 Photomicrographs of TDP‐43 immunohistochemistry (IHC) show abnormal ( A,C ) and normal ( B,D ) staining, which can be a challenge to discriminate. Unlike most neurodegeneration‐related immunostains, TDP‐43 IHC is strongly positive in normal tissues. In some end‐stage AD cases we interpreted to be abTDP‐43(−) there were “regularly irregular” staining in the dentate granule cells ( B ) and hyperchromatic cells ( D , horizontal arrows) that were different from the normal‐appearing cells (vertical arrow) but distinct also from the aberrant intracellular inclusions ( C ). AbTDP‐43 staining in CA1 ( C ) showing the neurofibrillary tangle‐like (arrow) and neuritic thread‐like structures (arrows). Immunopositivity was scored blindly. Scale bars = 30 µm ( A,B ) or 50 µm ( C,D ). DISCUSSION Data from a large autopsy series were used to model the association of 43 clinical and pathological variables with CILA. Our results underscore the fundamental importance of AD pathology in our research cohorts, and indicate that many clinical variables (including but not limited to ApoE allele subtype) probably influence dementia if at all through altering “downstream” pathology such as NFTs. Most of the remainder of CILA in our cohort is associated with the presence of synucleinopathy, HS and cerebrovascular pathology. Our strategy provides insights into disease mechanisms that are associated with cognitive impairment in older persons. It has been known for some time that dementia is a syndrome with many contributing pathologies. However, there have been few studies that involve weighing the direct associations (rather than relative risks) of clinical and pathological variables with cognitive impairment in patients who died encompassing a full range of cognitive abilities. Three goals motivated our analytic strategy: (i) Provide a framework to know how adequately our current understanding of aging‐related brain diseases correlates with actual cognitive decline in humans. With the caveat that “correlation is not causation,” we highlight a handful of clinical and pathological variables that are associated with a significant majority of the variability of MMSE scores in our cohort. (ii) Highlight the specific clinical and pathological variables that are most directly associated with CILA in a quantitative, statistical framework. Specifically, AD‐related pathologies are by far most important (at least in this cohort), whereas some other variables such as AGs are not associated with CILA severity. (iii) Identify groups of individuals that are apparent “discrepancies” or outliers. These cases may either have unrecognized clinical contributors to CILA, or have pathological processes that are not currently recognized. In some patients with more CILA than predicted by other clinical and pathological indices, abTDP‐43 pathology is observed disproportionately. The clinical and pathological indices most strongly associated with CILA in the present study are compatible with prior research but also provide new insights. The data underscore the central importance of AD pathology in CILA, which has been described by a number of researchers previously (5–8, 10, 14–16, 20, 47, 48, 59, 63, 87, 89, 119) . Of the clinical and pathological variables, AD‐type pathology was associated with greater than two‐thirds of the variability in MMSE scores in our research group. In addition, the data had implications about the pathological contribution of the different subtypes and the neuroanatomical distribution of AD‐type pathology. Our statistical model supports an independent role for the contribution of NFTs in the isocortex (sampled six‐layered cerebral cortex comprising Brodmann areas 9, 17, 18, 21, 22, 39 and 40), NPs in the isocortex and NFTs in the mesial temporal lobe structures (CA1, subiculum and entorhinal cortex of the hippocampal formation, and amygdala). In persons with milder CILA, the predominant contribution to MMSE loss was NFTs in the mesial temporal lobe; however, in more demented patients, the isocortical NFTs contributed most profoundly. These findings are compatible with Braak staging (19, 20) . We (87) and others [for example, (6, 10, 85) ] have previously interpreted clinicopathological data to argue against the importance of amyloid plaques in contributing directly to cognitive impairment, focusing instead on isocortical NFTs. This interpretation derived from the relatively poor associations between cerebral cortical NP densities found at autopsy and the severity of ante–mortem cognitive impairment (6, 87) . However, the present study involves a larger cohort than our prior study and reflects a new approach that allows relatively unbiased modeling of the association of multiple variables with CILA. These data show an independent association between the density of isocortical NPs and CILA severity, although this only became statistically significant after both research cohorts were combined. The parameter coefficient for isocortical NP pathology is less than that for NFTs and these data should be interpreted with caution. However, the overall apparent contribution of isocortical NPs to CILA was statistically significant, accounting for approximately 20% of the total MMSE score variability in the sample. These data support the hypothesis that NPs themselves can contribute directly to cognitive impairment. Thus, isocortical NPs and medial temporal lobe NFTs—although their densities correlate poorly at death with ante–mortem cognitive impairment—nonetheless may contribute significantly to CILA. This is true especially at the earlier stages of the disease and in patients with mixed pathology. This theoretical point, which is harmonious with prior literature (56, 115) , is illustrated in Figure 5 . By contrast, our data suggest that ApoE alleles do not contribute directly to CILA. Rather, ApoE alleles probably act by differentially triggering the formation of NPs and NFTs in AD as suggested by Bennett et al (12) . The interactions between ApoE and plaque formation is underscored by the fact that when ApoE is included in the combined model, the statistically significant association between isocortical NPs and MMSE scores becomes non‐significant; no other parameters are likewise affected. It should also be noted that other clinical parameters may also impact AD via NPs and NFTs, but these indirect interactions or “upstream” modifiers would not be detected in the present study. 5 A theoretical model that harmonizes data from the present study and prior clinico‐pathological correlation research . Because isocortical neuritic plaques (NPs) and hippocampal neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) have significant “floor” and “ceiling” effects in relationship to the course of dementia, they are not strongly related to the cognitive impairment linked to aging (CILA) severity prior to autopsy (87) . However, they may still play a strong pathogenetic role and contribute directly to CILA particularly in the earliest stages of Alzheimer's disease. Multivariable correlation also allowed for a delineation of the associative impact of HS on CILA. In our sample, the presence of bilateral or of unilateral HS were associated, on average, with patients whose final MMSE scores were ∼8 points lower. These data are important for a number of reasons. First, we confirm the biological importance of HS as reported previously (27, 30, 38, 83, 91) , and we assessed quantitatively the associative impact of HS in cognitive impairment. Second, as HS is associated with TDP‐43 proteinopathy (2, 3, 28, 93) , TDP‐43 pathology is apparently an important contributor to CILA in human populations through multiple means. Previous studies have shown that TDP‐43 proteinopathy is also present in relatively rare subtypes of FTD (3, 4, 32, 77, 88, 105, 110, 127) . It has also been shown previously that a subset of AD patients have abTDP‐43, and cases with abTDP‐43 may have a faster disease course and more hippocampal atrophy (67) . A subset of patients in our study had a degree of cognitive impairment that is not “explained” by an association with any of the clinical or pathological variables, and abTDP‐43 is present in a significant proportion of these. This is the first formal demonstration of evidence for a role for TDP‐43 proteinopathy in CILA outside of the studies mentioned earlier. In contrast to AD‐type pathology and HS, some of the factors that have been associated with CILA in the past were not associated with lower MMSE scores in our cohort. For example, the presence of brain AGs was not associated with CILA. Prior studies have suggested that AGs may not associate strongly with CILA (68, 70) . In our cohort there was not even a weak association between the presence of AGs and CILA. This finding was not because of our exclusion of FTDs from the analyses (see Results section). It is possible that the presence of AGs represent a phase of pathological development such that in later stages of dementia the AGs regress. It is also possible that had we used anti‐Tau 4R antibodies we may have had greater sensitivity for AGs (116, 117, 126) , although in some cases with late‐stage AD pathology some AGs could be seen. Nonetheless, persons in our cohort did not show end‐stage dementia in the form of pure AG disease. We also confirm prior studies about the lack of impact on CILA by amygdala Lewy bodies (121) . Other factors that have been previously shown to be correlated to CILA were also found not to be in our cohort. These included the number of years of formal education; whether or not patients had undergone coronary artery bypass grafts; and dozens of other clinical and pathological variables (see Table 3 ). Because of the strong environmental risk factors, cerebrovascular‐related morbidity and mortality is a source for potential clinical and pathological differences between populations (87, 123, 124) . Although more than two‐thirds of individuals aged 80 years and above had CVD, our sample is probably biased against high‐infarct cases [cases of infarct‐associated aphasia (n = 3) were excluded from the statistical study]. Interestingly, in our sample microinfarcts did not constitute a parameter that associated with MMSE score variability, unlike other studies (109, 123) . This may be partly because other clinical and pathological variables linked to microinfarcts “washed out” the effect in our regression model. By contrast, and in agreement with prior studies (49, 104) , we did find a positive impact from lacunar infarcts in the model. These data highlight the need for a clinically relevant, precise, collectively agreed‐upon classification scheme for smaller infarcts. The presence on autopsy of large infarcts were associated with lower MMSE scores in the context of the pathology parameters alone, and history of transient ischemic attacks was linked to lower MMSE scores in the “initially normal” cohort. Our current model will be applied in the future to separate cohorts with even higher CVD incidence to enhance our understanding of clinicopathological correlation in CVD. Aside from the inherent difficulty with studying CVD, there were other limitations and potential confounds relevant to our study. The studied group represents a convenience sample rather than an epidemiological or population‐based cohort. These UK ADC research cohorts are biased toward aged, Caucasian and highly educated individuals ( Table 1 ). There also is a relative lack of movement disorders such as “pure” Parkinson's disease in our cohort. Although there are drawbacks to the demographic and pathological homogeneity, there are also benefits from the dampening of variability in our cohort. Athough perhaps less able to generalize conclusions to other populations, we are able to better establish relationships between the types of disease mechanisms and the severity of clinical deterioration seen in these individuals. In the present study, MMSE scores were used as the sole indicator of cognitive impairment severity. This is an imperfect metric for a number of reasons. Perhaps most importantly, a linear regression algorithm was applied although MMSE and other tests of cognition (and indeed, “cognition” itself) do not decline in a perfectly linear fashion during brain disease (29, 82, 84, 94) . This discrepancy would theoretically be reflected by artificially decreasing the degree to which accurate parameter coefficients could be established. Non‐linear regression models are available; however, they tend to have the disadvantage of providing less information about multivariable correlations. MMSE, like other cognitive assessments, also has “ceiling” and “floor” effects (39, 46, 61) . Other measures such as Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale—cognitive subscale assessments are more sensitive tools for detecting cognitive impairment (102) . Hence our analyses are not able to fully take into account subtle cognitive changes representative of “preclinical” cognitive impairment. The many patients in our cohort that achieved perfect MMSE scores of 30 prior to death may be misleading, given that our “predicted” MMSE (using our derived parameter estimates) was somewhat lower. Despite the imperfections of using MMSE scores, there are also compelling reasons to use this tool in the particular context of multivariable clinicopathological correlation. For a variety of reasons, MMSE scores (1, 8, 22, 28, 31, 33, 35, 37, 51, 60, 62, 64, 68, 71, 73, 84, 87, 98, 112, 114, 115, 122) , or the even less finely graded Clinical Dementia Rating scale (14, 15, 48, 57, 103, 107) , have been used as metrics of dementia severity in many informative clinicopathological correlation studies. MMSE scores offer good sensitivity in predicting disease (53, 69, 75) , with well‐known normative data. The test evaluates a number of cognitive domains, including orientation, language, attention/calculation, short‐term memory/recall and praxis, although not executive or psychiatric domains. MMSE data can be used for cross‐study comparisons and for biologically informative statistical rendering (25, 36, 43, 55, 106, 113, 115, 118) . Although MMSE scores are an imperfect metric of cognition (26, 81) , relatively small MMSE score changes (>2–4 points) have been found to be statistically reliable indicators of cognitive impairment (42, 58) even in many mild cognitive impairment cases (13, 34) . In the present study, the statistically significant parameter estimates tended to be greater than four MMSE score points. Some short‐term, memory‐intensive cognitive tests are less accurate in the detection of cognitive changes in late‐stage cases (102) . Moreover, other cognitive assessment tools have undergone repeated “fine‐tuning” over time, and—as such—cannot be used for longitudinal or retrospective comparisons over many years. In sum, MMSE scores were the best option in this context as a robust metric for multiple cognitive domains that we have documented in a consistent fashion between 1984 and 2008. Some aspects of the statistical modeling also deserve comment. We are studying variability in MMSE scores as a surrogate for CILA severity; however, we are using a cross‐sectional approach instead of comparing the trajectory of cognitive changes in individuals. It would be desirable to have a larger sample for the statistical modeling to assess with higher resolution the contribution of each parameter in association with MMSE score variability. The present approach is tailored to finding the relatively large, more direct contributing parameters that are associated with differences seen in final MMSE scores proximal to death. Inevitably there are both clinical and pathological variables left out of this study. For example, we do not measure synaptic density although others have found that synaptic density correlates well with CILA in AD brains (79, 80) . Instead, we concentrated on the clinical and pathological variables that were available in our dataset and were then considered mostly likely to relate to CILA. Ultimately, our statistical modeling approach is helpful only to the extent that it allows one to address clinically and biologically interesting questions. It is necessary to show that the clinicopathological correlations are not merely relevant to one population group. The present study is only the first step in developing parameter estimates for clinical and pathological variables that can be applied with greater confidence to more heterogeneous populations. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We are deeply grateful to all of the participants in our longitudinal aging study and to the patients with AD in our Alzheimer's Disease Center's research clinic. We thank Ann Tudor, Paula Thomason, Dr. Huaichen Liu and Sonya Anderson for the technical support, and Gregory Cooper, MD, PhD, Nancy Stiles, MD and Allison Caban‐Holt, PhD for the clinical evaluations.

Journal

Brain PathologyWiley

Published: Jan 1, 2010

There are no references for this article.