Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
K. Szpunar, E. Schellenberg, P. Pliner, Glenn Schellenberg (2004)
Liking and memory for musical stimuli as a function of exposure.Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 30 2
(1974)
The new experimental aesthetics
E. Schellenberg, Ania Krysciak, R. Campbell (2000)
Perceiving emotion in melody: Interactive effects of pitch and rhythm.Music Perception, 18
Gregory Webster, Catherine Weir (2005)
Emotional Responses to Music: Interactive Effects of Mode, Texture, and TempoMotivation and Emotion, 29
R. Zajonc (1980)
Feeling and thinking : Preferences need no inferencesAmerican Psychologist, 35
L. Jacoby (1983)
Perceptual enhancement: persistent effects of an experience.Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 9 1
Mixed moods: Affective responding to music with conflicting cues
R. Bornstein, P. D’Agostino (1994)
The Attribution and Discounting of Perceptual Fluency: Preliminary Tests of a Perceptual Fluency/Attributional Model of the Mere Exposure EffectSocial Cognition, 12
A. Gabrielsson, Erik Lindström (2001)
The influence of musical structure on emotional expression.
Patrick Hunter, E. Schellenberg, U. Schimmack (2008)
Mixed affective responses to music with conflicting cuesCognition and Emotion, 22
O. Bergh, S. Vrana (1998)
Repetition and Boredom in a Perceptual Fluency/ Attributional Model of Affective JudgementsCognition & Emotion, 12
Marcia Johnson, Jung Kim, G. Risse (1985)
Do alcoholic Korsakoff's syndrome patients acquire affective reactions?Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 11 1
Alex Heingartner, Joan Hall (1974)
Affective consequences in adults and children of repeated exposure to auditory stimuli.Journal of personality and social psychology, 29 6
M. Rigg (1940)
Speed as a determiner of musical mood.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27
(1992)
Inhibitory effects of awareness on affective responding Emotion: Review of personality and social psychology
G. Bonanno, N. Stillings (1986)
Preference, familiarity, and recognition after repeated brief exposures to random geometric shapes.The American journal of psychology, 99 3
Gabriela Husain, W. Thompson, Glenn Schellenberg (2002)
Effects of Musical Tempo and Mode on Arousal, Mood, and Spatial AbilitiesMusic Perception, 20
R. Gundlach (1935)
Factors Determining the Characterization of Musical PhrasesAmerican Journal of Psychology, 47
Sheila Murphy, J. Monahan, R. Zajonc (1995)
Additivity of nonconscious affect: combined effects of priming and exposure.Journal of personality and social psychology, 69 4
S. Mithen, I. Morley, A. Wray, M. Tallerman, C. Gamble (2006)
The Singing Neanderthals: the Origins of Music, Language, Mind and Body, by Steven Mithen. London: Weidenfeld & Nicholson, 2005. ISBN 0-297-64317-7 hardback £20 & US$25.2; ix+374 pp.Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 16
M. Rigg (1937)
An experiment to determine how accurately college students can interpret the intended meanings of musical compositions.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 21
K. Scherer, James Oshinsky (1977)
Cue utilization in emotion attribution from auditory stimuliMotivation and Emotion, 1
J. Monahan, S. Murphy, R. Zajonc (2000)
Subliminal Mere Exposure: Specific, General, and Diffuse EffectsPsychological Science, 11
I. Peretz, D. Gaudreau, A. Bonnel (1998)
Exposure effects on music preference and recognitionMemory & Cognition, 26
B. Whittlesea, Lisa Williams (1998)
Why do strangers feel familiar, but friends don't? A discrepancy-attribution account of feelings of familiarity.Acta psychologica, 98 2-3
R. Reber, P. Winkielman, N. Schwarz (1998)
Effects of Perceptual Fluency on Affective JudgmentsPsychological Science, 9
W. Balch, Benjamin Lewis (1996)
MUSIC-DEPENDENT MEMORY : THE ROLES OF TEMPO CHANGE AND MOOD MEDIATIONJournal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22
(2009)
The Role of Music Preferences in Interpersonal Perception
K. Hevner (1937)
The affective value of pitch and tempo in music.American Journal of Psychology, 49
R. Reber, N. Schwarz, P. Winkielman (2004)
Processing Fluency and Aesthetic Pleasure: Is Beauty in the Perceiver's Processing Experience?Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8
George Gunkle, D. Berlyne (1975)
Aesthetics and Psychobiology, 27
PERETZ, VIEILLARD
C. Krumhansl (1997)
An exploratory study of musical emotions and psychophysiology.Canadian journal of experimental psychology = Revue canadienne de psychologie experimentale, 51 4
D. Stang (1974)
Methodological factors in mere exposure research.Psychological Bulletin, 81
J. Seamon, Pepper Williams, M. Crowley, Irene Kim, S. Langer, Peter Orne, Dana Wishengrad (1995)
The mere exposure effect is based on implicit memory: Effects of stimulus type, encoding conditions, and number of exposures on recognition and affect judgments.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21
J. Seamon, R. Marsh, N. Brody (1984)
Critical importance of exposure duration for affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized.Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 10 3
(1998)
immediacy, and isolation after brain
William Kunst‐Wilson, R. Zajonc (1980)
Affective discrimination of stimuli that cannot be recognized.Science, 207 4430
W. Thompson, E. Schellenberg, Gabriela Husain (2001)
Arousal, Mood, and The Mozart EffectPsychological Science, 12
Siu-Lan Tan, Matthew Spackman, Christy Peaslee (2006)
The Effects of Repeated Exposure on Liking and Judgments of Musical Unity of Intact and Patchwork CompositionsMusic Perception, 23
G. Mandler, Yoshio Nakamura, Billie Zandt (1987)
Nonspecific Effects of Exposure on Stimuli That Cannot Be RecognizedJournal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 13
A. Gregory, L. Worrall, A. Sarge (1996)
The development of emotional responses to music in young childrenMotivation and Emotion, 20
N. Gosselin, I. Peretz, M. Noulhiane, D. Hasboun, C. Beckett, M. Baulac, S. Samson (2005)
Impaired recognition of scary music following unilateral temporal lobe excision.Brain : a journal of neurology, 128 Pt 3
W. Wilson (1979)
Feeling more than we can know: Exposure effects without learning.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37
C. Heinlein (1928)
The affective characters of the major and minor modes in music.Journal of Comparative Psychology, 8
J. Cutting (2003)
Gustave Caillebotte, French Impressionism, and mere exposurePsychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10
J. Seamon, N. Brody, D. Kauff (1983)
Affective discrimination of stimuli that are not recognized: effects of shadowing, masking, and cerebral laterality.Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 9 3
Emery Schubert (1996)
Enjoyment of Negative Emotions in Music: An Associative Network ExplanationPsychology of Music, 24
Colleen Kelley, Matthew Rhodes (2002)
Making sense and nonsense of experience: Attributions in memory and judgmentPsychology of Learning and Motivation, 41
(1996)
The green mile: Night journey
P. Winkielman, N. Schwarz, Tedra Fazendeiro, R. Reber (2003)
The hedonic marking of processing fluency: Implications for evaluative judgment
W. Thompson, L. Balkwill, R. Vernescu (2000)
Expectancies generated by recent exposure to melodic sequencesMemory & Cognition, 28
R. Bornstein (1989)
Exposure and affect: Overview and meta-analysis of research, 1968–1987.Psychological Bulletin, 106
N. Gosselin, S. Samson, R. Adolphs, M. Noulhiane, M. Roy, D. Hasboun, M. Baulac, I. Peretz (2006)
Emotional responses to unpleasant music correlates with damage to the parahippocampal cortex.Brain : a journal of neurology, 129 Pt 10
M. Orr, S. Ohlsson (2005)
Relationship Between Complexity and Liking as a Function of ExpertiseMusic Perception, 22
V. Konečni (1996)
Daniel E. Berlyne (1924–1976): Two Decades LaterEmpirical Studies of the Arts, 14
M. Orr, S. Ohlsson (2001)
The Relationship Between Musical Complexity and Liking in Jazz and BluegrassPsychology of Music, 29
R. Bornstein, D. Leone, D. Galley (1987)
The Generalizability of Subliminal Mere Exposure Effects: Influence of Stimuli Perceived Without Awareness on Social BehaviorJournal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53
R. Zajonc (2006)
The Construction of Preference: Mere Exposure: A Gateway to the Subliminal
P. Barchas, K. Perlaki (1986)
Processing of preconsciously acquired information measured by hemispheric asymmetry and selection accuracy.Behavioral neuroscience, 100 3
I. Peretz, L. Gagnon, B. Bouchard (1998)
Music and emotion: perceptual determinants, immediacy, and isolation after brain damageCognition, 68
A. Gabrielsson, P. Juslin (1996)
Emotional Expression in Music Performance: Between the Performer's Intention and the Listener's ExperiencePsychology of Music, 24
I. Peretz, A. Blood, V. Penhune, R. Zatorre (2001)
Cortical deafness to dissonance.Brain : a journal of neurology, 124 Pt 5
G. Gerardi, L. Gerken (1995)
The Development of Affective Responses to Modality and Melodic ContourMusic Perception, 12
P. Rentfrow, S. Gosling (2003)
PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES The Do Re Mi’s of Everyday Life: The Structure and Personality Correlates of Music Preferences
(1939)
What features of a musical phrase have emotional suggestiveness? Bulletin of the Oklahoma
L. Gagnon, I. Peretz (2003)
Mode and tempo relative contributions to “happy-sad” judgements in equitone melodiesCognition and Emotion, 17
D. Berlyne (1970)
Novelty, complexity, and hedonic valuePerception & Psychophysics, 8
S. Bella, I. Peretz, L. Rousseau, N. Gosselin (2001)
A developmental study of the affective value of tempo and mode in musicCognition, 80
B. Whittlesea, Lisa Williams (2000)
The source of feelings of familiarity: the discrepancy-attribution hypothesis.Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition, 26 3
M. Meyer (1903)
Experimental studies in the psychology of musicAmerican Journal of Psychology, 14
((1992))
Inhibitory effects of awareness on affective responding
E. Verveer, H. Barry, W. Bousfield (1933)
Change in Affectivity with RepetitionAmerican Journal of Psychology, 45
We examined liking for happy- and sad-sounding music as a function of exposure, which varied both in quantity (number of exposures) and in quality (focused or incidental listening). Liking ratings were higher for happy than for sad music after focused listening, but similar after incidental listening. In the incidental condition, liking ratings increased linearly as a function of exposure. In the focused condition, liking ratings were an inverted U-shaped function of exposure, with initial increases in liking (after 2 exposures) followed by decreases (after 8 or 32 exposures). The results documented that: (1) sad music is liked as much as happy music in some instances; (2) frequency of exposure causes both familiarity (positive) and over-familiarity (negative) effects; and (3) effects of exposure on liking differ for focused and incidental listening.
Cognition & Emotion – Taylor & Francis
Published: Feb 1, 2008
Read and print from thousands of top scholarly journals.
Already have an account? Log in
Bookmark this article. You can see your Bookmarks on your DeepDyve Library.
To save an article, log in first, or sign up for a DeepDyve account if you don’t already have one.
Copy and paste the desired citation format or use the link below to download a file formatted for EndNote
Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
All DeepDyve websites use cookies to improve your online experience. They were placed on your computer when you launched this website. You can change your cookie settings through your browser.