Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
S. Kosslyn (1994)
Image and Brain: The Resolution of the Imagery DebateJournal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7
Judy Pearsall (1999)
The Concise Oxford Dictionary
(1990)
The pictographic molecular paradigm: pictorial communication in the chemical and biological sciences
M. Jemal (1999)
An Alternative Method of Introducing FugacityThe Chemical Educator, 4
L. Magnani, N. Nersessian, Paul Thagard (1999)
Model-Based Reasoning in Scientific Discovery
F. Abd‐El‐Khalick, Norman Lederman (2000)
Improving science teachers' conceptions of nature of science: a critical review of the literatureInternational Journal of Science Education, 22
Meg Miller, M. Watts (1997)
Reflection on reflectionsCounselling Psychology Review
É. Francoeur (1997)
The Forgotten Tool: The Design and Use of Molecular ModelsSocial Studies of Science, 27
(1991)
Developing students' understanding of chemical concepts
(2004)
World index of molecular visualization resources
R. Laugksch (2000)
Scientific literacy: A conceptual overviewScience Education, 84
(1994)
Psychology and displays in GIS
R. Kozma, J. Russell (1997)
Multimedia and understanding: Expert and novice responses to different representations of chemical phenomenaJournal of Research in Science Teaching, 34
A. Johnstone (1993)
The development of chemistry teaching: a changing response to changing demandJournal of Chemical Education, 70
J. Brophy (2001)
Subject-Specific Instructional Methods and Activities
A. MacEachren, D. Taylor (1994)
Visualization in modern cartography
R. Kozma (2003)
The material features of multiple representations and their cognitive and social affordances for science understandingLearning and Instruction, 13
(1988)
Theories,principles and laws
M. McGee (1979)
Human spatial abilities: psychometric studies and environmental, genetic, hormonal, and neurological influences.Psychological bulletin, 86 5
R. Griffin (1997)
VisionQuest: Journeys toward Visual Literacy. Selected Readings from the Annual Conference of the International Visual Literacy Association (28th, Cheyenne, Wyoming, October, 1996).
C. Hertzog, R. Dixon (1994)
Metacognitive development in adulthood and old age.
W. Winn (1991)
Learning from maps and diagramsEducational Psychology Review, 3
(1994)
The importance of specific science content in the enhancement of metacognition
Michael Hinton, M. Nakhleh (1999)
Students’ Microscopic, Macroscopic, and Symbolic Representations of Chemical ReactionsThe Chemical Educator, 4
N. Barnea, Y. Dori (1999)
High-School Chemistry Students' Performance and Gender Differences in a Computerized Molecular Modeling Learning EnvironmentJournal of Science Education and Technology, 8
F. Korthagen (2001)
A reflection on reflection.
R. Kozma, E. Chin, J. Russell, Nancy Marx (2000)
The Roles of Representations and Tools in the Chemistry Laboratory and Their Implications for Chemistry LearningJournal of the Learning Sciences, 9
P. Fensham, R. Gunstone, R. White (1994)
The Content Of Science: A Constructive Approach To Its Teaching And Learning
P. Adey, M. Shayer (1994)
Really Raising Standards: Cognitive intervention and academic achievement
D. Ardaç, S. Akaygun (2004)
Effectiveness of multimedia-based instruction that emphasizes molecular representations on students' understanding of chemical changeJournal of Research in Science Teaching, 41
F. Weinert, R. Kluwe (1987)
Metacognition, motivation, and understanding
Justin Buchler (1941)
The Philosophy of Peirce: Selected WritingsThe New England Quarterly, 14
R. Haber (1970)
How we remember what we see.Scientific American, 222 5
M. Siegel (1995)
More Than Words: The Generative Power of Transmediation for Learning.Canadian Journal of Education, 20
G. Sinatra, P. Pintrich (2003)
Intentional Conceptual Change
Michael Pyryt (1998)
Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor analytic studiesGifted and talented international, 13
A. Balaban (1999)
Visual Chemistry: Three-Dimensional Perception of Chemical StructuresJournal of Science Education and Technology, 8
R. Garner, P. Alexander (1989)
Metacognition: Answered and Unanswered QuestionsEducational Psychologist, 24
(2003)
The significance of different kinds of molecular models in teaching and learning chemistry
Jerry Christopherson (1997)
The Growing Need for Visual Literacy at the University.
A. Rothenberg (1995)
Creative Cognitive Processes in Kekule's Discovery of the Structure of the Benzene MoleculeAmerican Journal of Psychology, 108
Jeffrey Pribyl, G. Bodner (1987)
Spatial Ability and its Role in Organic Chemistry: A Study of Four Organic Courses.Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 24
N. Barnea (2000)
Teaching and Learning about Chemistry and Modelling with a Computer managed Modelling System
S. Gilbert (1991)
Model building and a definition of scienceJournal of Research in Science Teaching, 28
S. Kosslyn (1987)
Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: a computational approach.Psychological review, 94 2
G. Bodner, T. McMillen (1986)
COGNITIVE RESTRUCTURING AS AN EARLY STAGE IN PROBLEM SOLVINGJournal of Research in Science Teaching, 23
(1987)
Executive decisions and regulation of problem solving behavior
J. Gilbert, Carolyn Boulter (2000)
Developing Models in Science Education
M. Whiting (1979)
Chemistry through models by Colin J. Suckling, Keith E. Suckling and Charles W. Suckling. Pp. 321. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge U.K. and New York. 1978. £15.00Biochemical Education, 7
D. Treagust, G. Chittleborough (2001)
Chemistry: A matter of understanding representations, 8
D. Gabel (1999)
Improving Teaching and Learning through Chemistry Education Research: A Look to the FutureJournal of Chemical Education, 76
W. Rouse, N. Morris (1986)
On Looking into the Black Box: Prospects and Limits in the Search for Mental ModelsPsychological Bulletin, 100
P. Keig, Peter Rubba (1993)
Translation of representations of the structure of matter and its relationship to reasoning, gender, spatial reasoning, and specific prior knowledgeJournal of Research in Science Teaching, 30
Daniela Bailer-Jones (1999)
Tracing the Development of Models in the Philosophy of Science
J. Tomasi (1988)
Models and modeling in theoretical chemistryJournal of Molecular Structure-theochem, 179
R. Mayer (2001)
Teaching for Thinking
P. Georghiades (2004)
From the general to the situated: three decades of metacognitionInternational Journal of Science Education, 26
J. Gilbert, Carolyn Boulter, M. Rutherford (2000)
Explanations with Models in Science Education
K. Korb (1991)
Explaining Science*The British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 42
T. Nelson, L. Narens (1994)
Why investigate metacognition
M. Peterson (1994)
Cognitive Issues in Cartographic Visualization, 2
(2004)
History of biological macromolecules
Hsin-Kai Wu, P. Shah (2004)
Exploring visuospatial thinking in chemistry learningScience Education, 88
R. Justi, J. Gilbert (1999)
A cause of ahistorical science teaching: Use of hybrid modelsScience Education, 83
S. Kosslyn, S. Pinker, G. Smith, Steven Shwartz (1979)
On the demystification of mental imageryBehavioral and Brain Sciences, 2
D. Hodson (1992)
In search of a meaningful relationship: an exploration of some issues relating to integration in science and science educationInternational Journal of Science Education, 14
G. Sinatra, P. Pintrich (2003)
Metacognitive Aspects of Students’ Reflective Discourse: Implications for Intentional Conceptual Change Teaching and Learning: M. Gertrude Hennessey
C. Peirce, Justin Buchler (1941)
The Philosophy of Peirce : Selected WritingsThe Journal of Philosophy, 38
P. Adey, A. Robertson, G. Venville (2002)
Effects of a cognitive acceleration programme on Year 1 pupils.The British journal of educational psychology, 72 Pt 1
K. Egan, Dan Nadaner (1988)
Imagination and education
Helen Tuckey, M. Selvaratnam (1993)
Studies Involving Three-Dimensional Visualisation Skills in Chemistry: A ReviewStudies in Science Education, 21
J. Gilbert, Carolyn Boulter, R. Elmer (2000)
Positioning Models in Science Education and in Design and Technology Education
J. Krygier (1995)
Visualization in Geographic Information SystemsJournal of the Brazilian Computer Society
(2001)
Teaching Multiliteracies Across the Curriculum
J. Flavell (1987)
Speculations about the nature and development of metacognition
L. Hudson (1970)
Frames of Mind: Ability, Perception and Self-Perception in the Arts and Sciences
[The range of terminology used in the field of ‘visualization’ is reviewed and, in the light of evidence that it plays a central role in the conduct of science, it is argued that it should play a correspondingly important role in science education. As all visualization is of, and produces, models, an epistemology and ontology for models as a class of entities is presented. Models can be placed in the public arena by means of a series of ‘modes and sub-modes of representation’. Visualization is central to learning, especially in the sciences, for students have to learn to navigate within and between the modes of representation. It is therefore argued that students -science students’ especially - must become metacognitive in respect of visualization, that they must show what I term ‘metavisual capability’. Without a metavisual capability, students find great difficulty in being able to undertake these demanding tasks. The development of metavisual capability is discussed in both theory and practice. Finally, some approaches to identifying students’ metavisual status are outlined and evaluated. It is concluded that much more research and development is needed in respect of visualization in science education if its importance is to be recognised and its potential realised.]
Published: Jan 1, 2005
Keywords: Mental Model; Bohr Model; Intentional Conceptual Change
Read and print from thousands of top scholarly journals.
Already have an account? Log in
Bookmark this article. You can see your Bookmarks on your DeepDyve Library.
To save an article, log in first, or sign up for a DeepDyve account if you don’t already have one.
Copy and paste the desired citation format or use the link below to download a file formatted for EndNote
Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
All DeepDyve websites use cookies to improve your online experience. They were placed on your computer when you launched this website. You can change your cookie settings through your browser.