Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
C. Wickens, Melanie LaClair, K. Sarno (1995)
Graph-Task Dependencies in Three-Dimensional Data: Influence of Three-Dimensionality and ColorProceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 39
Jeffrey Zacks, Ellen Levy, B. Tversky, D. Schiano (1998)
Reading bar graphs: Effects of extraneous depth cues and graphical context.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 4
J. Hollands, I. Spence (1992)
Judgments of Change and Proportion in Graphical PerceptionHuman Factors: The Journal of Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 34
David Simkin, R. Hastie (1987)
An Information-Processing Analysis of Graph PerceptionJournal of the American Statistical Association, 82
S. Dehaene, S. Bossini, P. Giraux (1993)
The mental representation of parity and number magnitudeSocial Science Review, 122
F. I. M. Craik, R. S. Lockhart (1972)
Levels of processing: A framework for memory researchJournal of Experimental Psychology: General, 11
S. Dehaene, Serge Bossini, P. Giraux (1993)
The mental representation of parity and number magnitude.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122
W. Barfield, Robert Robless (1989)
The effects of two- or three-dimensional graphics on the problem-solving performance of experienced and novice decision makersBehaviour & Information Technology, 8
M. Siegrist (1996)
The use or misuse of three-dimensional graphs to represent lower-dimensional dataBehav. Inf. Technol., 15
C. Carswell, Sylvia Frankenberger, Donald Bernhard (1991)
Graphing in depth: Perspectives on the use of three-dimensional graphs to represent lower-dimensional data.Behaviour & Information Technology, 10
F. E. Croxton, H. Stein (1932)
Graphic comparisons by bars, squares, circles, and cubesMathematical Cognition, 27
I. Spence, S. Lewandowsky (1991)
Displaying proportions and percentagesApplied Cognitive Psychology, 5
J. Hollands, B. Pierce, L. Magee (1995)
Displaying Quantitative Information in Two and Three DimensionsProceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 39
F. Croxton, Harold Stein (1932)
Graphic Comparisons by Bars, Squares, Circles, and CubesJournal of the American Statistical Association, 27
A. Baddeley (1990)
Human memory: Theory and practiceBehaviour and Information Technology
W. Cleveland, R. McGill (1984)
Graphical Perception: Theory, Experimentation, and Application to the Development of Graphical MethodsJournal of the American Statistical Association, 79
F. Craik, R. Lockhart (1972)
Levels of processing: A framework for memory researchJournal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11
S. Kosslyn (1993)
Elements of graph design
S. Fisher, J. Dempsey, R. Marousky (1997)
Data Visualization: Preference and Use of Two-Dimensional and Three-Dimensional GraphsSocial Science Computer Review, 15
C. M. Carswell, S. Frankenberger, D. Bernhard (1991)
Graphing in depth: Perspectives on the use of three‐dimensional graphs to represent lower‐dimensional dataJournal of the American Statistical Association, 10
W. Fias, M. Brysbaert, F. Geypens, G. d'Ydewalle (1996)
The importance of magnitude information in numerical processing: Evidence form the SNARC effectHuman Factors, 2
S. H. Fisher, J. V. Dempsey, R. T. Marousky (1997)
Data visualization: Preference and use of two‐dimensional and three‐dimensional graphsBehavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 15
Russell Jones, Ismael Careras (1996)
The empirical investigation of factors affecting graphical visualizationBehavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28
S. Kosslyn (1989)
Understanding charts and graphsApplied Cognitive Psychology, 3
W. Cleveland (1985)
The elements of graphing data
A. Baddeley (1990)
Human Memory: Theory and Practice, Revised Edition
D. Merwin, C. Wickens (1991)
2-D vs. 3-D Display for Multidimensional Data Visualization: The Relationship between Task Integrality and Display ProximityProceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting, 35
W. Fias (1996)
The Importance of Magnitude Information in Numerical Processing: Evidence from the SNARC EffectMathematical Cognition, 2
S. Pinker (1990)
A theory of graph comprehension.
Eight participants decided whether two‐ or three‐dimensional bars embedded within two‐ or three‐dimensional frames were semantically consistent with written inequalities of the form ‘A>B’. Inequalities were presented either before (Experiment 1) or after the graphs (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, irrelevant depth cues were associated with slower decision times and there was no processing cost associated with an inconsistency between the dimensionalities of bars and frames. Memory encoding and retrieval times in Experiment 2 were affected by both graph complexity and consistency. Neither a depth consistency heuristic nor the maximum ink–data ratio principle can account for these results. More appropriate guidance for graph design will come from elaborating the working memory component of current cognitive models of graph processing. Copyright © 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Applied Cognitive Psychology – Wiley
Published: Jan 1, 2000
Read and print from thousands of top scholarly journals.
Already have an account? Log in
Bookmark this article. You can see your Bookmarks on your DeepDyve Library.
To save an article, log in first, or sign up for a DeepDyve account if you don’t already have one.
Copy and paste the desired citation format or use the link below to download a file formatted for EndNote
Access the full text.
Sign up today, get DeepDyve free for 14 days.
All DeepDyve websites use cookies to improve your online experience. They were placed on your computer when you launched this website. You can change your cookie settings through your browser.