Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 14-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

Are Video Games a Gateway to Gambling? A Longitudinal Study Based on a Representative Norwegian Sample

Are Video Games a Gateway to Gambling? A Longitudinal Study Based on a Representative Norwegian... J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-018-9781-z ORIGINAL PAPER Are Video Games a Gateway to Gambling? A Longitudinal Study Based on a Representative Norwegian Sample 1 1 1 Helge Molde  · Bjørn Holmøy  · Aleksander Garvik Merkesdal  · 2 2 3 2 Torbjørn Torsheim  · Rune Aune Mentzoni  · Daniel Hanns  · Dominic Sagoe  · Ståle Pallesen Published online: 5 June 2018 © The Author(s) 2018 Abstract The scope and variety of video games and monetary gambling opportunities are expanding rapidly. In many ways, these forms of entertainment are converging on digi- tal and online video games and gambling sites. However, little is known about the rela- tionship between video gaming and gambling. The present study explored the possibility of a directional relationship between measures of problem gaming and problem gambling, while also controlling for the influence of sex and age. In contrast to most previous inves - tigations which are based on cross-sectional designs and non-representative samples, the present study utilized a longitudinal design conducted over 2 years (2013, 2015) and com- prising 4601 participants (males 47.2%, age range 16–74) drawn from a random sample from the general population. Video gaming and gambling were assessed using the Gaming Addiction Scale for Adolescents and the Canadian Problem Gambling Index, respectively. Using an autoregressive cross-lagged structural equation model, we found a positive rela- tionship between scores on problematic gaming and later scores on problematic gambling, whereas we found no evidence of the reverse relationship. Hence, video gaming problems appear to be a gateway behavior to problematic gambling behavior. In future research, one should continue to monitor the possible reciprocal behavioral influences between gambling and video gaming. Keywords Gambling · Video gaming · Longitudinal · Representative sample · Cross- lagged * Helge Molde helge.molde@uib.no Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway Hochschule Darmstadt, University of Applied Sciences, Darmstadt, Germany 1 3 546 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 Introduction Behavioral addictions can be understood as the excessive and continued involvement in activities that cause harm to the addict or to persons with important relationships to the addict (American Psychiatric Association 2013). The conceptualization of such addic- tions is often modeled on the diagnostic criteria for substance abuse disorders. Cur- rently, the most researched and so far only behavioral addiction recognized by formal psychiatric nosology, is gambling disorder (Petry et al. 2014). The term gambling gen- erally refers to games where the player bets money or other valuable items on an out- come that fully or partly relies on chance (Hodgins et al. 2011). Historically, gambling can be considered an almost universal feature of human societies. However, the diver- sity and accessibility of formalized gambling activities and platforms have increased dramatically during the last decades. The increased accessibility has entailed the devel- opment of massive commercial casinos and gambling destinations as well as the rising availability of online gambling. Likewise, the video gaming industry has expanded con- siderably during the past decade, similarly to the development of the gambling industry (King et al. 2010, 2015). For most people who gamble occasionally, gambling primarily represents recreation and an enjoyable activity (Back et  al. 2011). However, some people develop problem- atic patterns of gambling activity that are harmful to themselves and to society. Common characteristics of problematic gambling patterns include inappropriate amounts of time and money spent on gambling activities and the continuation of gambling activities even though gamblers’ personal relationships, financial assets, status or other important aspects of life are significantly impaired. Excessive gambling is thus associated with debts, bank - ruptcy and professional problems, as well as substantial psychological distress (Nower et al. 2015; Kessler et al. 2008). It is estimated that each problem gambler affects between five to fifteen other persons (Kalischuk et al. 2006). The estimated prevalence rates of gam- bling disorder vary depending on the specific country, survey year, and method of meas- urement; however, according to a standardized past year rate of problem gambling it ranges from 0.5 to 7.6%, with the average rate across all countries is 2.3% (Williams et al. 2012). Gambling problems are more common among some subsets of the general population, such as younger people, male gender, single or divorced/separated marital status, and low socio- economic status (Toneatto and Nguyen 2007; Petry et al. 2005). Gambling disorder is also associated with increased risk of other addictive disorders, especially alcoholism and sub- stance abuse disorder (Petry 2005). Other mental illnesses, such as mood disorders and anxiety disorders, have further been shown to have high comorbidity with gambling dis- order and comorbid mental illnesses might predispose high-risk individuals to develop or worsen pre-existing gambling problems (Kessler et al. 2008; Dowling et al. 2015). Nevertheless, little is known about the relationship between measures of problematic gambling and video gaming especially in terms of temporal associations. It is important, however, to elucidate the latter relationship for several reasons. Gaming and gambling seem increasingly to be converging. Gambling is constantly being digitized and diver- sified into a multitude of online games at the same time as video games increasingly contain themes and elements from more traditional gambling activities. Furthermore, websites offering both gaming and gambling increase dual accessibility of the activi- ties, which may cause higher levels of involvement (Fisher and Griffiths 1995). New developments such as betting on e-sport represent further examples of the increasing intertwinedness of gaming and gambling (Hutchins 2008). 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 547 While there is concern regarding how to properly distinguish gaming and gambling (King et  al. 2015), the potential for comorbidity and “recruitment” from one problem category to the other is immanent. So far, there are mixed results regarding comorbid- ity between video gaming and gambling. In a recent study with a sample of Australian video gamers, no significant relationship between frequency of video gaming and gam- bling was found (Forrest et  al. 2016). However, some studies suggest a link between involvement in gaming and gambling. For example, the frequency of visits to a non- gambling video game arcade was found to be positively associated with the frequency of disordered gambling (Ladouceur and Dubé 1995). A study of children found a posi- tive association between time spent playing video games and the likelihood to partake in risk-taking gambling (Gupta and Derevensky 1996). A study of German students aged 12–25 (Walther et al. 2012) reported a significant correlation between problematic video gaming and gambling. A recent study by McBride and Derevensky (2016) exam- ined the gambling and video game behaviors of 1229 adolescents and young adults. The results indicate that gamblers, compared with non-gamblers, were more likely to play video games and that video gamers, compared to non-gamers, were more likely to take part in gambling activities. Against this backdrop, the present study seeks to explore the directionality between problematic video gaming and gambling using a representative population sample and a longitudinal design based on assessments separated by 2 years. Method Procedure We conducted a longitudinal panel study covering a 2-year period. The first wave was carried out in 2013, the second wave in 2015. The survey was conducted on behalf of the Norwegian  Gaming and Foundation Authority and was approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Related Ethics in Western Norway (REK-Vest, project no. 2013/120). In all, 24,000 people aged 16–74 were randomly selected from the National Population Registry of Norway and invited to participate in a postal survey in 2013. A letter attached to the survey informed that all respondents would automati- cally be included in a raffle with a chance to win a gift voucher worth NOK 500. A max- imum of two reminders were sent to those who had not responded to the survey. This same procedure was used during the second wave. Participants In all, 10,081 valid answers were collected during the first wave (response rate 43.6%). These respondents were invited to participate again in 2015. In the second wave, 5809 valid answers were collected (response rate 57.6%); hence, these 5809 individuals make up the basis of this panel study. All participants included in the present study confirmed in at least one of the two waves that they had played video games during the previous 6 months and/or had participated in gambling activities during the past 12 months. This amounted to 4601 respondents, 2172 (47.2%) males and 2429 (52.8%) females, with an age range in wave 1 of 16–74 years of age (mean = 48 years, SD = 15.1). 1 3 548 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 Instruments The Gaming Addiction Scale for Adolescents (GASA) is a validated and frequently used measurement instrument. The GASA is based on previous research on video gam- ing and the biopsychosocial model of addiction (Griffiths 2005; Lemmens et al. 2009). Respondents are asked to respond to items reflecting different aspects of problematic video gaming during the previous 6  months. The response options range from never (0) to very often (4). The short version of this questionnaire contains seven items, each tapping into one of the criteria of addiction (salience, tolerance, mood modification, relapse, withdrawal, conflict and problems). We used a validated Norwegian version (Mentzoni et  al. 2011). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha was 0.83 in both waves. A composite sum score was computed for each individual participant by adding the scores on the items in the scale. Video gaming behavior was regarded as a phenome- non with a continuous severity dimension, where higher composite scores on the GASA indicate higher involvement/severity. To assess the extent of gambling problems, we administered the Canadian Problem Gambling Index (CPGI; Ferris and Wynne 2001). The CPGI consists of nine items, five of which measure problematic gambling behavior, and four measure negative conse- quences of gambling behavior. Each item is answered on a 4-point scale ranging from never (0) to always (3). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.92 in wave 1 and 0.84 in wave 2. A composite score was computed by adding the scores on the items in the scale for each individual participant. Gambling behavior was regarded as a phenomenon with a contin- uous severity dimension, where higher ordinary sum scores indicate higher involvement/ severity. Analyses To assess the unidimensionality of the CPGI and the GASA, we applied principal com- ponent analysis. The correlations among the seven items of the GASA and the nine items of the CPGI indicated that they each measure one underlying construct. We could therefore use the composite scores for both measures when conducting our main statisti- cal analyses. We used structural equation modeling (SEM) for the main analyses. To explore the correlations over time, we applied an autoregressive cross-lagged panel model using the composite scores (i.e., a model without indicators) for both video gaming and gambling (see Lemmens et  al. 2011b, for a comparable model). The lavaan package for struc- tural equation modelling in R (Rosseel 2011) was used to fit both models. Full infor - mation maximum likelihood (FIML) was applied, where the missing values were esti- mated from all available data. Robust Huber–White standard errors were applied due to skewness within the data. Because outliers could affect the correlations, we ran a model without outliers to investigate whether this was the case. The results revealed that the directional associations remained the same, and only marginal differences in strength appeared. Based on these results, we decided to use all data in our analyses. 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 549 Table 1 Descriptive statistics of GASA and CPGI in wave 1 and wave 2 Variable N Range Minimum Maximum Mean (SD) Median GASA W1 1719 23 0 23 2.56 (3.60) 1.00 GASA W2 1781 28 0 28 2.33 (3.52) 1.00 CPGI W1 3593 1 0 1 0.14 (0.35) 0.00 CPGI W2 3553 1 0 1 0.14 (0.34) 0.00 Results Descriptive and Correlational Statistics Descriptive data for the GASA and the CPGI are provided in Table 1. In the two waves, 3593 and 3533 respondents, respectively, confirmed that they had participated in gam- bling activities during the prior 12  months, and thus completed the CPGI. The num- bers of respondents who confirmed that they had played video games during the past 6 months were 1719 in wave 1 and 1781 in wave 2. In both waves, respondents generally had low scores on both the GASA and the CPGI, as indicated by the means. The means and standard deviations differed little between the first and the second wave, suggesting little change at the group level in terms of gaming and gambling problems. The variance was also similar across the two waves for both measures. Significant correlations were found between all variables, where the strongest correla- tions were between the wave 1 and wave 2 measures of the same construct. The largest coefficient was found for GASA in wave 1 and wave 2 (r = 0.60). The correlation coef- ficient for CPGI scores in wave 1 and wave 2 was r = 0.44. For more details, see Table 2. Autoregressive Cross‑Lagged Panel Analysis The results are displayed in Table  3 and Fig.  1. A strong model fit was indicated by the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA = 0.000) and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI = 1.000). This strong model fit is due to the use of observed composite scores with no indicators. The analysis showed that the measures of gambling and video gaming Table 2 Correlation coefficients within and between the study variables in wave 1 and wave 2 GASA W1 GASA W2 CPGI W1 CPGI W2 Sex Age GASA W1 – GASA W2 0.60** – CPGI W1 0.25** 0.19** – CPGI W2 0.25** 0.19** 0.44** – Sex 0.05** 0.06** 0.09** 0.09** – Age − 0.28** − 0.30** − 0.07** − 0.10** 0.09** – Sex was coded; female = 0, male = 1 *p <0.05; **p <0.01 1 3 550 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 Table 3 Autoregressive cross- Paths Unstandardized Stand- lagged panel model: covariances, estimates ardized regression weights and model fit estimates indices Covariances  GASA W1 ↔ CPGI W1 0.26** 0.22  GASA W2 ↔ CPGI W2 0.03 0.03  Sex ↔ age 0.64** 0.08 Autoregressive paths  GASA W1 → GASA W2 0.49** 0.50  CPGI W1 → CPGI W2 0.39** 0.39 Cross-lagged paths  GASA WI → CPGI W2 0.02** 0.15  CPGI W1 → GASA W2 0.45 0.05 Control variables  Sex → GASA W1 0.54** 0.08  Sex → GASA W2 0.07 0.01  Sex → CPGI W1 0.07** 0.10  Sex → CPGI W2 0.04** 0.06  Age → GASA W1 − 0.07** − 0.29  Age → GASA W2 − 0.03** − 0.14  Age → CPGI W1 − 0.00 ** − 0.09  Age → CPGI W2 − 0.00** − 0.05 Sex was coded; female = 0, male = 1 *p <0.05; **p <0.01 .50** GASA W 2 GASA W 1 .22** CPGI W 1 CPGI W 2 .39** Fig. 1 Configural model of the cross-lagged panel analysis. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 551 problems had a statistically significant positive covariation (β = 0.22, p <0.01) in wave 1. In wave 2, no such relationship was found (β = 0.03, p <0.36). As shown by the hori- zontal line in Fig. 1, scores on the GASA in wave 1 significantly predicted scores on the GASA in wave 2 (β = 0.50, p < 0.01). CPGI scores in wave 1 also significantly predicted CPGI scores in wave 2 (β = 0.39, p < 0.01). In addition, we investigated the effects of sex and age on the GASA and CPGI. Anal- ysis revealed that men reported higher total scores on the GASA compared to women in wave 1 (β = 0.08, p < 0.01); at the second wave, this relationship was no longer signifi- cant (β = 0.01, p < 0.65). Men reported higher scores on the CPGI compared to women in both waves (wave 1: β = 0.10, p < 0.01; wave 2: β = 0.06, p < 0.01). Age was found to be significantly and negatively related to the GASA scores in both waves (wave 1: β = − 0.29, p < 0.01; wave 2: β = − 0.14, p < 0.01). The same tendency, albeit smaller, was found for scores on the CPGI in both waves (wave 1: β = − 0.09, p = 0.01; wave 2: β = − 0.05, p < 0.05). Hence, the results indicate that younger respondents reported higher scores on both the GASA and the CPGI measures in both waves. The cross-lagged analyses also revealed that the scores on the GASA in wave 1 sig- nificantly predicted scores on the CPGI in wave 2 (β = 0.15, p < 0.01). No such rela- tionship was found between scores derived from the CPGI in wave 1 and scores on the GASA in wave 2 (β = 0.05, p < 0.12). This shows that a measure of video gaming prob- lems is positively related to a measure of gambling problems at a later time, while the reverse relationship is not statistically significant. Estimates of intercepts and variance for the variables are provided in Table 4. They show higher scores on both measures in wave 1. Table 4 Autoregressive cross- Unstandardized SE Stand- lagged panel model: intercepts estimates ardized and variance estimates Intercepts  GASA W1 4.45** 0.33 1.26  GASA W2 2.21** 0.35 0.65  CPGI W1 0.15** 0.03 0.42  CPGI W2 0.06 0.03 0.16  Sex 1.47** 0.01 2.95  Age 50.00** 0.22 3.30 Variance  GASA W1 11.33** 0.66 0.92  GASA W2 7.99** 0.66 0.68  CPGI W1 0.12** 0.00 0.99  CPGI W2 0.09** 0.00 0.78  Sex 0.25** 0.00 1.00  Age 229.57** 3.74 1.00 Sex was coded; female = 0, male = 1 **p < 0.01 1 3 552 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 Discussion A finding consistent with the research literature in general is that there were positive cor - relations between measures of the same construct over time, suggesting some stability over the 2-year period (Williams et al. 2015). As for the relationship between gaming and gambling, we found a significant positive correlation between a score on the gaming problem scale and gambling at the first wave. However, this was not the case in the second wave, after controlling for the influence of sex and age. This indicates an inconsistency over time in the relationship between gambling and video gaming, which seems to be plausible given the mixed results on the strength of this relationship and the somewhat unstable and transitional nature of gambling and video gaming problems (LaPlante et al. 2008; Kuss et al. 2014; Thege et al. 2015). Another pos- sible and perhaps complementary explanation for this finding may be that our sample had aged 2 years, which may have further reduced an initially weak correlation. This result may be seen in light of one of the most consistent findings in video game research—namely, that younger age is strongly correlated with more frequent playing (Ferguson et al. 2011; Toneatto and Nguyen, 2007). Perhaps most interestingly, our results show that scores on the gaming problem scale at wave 1 predicted the scores on the gambling problem scale at wave 2, but there was no evidence of the reverse relationship. This suggests that video gaming constituted a risk fac- tor for gambling 2 years later. Studies exploring the potential causal relationship between video gaming and gambling based on longitudinal designs and representative samples are very rare. The few existing studies generally show evidence of increased gambling among high-frequency video gamers (McBride and Derevensky 2016). However, there are mixed results regarding the strength of this relationship and whether extraneous variables are likely to mediate heightened involvement in both gaming and gambling (Delfabbro et  al. 2009). Results from the current study suggest and support a direct and causal relation- ship between video gaming and gambling. One possible explanation for the relationship between problematic gaming and gambling is that the participants had become 2  years older. This could indicate that a significant subset of those involved with gaming developed an involvement with gambling during the transition. As age restrictions apply more strictly and widely to gambling activities than to video gaming, most people who eventually start to gamble are introduced to video games at an earlier age. It is thus conceivable that those who follow a path progressing from problematic gaming to gambling are generally prone to addictive behaviors and that the manifestation is moderated by age (Sussman et al. 2011). Another set of factors possibly augmenting such a trajectory is the increasing convergence between video gaming and gambling (King et  al. 2010). This trend manifests itself, for example, in an increasing number of gambling products that adopt features from video games and also more video games containing intrinsic gambling themes (King et al. 2012, 2014; Walther et  al. 2012). Furthermore, advertisements and game-related incentives are substantial for enticing players to games that involve monetary stakes and outcomes (King et al. 2015; Gainsbury et al. 2014b). In addition, formal and informal gambling has become a large part of e-sports (Holden et al. 2016). It is thus conceivable that all these factors—age related transitions, proneness to addic- tive behavior, digital convergence and strategic marketing—may explain why this particu- lar directionality was found. Some previous studies indicate that factors such as physical proximity of video gaming and gambling opportunities could lead to an increased number of problem gamblers, and a similar type of exposure effect could potentially operate in 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 553 the world of digital and online games (Fisher and Griffiths 1995; Gainsbury et al. 2014a; Ladouceur and Dubé 1995). Keeping in mind that such a particular pathway is unlikely to apply to the majority of problem gamblers, it would still be very informative for research, clinical practice, and prevention agencies to be aware of the possibility that subgroups of gamers may be prone to developing gambling problems in line with the mechanisms/path- ways outlined here. Being male predicted higher scores on the CPGI in both waves. The same was found for GASA in the first wave, which could be expected from previous investigations (Petry et  al. 2015; Rehbein et  al. 2016). This was not the case in the second wave, as gender was found to be unrelated for GASA. The interpretation of this latter finding is that there are no gender differences beyond the association controlled for in wave 1. The finding of male preponderance and increased involvement in gambling and/or gaming are in accord- ance with previous research (Pallesen et al. 2016; Wittek et al. 2016). Still, attitudes toward video gaming activities seem to be changing, possibly due to more widespread acceptance, less restrictive stereotypes of gamers and increased interest on the part of females in video games (Ipsos MediaCT 2012). This is likely to be influenced by the development of a wider range of games, many of which are designed to be appealing to target groups that deviate far beyond the traditional “profile” of a teenage male video gamer. Some recent evidence also indicates that choice of gaming device is relevant, as Pallesen et al. (2016) found that females are more likely than males to play games through social media. Strengths and Limitations The findings from the present study have several important theoretical and potentially prac- tical implications. Generally, the research literature in the field of problematic video gam- ing is too heavily focused on narrow subsets of the population, that is, people who are known to be problematic gamers or who represent subgroups considered more likely to develop such problems. Such convenience and strategic sampling is insufficient to under - stand the extent and characteristics of gamers in general. In contrast, our sample was rep- resentative of the general population. In order to deepen our understanding of and to refine our efforts to deal effectively with both video gaming and gambling, representative sam- ples need be used from which generalizable results can be obtained. In addition to the large and representative sample, the validity of our conclusions is reinforced by the fact that the response rate was relatively high and that the distribution of male and female respond- ents was fairly even. Another asset of the current study is that it employed a longitudinal design that models the directions of relationships between variables. This allowed us to indicate which type of addictive behavior is predictive of the other, and how each behavior is related to potential confounding variables. The overall novelty of the field of video gam- ing research and the relative lack of studies that explicitly explore the association between video gaming and gambling make the current findings and research approach informative and relevant for future investigations. In term of limitations, it should be noted that all data were self-reported. Such data are susceptible to biased self-descriptions, limitations in recollection and other sources of inac- curate reporting (Arnold and Feldman 1981). As the age of the current sample ranged from 16 to 74, it can be questioned whether this instrument, which was developed and validated for use on adolescents (Lemmens et al. 2009), was suitable for the current sample. How- ever, studies have shown that the GASA has good psychometric properties when applied to adult samples (Andreassen et al. 2016; Festl et al. 2013). 1 3 554 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 One important methodological concern relates to the longitudinal design of the study, more specifically, the appropriateness of choosing two points of measurement separated by 2  years for exploring the relationship between the variables. It has been argued that due to the phenomenon of regression towards the mean, a minimum of three waves of measurement are needed to confidently draw causal inferences from longitudinal data (Segrin 2010). It should be noted, that several comparable studies in the field of video gaming and gambling have employed two points of measurement over even more limited periods (Lemmens et al. 2011a, b). One might expect that phenom- enon to be flexible, innovative and ever changing as video gaming and gambling are more subject to change over brief periods of time. Therefore, there seems to be a high demand for studies that can detect transitions occurring within relatively short time periods. Another possible methodological limitation relates to the chosen method of analysis, as cross-lagged models have been criticized for presuming multiple assump- tions that are unlikely to be met in reality (Segrin 2010). The majority of respondents in this study were neither problematic gamers nor problematic gamblers but the scales used in the present study represent continuous scores of problematic gaming and gam- bling. Thus, the present study should not be taken as categorical evidence that problem gamers develop into problem gamblers, but rather that the level of problem gaming is associated with a subsequent level of problem gambling, with the known caveat that much of the observed variation occurred on the lower end of both measures. Whether or not problem gamers as a category are at increased risk of developing subsequent problem gambling is a question that should be addressed in future research. Implications and Directions for Future Research Because only a few studies have explicitly compared problematic video gaming and gambling, especially with representative samples and longitudinal designs, the rela- tionship between these variables remains largely unclear. This highlights the point made by some authors that a focus solely on problematic video gaming or gambling might limit our understanding of these phenomena, as some research indicates that video gaming and gambling can yield some positive effects for its users as well (Granic et  al. 2014). Broadening the interest in this manner might expand our understanding of the differences between normal and pathological involvement, and the causes and trajectories involved when these lines are crossed. Therefore, it would seem that more longitudinal studies based on representative samples are warranted. One factor we did not explore was the influence of types of games played, which seems to be an important nuance, especially when considering male and female gam- bling preferences. In a similar vein, one might also suspect that the strong relationship between young age and video gaming and gambling will lessen with time. Such a trend would be expected due to the likely cohort-effects involved in usage of technological entertainment and developmental matureness (e.g., biological), but could be hastened by increased competence within older cohorts and improved efforts from developers to reach wider audiences. Given the massive public interest in, availability of, and increasing use of the Internet, it seems highly plausible that the groups of people who are involved in video gaming as well as the ways in which video games are played will continue to expand and diversify. 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 555 Conclusion The present study explored the prospective relationships between video gaming and gam- bling in a representative sample of the general Norwegian population using two waves of data collection spaced by an interval of 2  years. The results indicated that scores on the problem gaming scale predicted scores on the problem gambling scale, whereas the reverse relationship was not found. The scores on the two instruments predicted their respective scores on the same measure 2  years later, suggesting some consistency of measures of gaming and gambling problems. Given that both the variety and diversity of video gam- ing and gambling are rapidly developing, changing and expanding, one should continue to monitor the impact that involvement in either of the two behaviors has on the other, in both the short and the long term. Compliance with Ethical Standards Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. Ethical Approval All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and the national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Inter- national License (http://creat iveco mmons .org/licen ses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. References American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5 ). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. Andreassen, C. S., Billieux, J., Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., Demetrovics, Z., Mazzoni, E., et  al. (2016). The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30(2), 252. https :// doi.org/10.1037/adb00 00160 . Arnold, H. J., & Feldman, D. C. (1981). Social desirability response bias in self-report choice situations. Academy of Management Journal, 24(2), 377–385. https ://doi.org/10.2307/25584 8. Back, K.-J., Lee, C.-K., & Stinchfield, R. (2011). Gambling motivation and passion: A comparison study of recreational and pathological gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27(3), 355–370. https ://doi. org/10.1007/s1089 9-010-9212-2. Delfabbro, P., King, D., Lambos, C., & Puglies, S. (2009). Is video-game playing a risk factor for patho- logical gambling in Australian adolescents? Journal of Gambling Studies, 25(3), 391–405. https ://doi. org/10.1007/s1089 9-009-9138-8. Dowling, N. A., Cowlishaw, S., Jackson, A. C., Merkouris, S. S., Francis, K. L., & Christensen, D. R. (2015). The prevalence of comorbid personality disorders in treatment-seeking problem gamblers: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Personality Disorders, 29(6), 735–754. https ://doi. org/10.1521/pedi_2014_28_168. Ferguson, C. J., Coulson, M., & Barnett, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of pathological gaming prevalence and comorbidity with mental health, academic and social problems. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(12), 1573–1578. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsyc hires .2011.09.005. Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian problem gambling index. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse. Festl, R., Scharkow, M., & Quandt, T. (2013). Problematic computer game use among adolescents, younger and older adults. Addiction, 108(3), 592–599. https ://doi.org/10.1111/add.12016 . 1 3 556 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 Fisher, S., & Griffiths, M. (1995). Current trends in slot machine gambling: Research and policy issues. Journal of Gambling Studies, 11(3), 239–247. https ://doi.org/10.1007/BF021 04791 . Forrest, C. J., King, D. L., & Delfabbro, P. H. (2016). The gambling preferences and behaviors of a commu- nity sample of Australian regular video game players. Journal of Gambling Studies, 32(2), 409–420. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1089 9-015-9535-0. Gainsbury, S. M., Hing, N., Delfabbro, P. H., & King, D. L. (2014a). A taxonomy of gambling and casino games via social media and online technologies. International Gambling Studies, 14(2), 196–213. https ://doi.org/10.1080/14459 795.2014.89063 4. Gainsbury, S. M., Russell, A., Hing, N., Wood, R., Lubman, D. I., & Blaszczynski, A. (2014b). The prevalence and determinants of problem gambling in Australia: Assessing the impact of interac- tive gambling and new technologies. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(3), 769. https ://doi. org/10.1037/a0036 207. Granic, I., Lobel, A., & Engels, R. C. (2014). The benefits of playing video games. American Psycholo- gist, 69(1), 66. https ://doi.org/10.1037/a0034 857. Griffiths, M. (2005). A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. Journal of Substance Use, 10(4), 191–197. https ://doi.org/10.1080/14659 89050 01143 59. Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. L. (1996). The relationship between gambling and video-game playing behavior in children and adolescents. Journal of Gambling Studies, 12(4), 375–394. https ://doi. org/10.1007/BF015 39183 . Hodgins, D. C., Stea, J. N., & Grant, J. E. (2011). Gambling disorders. The Lancet, 378(9806), 1874– 1884. https ://doi.org/10.1016/S0140 -6736(10)62185 -X. Holden, J. T., Rodenberg, R. M., & Kaburakis, A. (2016). Esports corruption: Gambling, doping, and global governance. Maryland Journal of International Law, 32(1), 236–273. Hutchins, B. (2008). Signs of meta-change in second modernity: The growth of e-sport and the World Cyber Games. New Media & Society, 10, 851–869. Ipsos MediaCT. (2012). Videogames in Europe: Consumer study. European summary report. Retrieved from the Interactive Software Federation of Europe website: http://www.isfe.eu/sites /isfe.eu/files / attac hment s/euro_summa ry_-_isfe_consu mer_study .pdf. Retrieved 9 June 2017. Kalischuk, R. G., Nowatzki, N., Cardwell, K., Klein, K., & Solowoniuk, J. (2006). Problem gambling and its impact on families: A literature review. International Gambling Studies, 6, 31–60. Kessler, R. C., Hwang, I., LaBrie, R., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Winters, K. C., et  al. (2008). DSM-IV pathological gambling in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychological Medicine, 38(09), 1351–1360. https ://doi.org/10.1017/S0033 29170 80029 00. King, D., Delfabbro, P., & Griffiths, M. (2010). The convergence of gambling and digital media: Impli- cations for gambling in young people. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26(2), 175–187. https ://doi. org/10.1007/s1089 9-009-9153-9. King, D. L., Delfabbro, P. H., Kaptsis, D., & Zwaans, T. (2014). Adolescent simulated gambling via digital and social media: An emerging problem. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 305–313. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.048. King, D. L., Ejova, A., & Delfabbro, P. H. (2012). Illusory control, gambling, and video gaming: An investigation of regular gamblers and video game players. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28(3), 421–435. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1089 9-011-9271-z. King, D. L., Gainsbury, S. M., Delfabbro, P. H., Hing, N., & Abarbanel, B. (2015). Distinguishing between gaming and gambling activities in addiction research. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4(4), 215–220. https ://doi.org/10.1556/2006.4.2015.045. Kuss, D. J., Griffiths, M. D., Karila, L., & Billieux, J. (2014). Internet addiction: A systematic review of epidemiological research for the last decade. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(25), 4026–4052. https ://doi.org/10.2174/13816 12811 31999 90617 . Ladouceur, R., & Dubé, D. (1995). Prevalence of pathological gambling and associated problems in individuals who visit non-gambling video arcades. Journal of Gambling Studies, 11(4), 361–365. https ://doi.org/10.1007/BF021 08614 . LaPlante, D. A., Nelson, S. E., LaBrie, R. A., & Shaffer, H. J. (2008). Stability and progression of dis- ordered gambling: Lessons from longitudinal studies. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 53(1), 52–60. https ://doi.org/10.1177/07067 43708 05300 108. Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2009). Development and validation of a game addiction scale for adolescents. Media Psychology, 12(1), 77–95. https ://doi.org/10.1080/15213 26080 26694 Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2011a). The effects of pathological gaming on aggres- sive behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(1), 38–47. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1096 4-010-9558-x. 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 557 Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2011b). Psychosocial causes and consequences of pathological gaming. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(1), 144–152. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j. chb.2010.07.015. McBride, J., & Derevensky, J. (2016). Gambling and video game playing among youth. Journal of Gam- bling Issues. https ://doi.org/10.4309/jgi.2016.34.9. Mentzoni, R. A., Brunborg, G. S., Molde, H., Myrseth, H., Skouverøe, K. J. M., Hetland, J., et al. (2011). Problematic video game use: Estimated prevalence and associations with mental and physical health. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(10), 591–596. https ://doi.org/10.1089/cyber .2010.0260. Nower, L., Eyrich-Garg, K. M., Pollio, D. E., & North, C. S. (2015). Problem gambling and homeless- ness: Results from an epidemiologic study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 31(2), 533–545. https ://doi. org/10.1007/s1089 9-013-9435-0. Pallesen, S., Hanss, D., Mentzoni, R. A., Molde, H., & Morken, A. (2016). Omfang av penge- og dataspill- problemer i Norge 2015. Rapport for Lotteri og stiftelsestilsynet, Universitetet i Bergen, Institutt for samfunnspsykologi. Petry, N. M. (2005). Pathological gambling: Etiology, comorbidity, and treatment. American Psychological Association. https ://doi.org/10.1037/10894 -000. Petry, N. M., Blanco, C., Auriacombe, M., Borges, G., Bucholz, K., Crowley, T. J., et al. (2014). An over- view of and rationale for changes proposed for pathological gambling in DSM-5. Journal of Gambling Studies, 30(2), 493–502. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1089 9-013-9370-0. Petry, N. M., Rehbein, F., Ko, C.-H., & O’Brien, C. P. (2015). Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5. Cur- rent Psychiatry Reports, 17(9), 72. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1192 0-015-0610-0. Petry, N. M., Stinson, F. S., & Grant, B. F. (2005). Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on alcohol and related conditions. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. https ://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v66n0 504. Rehbein, F., Staudt, A., Hanslmaier, M., & Kliem, S. (2016). Video game playing in the general adult popu- lation of Germany: Can higher gaming time of males be explained by gender specific genre prefer - ences? Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 729–735. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.10.016. Rosseel, Y. (2011). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1–36. Segrin, C. (2010). Panel Design. In N. J. Salkind (Ed.), Encyclopedia of research design (pp. 990–993). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Sussman, S., Lisha, N., & Griffiths, M. (2011). Prevalence of the addictions: A problem of the majority or the minority? Evaluation & The Health Profession. 34(1), 3–56. https ://doi.org/10.1177/01632 78710 38012 4. Thege, B. K., Woodin, E. M., Hodgins, D. C., & Williams, R. J. (2015). Natural course of behavioral addictions: A 5-year longitudinal study. BMC Psychiatry, 15(1), 4. https ://doi.org/10.1186/s1288 8-015-0383-3. Toneatto, T., & Nguyen, L. (2007). Individual characteristics and problem gambling behavior. In G. Smith, D. C. Hodgins, & R. J. Williams (Eds.), Research and measurement issues in gambling studies (pp. 279–303). New York, NY: Elsevier. Walther, B., Morgenstern, M., & Hanewinkel, R. (2012). Co-occurrence of addictive behaviours: Personal- ity factors related to substance use, gambling and computer gaming. European Addiction Research, 18(4), 167–174. https ://doi.org/10.1159/00033 5662. Williams, R. J., Volberg, R. A., & Stevens, R. M. G. (2012). The population prevalence of problem gam- bling: Methodological influences, standardized rates, jurisdictional differences, and worldwide trends. Alberta: Report prepared for the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre and the Ontario Ministry of Health and Long Term Care. Williams, R. J., Hann, R. G., Schopflocher, D., West, B., McLaughlin, P., White, N., et al. (2015). Quinte longitudinal study of gambling and problem gambling. Guelph, ON: Report prepared for the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre. Wittek, C. T., Finserås, T. R., Pallesen, S., Mentzoni, R. A., Hanss, D., Griffiths, M. D., et  al. (2016). Prevalence and predictors of video game addiction: A study based on a national representative sam- ple of gamers. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 14(5), 672–686. https ://doi. org/10.1007/s1146 9-015-9592-8. 1 3 http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png Journal of Gambling Studies Springer Journals

Are Video Games a Gateway to Gambling? A Longitudinal Study Based on a Representative Norwegian Sample

Loading next page...
1
 
/lp/springer_journal/are-video-games-a-gateway-to-gambling-a-longitudinal-study-based-on-a-nMTkOwCd6r

References (56)

Publisher
Springer Journals
Copyright
Copyright © 2018 by The Author(s)
Subject
Medicine & Public Health; Psychiatry; Sociology, general; Community and Environmental Psychology; Economics, general
eISSN
1573-3602
DOI
10.1007/s10899-018-9781-z
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10899-018-9781-z ORIGINAL PAPER Are Video Games a Gateway to Gambling? A Longitudinal Study Based on a Representative Norwegian Sample 1 1 1 Helge Molde  · Bjørn Holmøy  · Aleksander Garvik Merkesdal  · 2 2 3 2 Torbjørn Torsheim  · Rune Aune Mentzoni  · Daniel Hanns  · Dominic Sagoe  · Ståle Pallesen Published online: 5 June 2018 © The Author(s) 2018 Abstract The scope and variety of video games and monetary gambling opportunities are expanding rapidly. In many ways, these forms of entertainment are converging on digi- tal and online video games and gambling sites. However, little is known about the rela- tionship between video gaming and gambling. The present study explored the possibility of a directional relationship between measures of problem gaming and problem gambling, while also controlling for the influence of sex and age. In contrast to most previous inves - tigations which are based on cross-sectional designs and non-representative samples, the present study utilized a longitudinal design conducted over 2 years (2013, 2015) and com- prising 4601 participants (males 47.2%, age range 16–74) drawn from a random sample from the general population. Video gaming and gambling were assessed using the Gaming Addiction Scale for Adolescents and the Canadian Problem Gambling Index, respectively. Using an autoregressive cross-lagged structural equation model, we found a positive rela- tionship between scores on problematic gaming and later scores on problematic gambling, whereas we found no evidence of the reverse relationship. Hence, video gaming problems appear to be a gateway behavior to problematic gambling behavior. In future research, one should continue to monitor the possible reciprocal behavioral influences between gambling and video gaming. Keywords Gambling · Video gaming · Longitudinal · Representative sample · Cross- lagged * Helge Molde helge.molde@uib.no Department of Clinical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway Department of Psychosocial Science, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway Hochschule Darmstadt, University of Applied Sciences, Darmstadt, Germany 1 3 546 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 Introduction Behavioral addictions can be understood as the excessive and continued involvement in activities that cause harm to the addict or to persons with important relationships to the addict (American Psychiatric Association 2013). The conceptualization of such addic- tions is often modeled on the diagnostic criteria for substance abuse disorders. Cur- rently, the most researched and so far only behavioral addiction recognized by formal psychiatric nosology, is gambling disorder (Petry et al. 2014). The term gambling gen- erally refers to games where the player bets money or other valuable items on an out- come that fully or partly relies on chance (Hodgins et al. 2011). Historically, gambling can be considered an almost universal feature of human societies. However, the diver- sity and accessibility of formalized gambling activities and platforms have increased dramatically during the last decades. The increased accessibility has entailed the devel- opment of massive commercial casinos and gambling destinations as well as the rising availability of online gambling. Likewise, the video gaming industry has expanded con- siderably during the past decade, similarly to the development of the gambling industry (King et al. 2010, 2015). For most people who gamble occasionally, gambling primarily represents recreation and an enjoyable activity (Back et  al. 2011). However, some people develop problem- atic patterns of gambling activity that are harmful to themselves and to society. Common characteristics of problematic gambling patterns include inappropriate amounts of time and money spent on gambling activities and the continuation of gambling activities even though gamblers’ personal relationships, financial assets, status or other important aspects of life are significantly impaired. Excessive gambling is thus associated with debts, bank - ruptcy and professional problems, as well as substantial psychological distress (Nower et al. 2015; Kessler et al. 2008). It is estimated that each problem gambler affects between five to fifteen other persons (Kalischuk et al. 2006). The estimated prevalence rates of gam- bling disorder vary depending on the specific country, survey year, and method of meas- urement; however, according to a standardized past year rate of problem gambling it ranges from 0.5 to 7.6%, with the average rate across all countries is 2.3% (Williams et al. 2012). Gambling problems are more common among some subsets of the general population, such as younger people, male gender, single or divorced/separated marital status, and low socio- economic status (Toneatto and Nguyen 2007; Petry et al. 2005). Gambling disorder is also associated with increased risk of other addictive disorders, especially alcoholism and sub- stance abuse disorder (Petry 2005). Other mental illnesses, such as mood disorders and anxiety disorders, have further been shown to have high comorbidity with gambling dis- order and comorbid mental illnesses might predispose high-risk individuals to develop or worsen pre-existing gambling problems (Kessler et al. 2008; Dowling et al. 2015). Nevertheless, little is known about the relationship between measures of problematic gambling and video gaming especially in terms of temporal associations. It is important, however, to elucidate the latter relationship for several reasons. Gaming and gambling seem increasingly to be converging. Gambling is constantly being digitized and diver- sified into a multitude of online games at the same time as video games increasingly contain themes and elements from more traditional gambling activities. Furthermore, websites offering both gaming and gambling increase dual accessibility of the activi- ties, which may cause higher levels of involvement (Fisher and Griffiths 1995). New developments such as betting on e-sport represent further examples of the increasing intertwinedness of gaming and gambling (Hutchins 2008). 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 547 While there is concern regarding how to properly distinguish gaming and gambling (King et  al. 2015), the potential for comorbidity and “recruitment” from one problem category to the other is immanent. So far, there are mixed results regarding comorbid- ity between video gaming and gambling. In a recent study with a sample of Australian video gamers, no significant relationship between frequency of video gaming and gam- bling was found (Forrest et  al. 2016). However, some studies suggest a link between involvement in gaming and gambling. For example, the frequency of visits to a non- gambling video game arcade was found to be positively associated with the frequency of disordered gambling (Ladouceur and Dubé 1995). A study of children found a posi- tive association between time spent playing video games and the likelihood to partake in risk-taking gambling (Gupta and Derevensky 1996). A study of German students aged 12–25 (Walther et al. 2012) reported a significant correlation between problematic video gaming and gambling. A recent study by McBride and Derevensky (2016) exam- ined the gambling and video game behaviors of 1229 adolescents and young adults. The results indicate that gamblers, compared with non-gamblers, were more likely to play video games and that video gamers, compared to non-gamers, were more likely to take part in gambling activities. Against this backdrop, the present study seeks to explore the directionality between problematic video gaming and gambling using a representative population sample and a longitudinal design based on assessments separated by 2 years. Method Procedure We conducted a longitudinal panel study covering a 2-year period. The first wave was carried out in 2013, the second wave in 2015. The survey was conducted on behalf of the Norwegian  Gaming and Foundation Authority and was approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Related Ethics in Western Norway (REK-Vest, project no. 2013/120). In all, 24,000 people aged 16–74 were randomly selected from the National Population Registry of Norway and invited to participate in a postal survey in 2013. A letter attached to the survey informed that all respondents would automati- cally be included in a raffle with a chance to win a gift voucher worth NOK 500. A max- imum of two reminders were sent to those who had not responded to the survey. This same procedure was used during the second wave. Participants In all, 10,081 valid answers were collected during the first wave (response rate 43.6%). These respondents were invited to participate again in 2015. In the second wave, 5809 valid answers were collected (response rate 57.6%); hence, these 5809 individuals make up the basis of this panel study. All participants included in the present study confirmed in at least one of the two waves that they had played video games during the previous 6 months and/or had participated in gambling activities during the past 12 months. This amounted to 4601 respondents, 2172 (47.2%) males and 2429 (52.8%) females, with an age range in wave 1 of 16–74 years of age (mean = 48 years, SD = 15.1). 1 3 548 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 Instruments The Gaming Addiction Scale for Adolescents (GASA) is a validated and frequently used measurement instrument. The GASA is based on previous research on video gam- ing and the biopsychosocial model of addiction (Griffiths 2005; Lemmens et al. 2009). Respondents are asked to respond to items reflecting different aspects of problematic video gaming during the previous 6  months. The response options range from never (0) to very often (4). The short version of this questionnaire contains seven items, each tapping into one of the criteria of addiction (salience, tolerance, mood modification, relapse, withdrawal, conflict and problems). We used a validated Norwegian version (Mentzoni et  al. 2011). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha was 0.83 in both waves. A composite sum score was computed for each individual participant by adding the scores on the items in the scale. Video gaming behavior was regarded as a phenome- non with a continuous severity dimension, where higher composite scores on the GASA indicate higher involvement/severity. To assess the extent of gambling problems, we administered the Canadian Problem Gambling Index (CPGI; Ferris and Wynne 2001). The CPGI consists of nine items, five of which measure problematic gambling behavior, and four measure negative conse- quences of gambling behavior. Each item is answered on a 4-point scale ranging from never (0) to always (3). Cronbach’s alpha was 0.92 in wave 1 and 0.84 in wave 2. A composite score was computed by adding the scores on the items in the scale for each individual participant. Gambling behavior was regarded as a phenomenon with a contin- uous severity dimension, where higher ordinary sum scores indicate higher involvement/ severity. Analyses To assess the unidimensionality of the CPGI and the GASA, we applied principal com- ponent analysis. The correlations among the seven items of the GASA and the nine items of the CPGI indicated that they each measure one underlying construct. We could therefore use the composite scores for both measures when conducting our main statisti- cal analyses. We used structural equation modeling (SEM) for the main analyses. To explore the correlations over time, we applied an autoregressive cross-lagged panel model using the composite scores (i.e., a model without indicators) for both video gaming and gambling (see Lemmens et  al. 2011b, for a comparable model). The lavaan package for struc- tural equation modelling in R (Rosseel 2011) was used to fit both models. Full infor - mation maximum likelihood (FIML) was applied, where the missing values were esti- mated from all available data. Robust Huber–White standard errors were applied due to skewness within the data. Because outliers could affect the correlations, we ran a model without outliers to investigate whether this was the case. The results revealed that the directional associations remained the same, and only marginal differences in strength appeared. Based on these results, we decided to use all data in our analyses. 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 549 Table 1 Descriptive statistics of GASA and CPGI in wave 1 and wave 2 Variable N Range Minimum Maximum Mean (SD) Median GASA W1 1719 23 0 23 2.56 (3.60) 1.00 GASA W2 1781 28 0 28 2.33 (3.52) 1.00 CPGI W1 3593 1 0 1 0.14 (0.35) 0.00 CPGI W2 3553 1 0 1 0.14 (0.34) 0.00 Results Descriptive and Correlational Statistics Descriptive data for the GASA and the CPGI are provided in Table 1. In the two waves, 3593 and 3533 respondents, respectively, confirmed that they had participated in gam- bling activities during the prior 12  months, and thus completed the CPGI. The num- bers of respondents who confirmed that they had played video games during the past 6 months were 1719 in wave 1 and 1781 in wave 2. In both waves, respondents generally had low scores on both the GASA and the CPGI, as indicated by the means. The means and standard deviations differed little between the first and the second wave, suggesting little change at the group level in terms of gaming and gambling problems. The variance was also similar across the two waves for both measures. Significant correlations were found between all variables, where the strongest correla- tions were between the wave 1 and wave 2 measures of the same construct. The largest coefficient was found for GASA in wave 1 and wave 2 (r = 0.60). The correlation coef- ficient for CPGI scores in wave 1 and wave 2 was r = 0.44. For more details, see Table 2. Autoregressive Cross‑Lagged Panel Analysis The results are displayed in Table  3 and Fig.  1. A strong model fit was indicated by the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA = 0.000) and Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI = 1.000). This strong model fit is due to the use of observed composite scores with no indicators. The analysis showed that the measures of gambling and video gaming Table 2 Correlation coefficients within and between the study variables in wave 1 and wave 2 GASA W1 GASA W2 CPGI W1 CPGI W2 Sex Age GASA W1 – GASA W2 0.60** – CPGI W1 0.25** 0.19** – CPGI W2 0.25** 0.19** 0.44** – Sex 0.05** 0.06** 0.09** 0.09** – Age − 0.28** − 0.30** − 0.07** − 0.10** 0.09** – Sex was coded; female = 0, male = 1 *p <0.05; **p <0.01 1 3 550 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 Table 3 Autoregressive cross- Paths Unstandardized Stand- lagged panel model: covariances, estimates ardized regression weights and model fit estimates indices Covariances  GASA W1 ↔ CPGI W1 0.26** 0.22  GASA W2 ↔ CPGI W2 0.03 0.03  Sex ↔ age 0.64** 0.08 Autoregressive paths  GASA W1 → GASA W2 0.49** 0.50  CPGI W1 → CPGI W2 0.39** 0.39 Cross-lagged paths  GASA WI → CPGI W2 0.02** 0.15  CPGI W1 → GASA W2 0.45 0.05 Control variables  Sex → GASA W1 0.54** 0.08  Sex → GASA W2 0.07 0.01  Sex → CPGI W1 0.07** 0.10  Sex → CPGI W2 0.04** 0.06  Age → GASA W1 − 0.07** − 0.29  Age → GASA W2 − 0.03** − 0.14  Age → CPGI W1 − 0.00 ** − 0.09  Age → CPGI W2 − 0.00** − 0.05 Sex was coded; female = 0, male = 1 *p <0.05; **p <0.01 .50** GASA W 2 GASA W 1 .22** CPGI W 1 CPGI W 2 .39** Fig. 1 Configural model of the cross-lagged panel analysis. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 551 problems had a statistically significant positive covariation (β = 0.22, p <0.01) in wave 1. In wave 2, no such relationship was found (β = 0.03, p <0.36). As shown by the hori- zontal line in Fig. 1, scores on the GASA in wave 1 significantly predicted scores on the GASA in wave 2 (β = 0.50, p < 0.01). CPGI scores in wave 1 also significantly predicted CPGI scores in wave 2 (β = 0.39, p < 0.01). In addition, we investigated the effects of sex and age on the GASA and CPGI. Anal- ysis revealed that men reported higher total scores on the GASA compared to women in wave 1 (β = 0.08, p < 0.01); at the second wave, this relationship was no longer signifi- cant (β = 0.01, p < 0.65). Men reported higher scores on the CPGI compared to women in both waves (wave 1: β = 0.10, p < 0.01; wave 2: β = 0.06, p < 0.01). Age was found to be significantly and negatively related to the GASA scores in both waves (wave 1: β = − 0.29, p < 0.01; wave 2: β = − 0.14, p < 0.01). The same tendency, albeit smaller, was found for scores on the CPGI in both waves (wave 1: β = − 0.09, p = 0.01; wave 2: β = − 0.05, p < 0.05). Hence, the results indicate that younger respondents reported higher scores on both the GASA and the CPGI measures in both waves. The cross-lagged analyses also revealed that the scores on the GASA in wave 1 sig- nificantly predicted scores on the CPGI in wave 2 (β = 0.15, p < 0.01). No such rela- tionship was found between scores derived from the CPGI in wave 1 and scores on the GASA in wave 2 (β = 0.05, p < 0.12). This shows that a measure of video gaming prob- lems is positively related to a measure of gambling problems at a later time, while the reverse relationship is not statistically significant. Estimates of intercepts and variance for the variables are provided in Table 4. They show higher scores on both measures in wave 1. Table 4 Autoregressive cross- Unstandardized SE Stand- lagged panel model: intercepts estimates ardized and variance estimates Intercepts  GASA W1 4.45** 0.33 1.26  GASA W2 2.21** 0.35 0.65  CPGI W1 0.15** 0.03 0.42  CPGI W2 0.06 0.03 0.16  Sex 1.47** 0.01 2.95  Age 50.00** 0.22 3.30 Variance  GASA W1 11.33** 0.66 0.92  GASA W2 7.99** 0.66 0.68  CPGI W1 0.12** 0.00 0.99  CPGI W2 0.09** 0.00 0.78  Sex 0.25** 0.00 1.00  Age 229.57** 3.74 1.00 Sex was coded; female = 0, male = 1 **p < 0.01 1 3 552 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 Discussion A finding consistent with the research literature in general is that there were positive cor - relations between measures of the same construct over time, suggesting some stability over the 2-year period (Williams et al. 2015). As for the relationship between gaming and gambling, we found a significant positive correlation between a score on the gaming problem scale and gambling at the first wave. However, this was not the case in the second wave, after controlling for the influence of sex and age. This indicates an inconsistency over time in the relationship between gambling and video gaming, which seems to be plausible given the mixed results on the strength of this relationship and the somewhat unstable and transitional nature of gambling and video gaming problems (LaPlante et al. 2008; Kuss et al. 2014; Thege et al. 2015). Another pos- sible and perhaps complementary explanation for this finding may be that our sample had aged 2 years, which may have further reduced an initially weak correlation. This result may be seen in light of one of the most consistent findings in video game research—namely, that younger age is strongly correlated with more frequent playing (Ferguson et al. 2011; Toneatto and Nguyen, 2007). Perhaps most interestingly, our results show that scores on the gaming problem scale at wave 1 predicted the scores on the gambling problem scale at wave 2, but there was no evidence of the reverse relationship. This suggests that video gaming constituted a risk fac- tor for gambling 2 years later. Studies exploring the potential causal relationship between video gaming and gambling based on longitudinal designs and representative samples are very rare. The few existing studies generally show evidence of increased gambling among high-frequency video gamers (McBride and Derevensky 2016). However, there are mixed results regarding the strength of this relationship and whether extraneous variables are likely to mediate heightened involvement in both gaming and gambling (Delfabbro et  al. 2009). Results from the current study suggest and support a direct and causal relation- ship between video gaming and gambling. One possible explanation for the relationship between problematic gaming and gambling is that the participants had become 2  years older. This could indicate that a significant subset of those involved with gaming developed an involvement with gambling during the transition. As age restrictions apply more strictly and widely to gambling activities than to video gaming, most people who eventually start to gamble are introduced to video games at an earlier age. It is thus conceivable that those who follow a path progressing from problematic gaming to gambling are generally prone to addictive behaviors and that the manifestation is moderated by age (Sussman et al. 2011). Another set of factors possibly augmenting such a trajectory is the increasing convergence between video gaming and gambling (King et  al. 2010). This trend manifests itself, for example, in an increasing number of gambling products that adopt features from video games and also more video games containing intrinsic gambling themes (King et al. 2012, 2014; Walther et  al. 2012). Furthermore, advertisements and game-related incentives are substantial for enticing players to games that involve monetary stakes and outcomes (King et al. 2015; Gainsbury et al. 2014b). In addition, formal and informal gambling has become a large part of e-sports (Holden et al. 2016). It is thus conceivable that all these factors—age related transitions, proneness to addic- tive behavior, digital convergence and strategic marketing—may explain why this particu- lar directionality was found. Some previous studies indicate that factors such as physical proximity of video gaming and gambling opportunities could lead to an increased number of problem gamblers, and a similar type of exposure effect could potentially operate in 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 553 the world of digital and online games (Fisher and Griffiths 1995; Gainsbury et al. 2014a; Ladouceur and Dubé 1995). Keeping in mind that such a particular pathway is unlikely to apply to the majority of problem gamblers, it would still be very informative for research, clinical practice, and prevention agencies to be aware of the possibility that subgroups of gamers may be prone to developing gambling problems in line with the mechanisms/path- ways outlined here. Being male predicted higher scores on the CPGI in both waves. The same was found for GASA in the first wave, which could be expected from previous investigations (Petry et  al. 2015; Rehbein et  al. 2016). This was not the case in the second wave, as gender was found to be unrelated for GASA. The interpretation of this latter finding is that there are no gender differences beyond the association controlled for in wave 1. The finding of male preponderance and increased involvement in gambling and/or gaming are in accord- ance with previous research (Pallesen et al. 2016; Wittek et al. 2016). Still, attitudes toward video gaming activities seem to be changing, possibly due to more widespread acceptance, less restrictive stereotypes of gamers and increased interest on the part of females in video games (Ipsos MediaCT 2012). This is likely to be influenced by the development of a wider range of games, many of which are designed to be appealing to target groups that deviate far beyond the traditional “profile” of a teenage male video gamer. Some recent evidence also indicates that choice of gaming device is relevant, as Pallesen et al. (2016) found that females are more likely than males to play games through social media. Strengths and Limitations The findings from the present study have several important theoretical and potentially prac- tical implications. Generally, the research literature in the field of problematic video gam- ing is too heavily focused on narrow subsets of the population, that is, people who are known to be problematic gamers or who represent subgroups considered more likely to develop such problems. Such convenience and strategic sampling is insufficient to under - stand the extent and characteristics of gamers in general. In contrast, our sample was rep- resentative of the general population. In order to deepen our understanding of and to refine our efforts to deal effectively with both video gaming and gambling, representative sam- ples need be used from which generalizable results can be obtained. In addition to the large and representative sample, the validity of our conclusions is reinforced by the fact that the response rate was relatively high and that the distribution of male and female respond- ents was fairly even. Another asset of the current study is that it employed a longitudinal design that models the directions of relationships between variables. This allowed us to indicate which type of addictive behavior is predictive of the other, and how each behavior is related to potential confounding variables. The overall novelty of the field of video gam- ing research and the relative lack of studies that explicitly explore the association between video gaming and gambling make the current findings and research approach informative and relevant for future investigations. In term of limitations, it should be noted that all data were self-reported. Such data are susceptible to biased self-descriptions, limitations in recollection and other sources of inac- curate reporting (Arnold and Feldman 1981). As the age of the current sample ranged from 16 to 74, it can be questioned whether this instrument, which was developed and validated for use on adolescents (Lemmens et al. 2009), was suitable for the current sample. How- ever, studies have shown that the GASA has good psychometric properties when applied to adult samples (Andreassen et al. 2016; Festl et al. 2013). 1 3 554 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 One important methodological concern relates to the longitudinal design of the study, more specifically, the appropriateness of choosing two points of measurement separated by 2  years for exploring the relationship between the variables. It has been argued that due to the phenomenon of regression towards the mean, a minimum of three waves of measurement are needed to confidently draw causal inferences from longitudinal data (Segrin 2010). It should be noted, that several comparable studies in the field of video gaming and gambling have employed two points of measurement over even more limited periods (Lemmens et al. 2011a, b). One might expect that phenom- enon to be flexible, innovative and ever changing as video gaming and gambling are more subject to change over brief periods of time. Therefore, there seems to be a high demand for studies that can detect transitions occurring within relatively short time periods. Another possible methodological limitation relates to the chosen method of analysis, as cross-lagged models have been criticized for presuming multiple assump- tions that are unlikely to be met in reality (Segrin 2010). The majority of respondents in this study were neither problematic gamers nor problematic gamblers but the scales used in the present study represent continuous scores of problematic gaming and gam- bling. Thus, the present study should not be taken as categorical evidence that problem gamers develop into problem gamblers, but rather that the level of problem gaming is associated with a subsequent level of problem gambling, with the known caveat that much of the observed variation occurred on the lower end of both measures. Whether or not problem gamers as a category are at increased risk of developing subsequent problem gambling is a question that should be addressed in future research. Implications and Directions for Future Research Because only a few studies have explicitly compared problematic video gaming and gambling, especially with representative samples and longitudinal designs, the rela- tionship between these variables remains largely unclear. This highlights the point made by some authors that a focus solely on problematic video gaming or gambling might limit our understanding of these phenomena, as some research indicates that video gaming and gambling can yield some positive effects for its users as well (Granic et  al. 2014). Broadening the interest in this manner might expand our understanding of the differences between normal and pathological involvement, and the causes and trajectories involved when these lines are crossed. Therefore, it would seem that more longitudinal studies based on representative samples are warranted. One factor we did not explore was the influence of types of games played, which seems to be an important nuance, especially when considering male and female gam- bling preferences. In a similar vein, one might also suspect that the strong relationship between young age and video gaming and gambling will lessen with time. Such a trend would be expected due to the likely cohort-effects involved in usage of technological entertainment and developmental matureness (e.g., biological), but could be hastened by increased competence within older cohorts and improved efforts from developers to reach wider audiences. Given the massive public interest in, availability of, and increasing use of the Internet, it seems highly plausible that the groups of people who are involved in video gaming as well as the ways in which video games are played will continue to expand and diversify. 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 555 Conclusion The present study explored the prospective relationships between video gaming and gam- bling in a representative sample of the general Norwegian population using two waves of data collection spaced by an interval of 2  years. The results indicated that scores on the problem gaming scale predicted scores on the problem gambling scale, whereas the reverse relationship was not found. The scores on the two instruments predicted their respective scores on the same measure 2  years later, suggesting some consistency of measures of gaming and gambling problems. Given that both the variety and diversity of video gam- ing and gambling are rapidly developing, changing and expanding, one should continue to monitor the impact that involvement in either of the two behaviors has on the other, in both the short and the long term. Compliance with Ethical Standards Conflict of interest The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest. Ethical Approval All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and the national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Inter- national License (http://creat iveco mmons .org/licen ses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. References American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5 ). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association. Andreassen, C. S., Billieux, J., Griffiths, M. D., Kuss, D. J., Demetrovics, Z., Mazzoni, E., et  al. (2016). The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional study. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 30(2), 252. https :// doi.org/10.1037/adb00 00160 . Arnold, H. J., & Feldman, D. C. (1981). Social desirability response bias in self-report choice situations. Academy of Management Journal, 24(2), 377–385. https ://doi.org/10.2307/25584 8. Back, K.-J., Lee, C.-K., & Stinchfield, R. (2011). Gambling motivation and passion: A comparison study of recreational and pathological gamblers. Journal of Gambling Studies, 27(3), 355–370. https ://doi. org/10.1007/s1089 9-010-9212-2. Delfabbro, P., King, D., Lambos, C., & Puglies, S. (2009). Is video-game playing a risk factor for patho- logical gambling in Australian adolescents? Journal of Gambling Studies, 25(3), 391–405. https ://doi. org/10.1007/s1089 9-009-9138-8. Dowling, N. A., Cowlishaw, S., Jackson, A. C., Merkouris, S. S., Francis, K. L., & Christensen, D. R. (2015). The prevalence of comorbid personality disorders in treatment-seeking problem gamblers: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Personality Disorders, 29(6), 735–754. https ://doi. org/10.1521/pedi_2014_28_168. Ferguson, C. J., Coulson, M., & Barnett, J. (2011). A meta-analysis of pathological gaming prevalence and comorbidity with mental health, academic and social problems. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(12), 1573–1578. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsyc hires .2011.09.005. Ferris, J., & Wynne, H. (2001). The Canadian problem gambling index. Ottawa, ON: Canadian Centre on Substance Abuse. Festl, R., Scharkow, M., & Quandt, T. (2013). Problematic computer game use among adolescents, younger and older adults. Addiction, 108(3), 592–599. https ://doi.org/10.1111/add.12016 . 1 3 556 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 Fisher, S., & Griffiths, M. (1995). Current trends in slot machine gambling: Research and policy issues. Journal of Gambling Studies, 11(3), 239–247. https ://doi.org/10.1007/BF021 04791 . Forrest, C. J., King, D. L., & Delfabbro, P. H. (2016). The gambling preferences and behaviors of a commu- nity sample of Australian regular video game players. Journal of Gambling Studies, 32(2), 409–420. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1089 9-015-9535-0. Gainsbury, S. M., Hing, N., Delfabbro, P. H., & King, D. L. (2014a). A taxonomy of gambling and casino games via social media and online technologies. International Gambling Studies, 14(2), 196–213. https ://doi.org/10.1080/14459 795.2014.89063 4. Gainsbury, S. M., Russell, A., Hing, N., Wood, R., Lubman, D. I., & Blaszczynski, A. (2014b). The prevalence and determinants of problem gambling in Australia: Assessing the impact of interac- tive gambling and new technologies. Psychology of Addictive Behaviors, 28(3), 769. https ://doi. org/10.1037/a0036 207. Granic, I., Lobel, A., & Engels, R. C. (2014). The benefits of playing video games. American Psycholo- gist, 69(1), 66. https ://doi.org/10.1037/a0034 857. Griffiths, M. (2005). A ‘components’ model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework. Journal of Substance Use, 10(4), 191–197. https ://doi.org/10.1080/14659 89050 01143 59. Gupta, R., & Derevensky, J. L. (1996). The relationship between gambling and video-game playing behavior in children and adolescents. Journal of Gambling Studies, 12(4), 375–394. https ://doi. org/10.1007/BF015 39183 . Hodgins, D. C., Stea, J. N., & Grant, J. E. (2011). Gambling disorders. The Lancet, 378(9806), 1874– 1884. https ://doi.org/10.1016/S0140 -6736(10)62185 -X. Holden, J. T., Rodenberg, R. M., & Kaburakis, A. (2016). Esports corruption: Gambling, doping, and global governance. Maryland Journal of International Law, 32(1), 236–273. Hutchins, B. (2008). Signs of meta-change in second modernity: The growth of e-sport and the World Cyber Games. New Media & Society, 10, 851–869. Ipsos MediaCT. (2012). Videogames in Europe: Consumer study. European summary report. Retrieved from the Interactive Software Federation of Europe website: http://www.isfe.eu/sites /isfe.eu/files / attac hment s/euro_summa ry_-_isfe_consu mer_study .pdf. Retrieved 9 June 2017. Kalischuk, R. G., Nowatzki, N., Cardwell, K., Klein, K., & Solowoniuk, J. (2006). Problem gambling and its impact on families: A literature review. International Gambling Studies, 6, 31–60. Kessler, R. C., Hwang, I., LaBrie, R., Petukhova, M., Sampson, N. A., Winters, K. C., et  al. (2008). DSM-IV pathological gambling in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Psychological Medicine, 38(09), 1351–1360. https ://doi.org/10.1017/S0033 29170 80029 00. King, D., Delfabbro, P., & Griffiths, M. (2010). The convergence of gambling and digital media: Impli- cations for gambling in young people. Journal of Gambling Studies, 26(2), 175–187. https ://doi. org/10.1007/s1089 9-009-9153-9. King, D. L., Delfabbro, P. H., Kaptsis, D., & Zwaans, T. (2014). Adolescent simulated gambling via digital and social media: An emerging problem. Computers in Human Behavior, 31, 305–313. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.048. King, D. L., Ejova, A., & Delfabbro, P. H. (2012). Illusory control, gambling, and video gaming: An investigation of regular gamblers and video game players. Journal of Gambling Studies, 28(3), 421–435. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1089 9-011-9271-z. King, D. L., Gainsbury, S. M., Delfabbro, P. H., Hing, N., & Abarbanel, B. (2015). Distinguishing between gaming and gambling activities in addiction research. Journal of Behavioral Addictions, 4(4), 215–220. https ://doi.org/10.1556/2006.4.2015.045. Kuss, D. J., Griffiths, M. D., Karila, L., & Billieux, J. (2014). Internet addiction: A systematic review of epidemiological research for the last decade. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(25), 4026–4052. https ://doi.org/10.2174/13816 12811 31999 90617 . Ladouceur, R., & Dubé, D. (1995). Prevalence of pathological gambling and associated problems in individuals who visit non-gambling video arcades. Journal of Gambling Studies, 11(4), 361–365. https ://doi.org/10.1007/BF021 08614 . LaPlante, D. A., Nelson, S. E., LaBrie, R. A., & Shaffer, H. J. (2008). Stability and progression of dis- ordered gambling: Lessons from longitudinal studies. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 53(1), 52–60. https ://doi.org/10.1177/07067 43708 05300 108. Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2009). Development and validation of a game addiction scale for adolescents. Media Psychology, 12(1), 77–95. https ://doi.org/10.1080/15213 26080 26694 Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2011a). The effects of pathological gaming on aggres- sive behavior. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 40(1), 38–47. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1096 4-010-9558-x. 1 3 J Gambl Stud (2019) 35:545–557 557 Lemmens, J. S., Valkenburg, P. M., & Peter, J. (2011b). Psychosocial causes and consequences of pathological gaming. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(1), 144–152. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j. chb.2010.07.015. McBride, J., & Derevensky, J. (2016). Gambling and video game playing among youth. Journal of Gam- bling Issues. https ://doi.org/10.4309/jgi.2016.34.9. Mentzoni, R. A., Brunborg, G. S., Molde, H., Myrseth, H., Skouverøe, K. J. M., Hetland, J., et al. (2011). Problematic video game use: Estimated prevalence and associations with mental and physical health. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 14(10), 591–596. https ://doi.org/10.1089/cyber .2010.0260. Nower, L., Eyrich-Garg, K. M., Pollio, D. E., & North, C. S. (2015). Problem gambling and homeless- ness: Results from an epidemiologic study. Journal of Gambling Studies, 31(2), 533–545. https ://doi. org/10.1007/s1089 9-013-9435-0. Pallesen, S., Hanss, D., Mentzoni, R. A., Molde, H., & Morken, A. (2016). Omfang av penge- og dataspill- problemer i Norge 2015. Rapport for Lotteri og stiftelsestilsynet, Universitetet i Bergen, Institutt for samfunnspsykologi. Petry, N. M. (2005). Pathological gambling: Etiology, comorbidity, and treatment. American Psychological Association. https ://doi.org/10.1037/10894 -000. Petry, N. M., Blanco, C., Auriacombe, M., Borges, G., Bucholz, K., Crowley, T. J., et al. (2014). An over- view of and rationale for changes proposed for pathological gambling in DSM-5. Journal of Gambling Studies, 30(2), 493–502. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1089 9-013-9370-0. Petry, N. M., Rehbein, F., Ko, C.-H., & O’Brien, C. P. (2015). Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5. Cur- rent Psychiatry Reports, 17(9), 72. https ://doi.org/10.1007/s1192 0-015-0610-0. Petry, N. M., Stinson, F. S., & Grant, B. F. (2005). Comorbidity of DSM-IV pathological gambling and other psychiatric disorders: Results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on alcohol and related conditions. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry. https ://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v66n0 504. Rehbein, F., Staudt, A., Hanslmaier, M., & Kliem, S. (2016). Video game playing in the general adult popu- lation of Germany: Can higher gaming time of males be explained by gender specific genre prefer - ences? Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 729–735. https ://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.10.016. Rosseel, Y. (2011). lavaan: An R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 1–36. Segrin, C. (2010). Panel Design. In N. J. Salkind (Ed.), Encyclopedia of research design (pp. 990–993). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Sussman, S., Lisha, N., & Griffiths, M. (2011). Prevalence of the addictions: A problem of the majority or the minority? Evaluation & The Health Profession. 34(1), 3–56. https ://doi.org/10.1177/01632 78710 38012 4. Thege, B. K., Woodin, E. M., Hodgins, D. C., & Williams, R. J. (2015). Natural course of behavioral addictions: A 5-year longitudinal study. BMC Psychiatry, 15(1), 4. https ://doi.org/10.1186/s1288 8-015-0383-3. Toneatto, T., & Nguyen, L. (2007). Individual characteristics and problem gambling behavior. In G. Smith, D. C. Hodgins, & R. J. Williams (Eds.), Research and measurement issues in gambling studies (pp. 279–303). New York, NY: Elsevier. Walther, B., Morgenstern, M., & Hanewinkel, R. (2012). Co-occurrence of addictive behaviours: Personal- ity factors related to substance use, gambling and computer gaming. European Addiction Research, 18(4), 167–174. https ://doi.org/10.1159/00033 5662. Williams, R. J., Volberg, R. A., & Stevens, R. M. G. (2012). The population prevalence of problem gam- bling: Methodological influences, standardized rates, jurisdictional differences, and worldwide trends. Alberta: Report prepared for the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre and the Ontario Ministry of Health and Long Term Care. Williams, R. J., Hann, R. G., Schopflocher, D., West, B., McLaughlin, P., White, N., et al. (2015). Quinte longitudinal study of gambling and problem gambling. Guelph, ON: Report prepared for the Ontario Problem Gambling Research Centre. Wittek, C. T., Finserås, T. R., Pallesen, S., Mentzoni, R. A., Hanss, D., Griffiths, M. D., et  al. (2016). Prevalence and predictors of video game addiction: A study based on a national representative sam- ple of gamers. International Journal of Mental Health and Addiction, 14(5), 672–686. https ://doi. org/10.1007/s1146 9-015-9592-8. 1 3

Journal

Journal of Gambling StudiesSpringer Journals

Published: Jun 5, 2018

There are no references for this article.