Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 14-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

Towards a Translational Approach to Food Addiction: Implications for Bulimia Nervosa

Towards a Translational Approach to Food Addiction: Implications for Bulimia Nervosa Purpose of Review In recent years, the food addiction hypothesis of loss-of-control eating has gained traction in the field of eating disorders. In particular, the neural process of food addiction plays a dominant role in the recently formulated “addictive appetite” model of bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder. Nonetheless, several components of the food addiction hypothesis, including the presence of withdrawal and tolerance effects, as well as the proposition that some foods possess “addicting” properties, remain highly controversial. In response, the current review synthesises existing evidence for withdrawal and tolerance effects in people with bulimia nervosa. Recent Findings The recent development of a validated tool to measure withdrawal from highly processed foods will aid in measuring withdrawal symptoms and testing hypotheses related to withdrawal in the context of food addiction. We subsequently describe preclinical and human evidence for a central insulin- and dopamine-mediated pathway by which recurrent loss-of- control binge eating is maintained in bulimia nervosa. Summary Evidence in populations with bulimia nervosa and loss-of-control eating provides preliminary support for the role of food addiction in the maintenance of bulimia nervosa. Future longitudinal research is needed to develop a clearer profile of illness progression and to clarify the extent to which dysregulation in glucose metabolism contributes to food craving and symptom maintenance in bulimia nervosa. . . . . Keywords Food addiction Bulimia nervosa Eating disorders Sugar Dopamine Background In this article, we will use bulimia nervosa (BN) as the exem- plar. The “Addictive Appetite Model” proposes that three pri- The concept of “food addiction” has received increasing at- mary processes maintain psychopathology in BN: (1) the high tention in the scientific literature of recent years. While cogent salience of palatable foods [3], which is moderated by a ge- arguments have been made against the establishment of food netic susceptibility to food approach tendencies, reduced effi- addiction as a psychiatric diagnosis in its own right [1], there ciency in satiation processes [4] and/or episodes of food re- is substantial evidence to suggest that processes similar to striction; (2) chronic stress and interpersonal difficulties those observed in substance abuse disorders play a significant resulting in a deficiency of alternative rewards and a primed role in the maintenance of eating disorders in which loss of stress system [5]; and (3) large swings in blood glucose, control of eating is a feature (e.g. anorexia nervosa-binge caused by the consumption of foods with a high glycaemic purge type, bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder) [2� ]. index, self-induced vomiting or insulin resistance (and insulin omission in diabetes mellitus). These pathways may contrib- ute to compulsive binge eating behaviour through aberrations This article is part of the Topical Collection on Food Addiction in dopaminergic function in a similar way to substance addictions. * Monica Leslie The current review presents a synthesis of the literature monica.leslie@kcl.ac.uk investigating some of the controversial aspects of applying the food addiction paradigm to eating disorders. For instance, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience (IoPPN), there is uncertainty as to whether tolerance and withdrawal Section of Eating Disorders, King’s College London (KCL), 103 criteria for an addictive disorder as specified within the Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 259 DSM-5 are met. We also present a synthesis of molecular, consumption [13], a finding which may be of particular im- preclinical, clinical and neuroimaging evidence illustrating portance to BN as it is characterised by intermittent fasting how fluxes in glucose and insulin moderate central dopami- and binge episodes [14]. A similar downregulation of dopa- nergic functioning. mine D2 receptors is found in humans addicted to drugs of abuse [15] and is thought to be a key driver of compensatory overconsumption in the Reward Hyposensitivity Theory [16, Does Bulimia Nervosa Meet DSM-5 Criteria 17]. Behavioural observations in people with substance use for an Addictive Disorder? disorders mirror these findings. Individuals with BN endorse higher levels of tolerance-like The DSM-5 criteria for an addictive disorder are presented in symptoms measured using the Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS) [8], compared with healthy controls [18, 19]. There Table 1. The extent to which BN meets these criteria has been reviewed extensively elsewhere [6]. In his 2014 review, are clinical reports of subthreshold BN patients initiating larg- er and more frequent binge episodes over time [20]. Brewerton acknowledges significant phenotypic overlap be- tween disorders of substance dependence and BN but high- Consistent with these accounts is cross-sectional evidence of the correlation between higher body weight and frequency and lights a paucity of systematic clinical evidence for tolerance and withdrawal in the latter, a point frequently cited as a major severity of binge eating episodes [21]. Individuals with binge- weakness of the food addiction hypothesis [7]. Much research type eating disorders endorse significantly greater levels of on tolerance and withdrawal symptoms in humans to date has eating for purposes of reward enhancement compared with been largely anecdotal [8]. However, herein we synthesise the weight-matched controls [22]. Such evidence, although com- evidence from new assessment methods. pelling, remains indirect and is insufficient to prove the exis- tence of tolerance in humans. A preference for intensely sweet food and larger quan- Tolerance tities of sweeteners in individuals with BN is a character- istic often presented as an indicator of tolerance [3, The most compelling evidence for food tolerance is demon- strated in animal models, as previously reviewed by Murray 23–25]. Furthermore, this preference remains after ingesting a glucose load [26]. Magnetic resonance imag- et al. [9]. Rats who voluntarily overeat highly palatable food exhibit evidence of a neural reward deficit due to downregu- ing (MRI) studies indicate hypofunctioning of gustatory and limbic circuitry in BN patients when tasting palatable lated dopamine D2 receptors, which worsens as weight is gained [10–12]. This decreased sensitivity to reward is direct- food compared with controls [27, 28] and compared with individuals recovered from BN [29, 30]. This evidence is ly linked to the onset of compulsive food seeking in rats [11]. Repetitive bingeing on sucrose interspersed with periods of consistent with the idea that individuals with BN ingest dietary restriction causes rats to triple their overall daily sugar more food over time because of a decreased sensitivity to Table 1 DSM-5 criteria for an addictive disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) Criteria 1 The substance is often taken in larger amounts or over a longer period than was intended Criteria 2 There is a persistent desire or unsuccessful efforts to cut down or control use of the substance Criteria 3 A great deal of time is spent in activities necessary to obtain the substance, use the substance or recover from its effects Criteria 4 Craving or a strong desire or urge to use the substance Criteria 5 Recurrent use of the substance despite having persistent or recurrent social or interpersonal problems caused or exacerbated by the effects of its use Criteria 6 Continued use of the substance despite having persistent or recurrent social or interpersonal problems caused or exacerbated by the effects of its use Criteria 7 Important social, occupational, or recreational activities are given up or reduced because of use of the substance Criteria 8 Recurrent use of the substance in situations in which it is physically hazardous Criteria 9 Tolerance, as defined by either of the following: A. A need for markedly increased amounts of the substance to achieve intoxication or desired effect B. A markedly diminished effect with continued use of the same amount of the substance Criteria 10 Withdrawal, as manifested by either of the following: A. The characteristic withdrawal syndrome for other substance B. The substance (or a closely related substance) is taken to relieve or avoid withdrawal symptoms Bold text indicate criteria for an addictive disorder which are discussed in depth in the current manuscript with reference to their relevance to bulimia nervosa 260 Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 sweet taste resulting from repeated bingeing on abstaining from salted food [49]. Furthermore, tiredness and hyperpalatable foods [27]. irritability have been cited as motivating factors for eating The development of impaired satiety mechanisms may be [45], providing some suggestion of food being used as a an indirect indicator of tolerance [6, 31, 32]. For example, a “pick-me-up”, or to avoid experiencing negative feelings recent functional MRI (fMRI) study found that women in stemming from withdrawal. remission from BN exhibited the same response to taste stim- Symptoms of psychological withdrawal are also widely uli in brain regions implicated in translating sensory informa- reported. Cross-sectional self-report accounts from patients tion about taste into motivated behaviour, regardless of wheth- with BN reveal that most feel tension, loneliness and physical er the individuals were hungry or sated, whilst healthy con- symptoms of anxiety before a binge, and the majority feel that trols showed an increased response to taste stimuli when hun- their negative psychological states are alleviated whilst en- gry versus when fed [33]. The authors also found an increased gaged in a binge [39� ]. Longitudinal studies using amygdala response in their remitted BN sample when fed Ecological Momentary Assessment technology also report compared to healthy controls, which they propose might pro- that binge eating and subsequent purging are usually preceded ject to the hypothalamus and motivate eating in the absence of by dysphoric mood states [50–52]. However, there is high hunger [33, 34]. It is possible that brain circuitry in BN fails to prevalence of depression and emotion dysregulation in BN de-value food reward when in a fed state, leading to eating populations [53], so it is not clear whether such presentations beyond metabolic need. of low mood represent psychological withdrawal from food. Five years on from Brewerton’s 2014 review, there remains Future research should aim to elucidate whether dysphoric a paucity of direct evidence of tolerance in humans in relation mood states before bingeing are distinct from more permanent to food intake and prospective, longitudinal studies are mood-related comorbidities, perhaps by comparing depressed needed. versus non-depressed individuals with BN. The first and only tool to evaluate withdrawal in the context Withdrawal Syndromes of addictive-like eating has been developed recently: the Highly Processed Food Withdrawal Scale—ProWS [39� ]. Preliminary evidence for a withdrawal syndrome in relation- This in part is derived from the premise that specific nutrition- ship to palatable food comes from animal models. There are al ingredients are capable of triggering addictive-like re- consistent observations of strong physical (e.g. forepaw trem- sponses [54]. In a pilot study, the ProWS was found to be or, teeth chattering) and psychological (e.g. aggression, anxi- positively associated with elevated YFAS symptoms, BMI ety) withdrawal responses in rats during periods of withdrawal and weight cycling in a community sample, and responses from sucrose [35–37]. The same observations, however, are on the ProWS explained an additional 11.2% of the variance not found with removal of high-fat foods [38] and have not yet in self-reported dieting success [39� ]. This tool may help dif- been studied with removal of highly processed foods [39� ]. ferentiate between the withdrawal effects from different types Neuroimaging studies show patterns consistent with this be- of palatable food [55, 56]. havioural data. Sugar-dependent rats show a significant in- crease in extracellular acetylcholine and a decrease in dopa- mine release in the nucleus accumbens shell, as compared to The Impact of Glucose Metabolism control groups, during a 36-h period of food deprivation [35], on Hedonic Eating Behaviour effects which are similar to withdrawal from morphine, nico- tine and alcohol. Preclinical Evidence Traditionally, withdrawal symptoms have not been clearly defined in the context of addictive-like eating, prompting crit- Sweet, pa latable foods act as unconditioned rewarding stimuli icism of the food addiction framework [40]. To date, the food in humans and rodent models, with evidence suggesting that addiction field has largely relied on observational and anec- merely tasting sucrose without digestion produces activation dotal clinical reports based on small cohorts or single case of dopaminergic circuits within the striatum [57]. However, studies [41–46] and on self-reported endorsement of with- there is evidence to suggest that palatable foods with a high drawal symptoms on the YFAS [18] and other withdrawal glycaemic index further contribute to the development of scales [47]. Cross-sectional self-report accounts are consistent compulsive binge eating behaviour through changes in dopa- in describing physiological symptoms of withdrawal similar minergic functioning triggered by wide swings in blood glu- to those experienced during opiate withdrawal [8, 48]. cose. One candidate mechanism for this effect relates to the Headaches, irritability and flu-like symptoms are reported by interaction between insulin and dopaminergic functioning. individuals abstaining from sugar [42], stomach pains, muscle The role of mesolimbic dopaminergic functioning in food spasms and shakiness by individuals abstaining or reducing approach behaviours has been reviewed extensively else- intake of carbohydrates [43, 45] and nausea by individuals where [58]. Dopamine-deficient mouse models exhibit severe Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 261 aphagia leading to weight loss and death [59]. Conversely, the threshold for intracranial self-stimulation, thus indicating a stimulation of dopaminergic activity within the striatum trig- reduction of reward functioning. gers food consumption in rats without enhancing “liking” re- sponses (e.g. lip-licking and paw licking). Thus, dopaminergic Evidence in Humans functioning is thought to hold a role in food approach behav- iours (wanting) that is discrete from the hedonic response to Insulin resistance impacts on central dopaminergic systems in food receipt [60]. In contrast, central insulin suppresses feed- humans. For example, Dunn et al. [71] conducted a positron ing [61]. It is thought that the effects of central insulin and emission tomography (PET) study using the dopamine D2/D3 dopamine on food intake are not independent but rather inter- receptor radioligand [18F] fallypride and found that insulin act to regulate hedonic eating behaviour. sensitivity is negatively correlated with dopamine type 2 re- Dopaminergic neurons within the ventral tegmental area ceptor availability in the ventral striatum in a heterogeneous (VTA) express insulin receptors [62, 63], presenting a possible sample of lean and obese women. In men, Anthony et al. [72] mechanism by which insulin might influence the dopaminer- found that exogenously administered insulin increases metab- gic induction of feeding behaviour. Furthermore, central insu- olism in the ventral striatum and prefrontal cortex, whilst de- lin enhances the expression of dopamine transporter protein creasing metabolism in the right amygdala, hippocampus and within the VTA via a protein kinase B (Akt) signalling system cerebellar vermis. Furthermore, the effect of insulin in increas- [64, 65]. The enhanced expression of dopamine transporters ing metabolism in the ventral striatum and prefrontal cortex on the cell surface induced by insulin exposure is associated was lower in insulin-resistant versus insulin-sensitive partici- with greater dopamine uptake [66], thus reducing levels of pants [72]. This pattern of findings is thus indicative of trait- synaptic dopamine. level differences in the effects of central insulin on dopami- With regard to the effects of insulin on postsynaptic dopa- nergic mesolimbic regions, known to be critical for food crav- minergic signalling, in vitro studies have found that insulin ing and food approach behaviour [59, 73]. exposure invokes long-term depression of excitatory signal- Interactions between insulin resistance and central do- ling within VTA dopamine neurons extracted from male paminergic functioning may have functional significance C57BL/6J mice [67]. This effect appears to be long-lasting for food craving in humans. Chechlacz et al. [74], in an as, once induced, the long-term depression of VTA dopamine fMRI study, found that people with type II diabetes cells is not reversed by application of the insulin receptor mellitus (characterised by insulin resistance) exhibit great- antagonist S961 or through a tyrosine kinase inhibitor, which er blood oxygenated level dependent (BOLD) response to suppresses insulin receptor functioning [67]. food versus non-food images in the insula, orbitofrontal The effects of central insulin on dopaminergic functioning cortex (OFC) and basal ganglia, when compared to people within the VTA likely have downstream effects in suppressing without diabetes mellitus. Moreover, this increased activa- feeding and particularly hedonic feeding. For example, tion within the insula and OFC is positively correlated with Bruijnzeel et al. [68] found that injecting insulin directly into self-reported external eating. These findings, taken togeth- the VTA of female rats decreased 24-h food intake. Mebel er, thus provide evidence that insulin resistance, commonly et al. [69] have similarly found that injecting insulin directly observed following repeated excess consumption of fruc- into the VTA suppresses subsequent feeding in male C57BL/ tose in combination with an overall excessive energy in- 6J mice; however, this effect was dependent on the hunger take [75], is positively correlated with a pattern of neural status of the animals. That is, insulin in the VTA suppressed response to food stimuli which is associated with greater the quantity of sweetened high-fat food consumed by sated external cue-driven eating. However, it should be noted mice but did not affect normal chow intake in hungry mice. that the correlational nature of these findings limits the This pattern of effects therefore suggests that insulin activity ability to draw firm causal inferences. Although there is in the VTA acts selectively to suppress subsequent hedonic relatively less evidence regarding food craving in type I feeding, with weaker evidence for effects on homeostatic diabetes, an fMRI study has found that insulin detemir, feeding behaviour. which more readily enters the brain compared to standard Central insulin functioning may play a role in blocking the forms of insulin, is associated with reduced BOLD re- memory of palatable food reward or attenuating the incentive sponse to food images in the bilateral insula, a brain region salience of cues associated with palatable food. Evidence for associated with the regulation of appetite [76]. The authors this hypothesis comes from studies demonstrating that have speculated that insulin detemir may therefore induce a injecting insulin either into the cerebral ventricles [70]or more effective satiety reaction, thus explaining the reduced VTA [67] of rats at the time of memory retrieval reduces levels of weight gain observed in people with type I dia- conditioned place preference for palatable food. betes mellitus taking insulin detemir [77]. Furthermore, Bruijnzeel et al. [68] found that injecting insulin In a related study, Jastreboff et al. [78] recruited 25 men and at a dose of 0.005 mU/side into the VTA elevated the reward women in the obese weight range and 25 lean controls. 262 Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 Fasting insulin and glucose were taken to measure insulin ventral striatum, whilst the opposite was observed in the resistance. During a subsequent fMRI task, audio scripts de- obese participant group [82]. Thus, there is evidence that signed to provoke relaxing imagery or favourite food imagery glucose impacts upon dopaminergic functioning separate- were played. Food craving was assessed before and after each ly from the effects of sweet taste alone, with BMI modu- imagery trial. The degree of food craving following food im- lating the direction of that effect. The reduced activation agery trials was positively associated with insulin resistance in stimulated by sugar consumption in obese participants is the obese, but not lean, participant group. Furthermore, the in line with previous evidence of downregulated striatal relationship between insulin resistance and food craving with- response to the receipt of sugar solutions, including choc- in the obese participant group was mediated by BOLD re- olate milk and milkshakes [83, 84]. sponses in dopaminergic regions including the VTA and There is a relative paucity of research investigating the substantia nigra. These studies suggest that insulin resistance effects of glucose metabolism on dopamine-mediated moderates craving and associated neural circuits in response feeding in BN and binge eating disorder without obesity. to food-related imagery. A recent meta-analysis of studies analysing insulin sensi- Thus, the above studies illustrate that interactions be- tivity in BN and binge eating disorders has found signif- tween central insulin and dopaminergic systems, known icantly reduced insulin sensitivity in both disorders [85��]. to impact on feeding behaviour in animals, also regulate Such insulin resistance therefore leads to greater flux in food craving in humans. Whilst this evidence therefore blood glucose following the consumption of foods with a supports a potential mechanism linking the short- and high glycaemic index, thus potentially contributing to long-term physiological effects of sugar consumption to food craving in a similar manner to that described above food craving in humans, it is also of interest to disentangle for populations with obesity [78]. The effect of insulin the effects of sweet taste on dopaminergic incentive sensi- resistance on glucose flux is further exacerbated by the tisation from the physiological effects of sugar in food wide swings in blood glucose induced by intermittent approach behaviour in humans. In support of the physio- fasting followed by objectively large binge eating epi- logical effects of glucose consumption on central dopami- sodes in people with BN [86]. Frank et al. [87] found nergic functioning, regardless of sweet taste, Haltia et al evidence of some trait differences in brain response to [79] found that the intravenous administration of glucose, glucose, with participants recovered from BN exhibiting versus placebo, was associated with increased D2 receptor suppressed BOLD response to a glucose, versus artificial binding potential in the right caudate nucleus and bilateral saliva solution, in the anterior cingulate cortex and left putamen in both lean and overweight women. However, cuneus in comparison to the control group. However, this intravenous glucose administration was rather associated study is confounded by the difference in sweet taste as well as nutritional content (glucose versus calorie-free with reductions in D2 receptor binding potential in the bilateral caudate nucleus, left putamen and right thalamus liquid). It will therefore be critical to carry out similar in men. It should be noted that the intravenous method of research to that described above for obesity in populations administration employed in this study bypassed the gastro- with bulimia-spectrum disorders in order to clarify the intestinal system, thus failing to stimulate the production of functional role of a glucose metabolism pathway in loss- gastrointestinal hormones, such as glucagon-like peptide 1, of-control binge eating versus the chronic overeating whichalsoimpactonappetitivefunctioning[80, 81]. This which commonly characterises overweight and obesity. study is therefore limited in the extent to which it directly bears upon the oral consumption of glucose versus calorie- free sweet taste. Nonetheless, these findings provide evi- Conclusion dence for the impact of glucose on mesolimbic dopaminer- gic functioning in the absence of sweet taste. The function- The current literature review has thus far served to illustrate al significance of the sexual dimorphism in brain response the existing state of the evidence with regard to food tolerance is not yet clear and would be an interesting avenue for and withdrawal effects in BN. Additionally, preclinical and future research. preliminary evidence in human studies has elucidated an In another PET study conducted in 19 participants with insulin-dependent mechanism whereby foods with a high BMIs ranging from the lean to obese weight range, dopa- glycaemic index interact with mesolimbic dopamine systems mine functioning was measured following the consump- and heighten food craving in cases of insulin dysregulation. tion of a 75-g oral glucose drink versus a calorie-free Nonetheless, there are several lines of evidence that should be sucralose drink of equal volume and sweetness. Within explored further before definite conclusions can be drawn the lean participant group, consuming the glucose drink, with regard to withdrawal and tolerance in BN and the phys- versus the calorie-free sucralose drink, was associated iological mechanisms which maintain addictive responses to with increased dopaminergic binding potential within the palatable food. Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 263 Funding ML was supported by grants from the Swiss Fund for Anorexia 9. Murray S, Gordillo M, Avena NM. Animal models of eating disor- Nervosa. JT was supported by the National Institute for Health Research ders, substance use disorders, and addictions. In: Eating disorders, (NIHR) Mental Health Biomedical Research Centre at South London and addictions and substance use disorders. Berlin: Springer; 2014. p. Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. 3–21. 10. Colantuoni C, et al. Excessive sugar intake alters binding to dopa- mine and mu-opioid receptors in the brain. Neuroreport. Compliance with Ethical Standards 2001;12(16):3549–52. 11. Johnson PM, Kenny PJ. Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like Conflict of Interest Ms Leslie reports grants from the Swiss Fund for reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nature Anorexia Nervosa, from null, during the conduct of the study; other from neuroscience. 2010;13(5):635. Opiant Pharmaceuticals Inc., outside the submitted work. Dr. Treasure 12. Robinson MJ, et al. Individual differences in cue-induced motiva- has nothing to disclose. Ms Lambert has nothing to disclose. tion and striatal systems in rats susceptible to diet-induced obesity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(9):2113. Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent This article does not 13. Rada P, avena N, Hoebel B. Daily bingeing on sugar repeatedly contain any studies with human or animal subjects performed by any of releases dopamine in the accumbens shell. Neuroscience. the authors. 2005;134(3):737–44. 14. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical man- Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative ualofmentaldisorders. Arlington: American Psychiatric Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http:// Publishing; 2013. creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, 15. Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appro- analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(8):760–73. priate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the 16. Blum K, et al. The reward deficiency syndrome: a biogenetic model Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive and compul- sive behaviors. Journal of psychoactive drugs. 2000;32(sup1):1– 17. Rogers PJ. Food and drug addictions: similarities and differences. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2017;153:182–90. 18. Meule A, Gearhardt AN. Five years of the Yale Food Addiction References Scale: taking stock and moving forward. Current Addiction Reports. 2014;1(3):193–205. 19. de Vries SK, Meule A. Food addiction and bulimia nervosa: new Papers of particular interest, published recently, have been data based on the Yale Food Addiction Scale 2.0. European Eating highlighted as: Disorders Review. 2016;24(6):518–22. � Of importance 20. Stice E, Marti CN, Shaw H, Jaconis M. An 8-year longitudinal study of the natural history of threshold, subthreshold, and partial �� Of major importance eating disorders from a community sample of adolescents. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2009;118(3):587. 1. Finlayson G. Food addiction and obesity: unnecessary medicaliza- 21. Picot AK, Lilenfeld LR. The relationship among binge severity, tion of hedonic overeating. Nature Reviews Endocrinology. personality psychopathology, and body mass index. International 2017;13(8):493. Journal of eating disorders. 2003;34(1):98–107. 2� Treasure J, Leslie M, Chami R, Fernández-Aranda F. Are trans 22. Leslie M, Turton R, Burgess E, Nazar BP, Treasure J. Testing the diagnostic models of eating disorders fit for purpose? A consider- addictive appetite model of binge eating: The importance of crav- ation of the evidence for food addiction. European Eating Disorders ing, coping, and reward enhancement. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2018. Review. 2018;26(2):83–91 This paper presents a new mainte- 23. Franko DL, Wolfe BE, Jimerson DC. Elevated sweet taste pleas- nance model of recurrent loss-of-control binge eating: the ad- antness ratings in bulimia nervosa. Physiology & behavior. dictive appetite model of bulimia and binge eating disorder. 1994;56(5):969–73. This paper posits the existence of specific maintenance process- 24. Drewnowski A, Bellisle F, Aimez P, Remy B. Taste and bulimia. es related to food addiction, which can be formulated as falsi- Physiology & behavior. 1987;41(6):621–6. fiable hypotheses. 25. Drewnowski A, Shrager EE, Lipsky C, Stellar E, Greenwood M. 3. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine in re- Sugar and fat: sensory and hedonic evaluation of liquid and solid ward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain foods. Physiology & behavior. 1989;45(1):177–83. research reviews. 1998;28(3):309–69. 26. Rodin J, Bartoshuk L, Peterson C, Schank D. Bulimia and taste: pos- 4. Carnell S, Wardle J. Appetite and adiposity in children: evidence for sible interactions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1990;99(1):32. a behavioral susceptibility theory of obesity. The American journal 27. Bohon C, Stice E. Reward abnormalities among women with full of clinical nutrition. 2008;88(1):22–9. and subthreshold bulimia nervosa: a functional magnetic resonance 5. Turton R, Chami R, Treasure J. Emotional eating, binge eating and imaging study (in English). International Journal of Eating animal models of binge-type eating disorders. Current obesity re- Disorders. 2011;44(7):585–95. ports. 2017;6(2):217–28. 28. Frank GK, Reynolds JR, Shott ME, O'Reilly RC. Altered temporal 6. Brewerton TD. Are eating disorders addictions? In: Eating disor- difference learning in bulimia nervosa. Biological psychiatry. ders, addictions and substance use disorders. Berlin: Springer; 2011;70(8):728–35. 2014. p. 267–99. 29. Oberndorfer TA, et al. Altered insula response to sweet taste pro- 7. Ziauddeen H, Farooqi IS, Fletcher PC. Obesity and the brain: how cessing after recovery from anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Am J convincing is the addiction model? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. Psychiatry. 2013;170(10):1143–51. 2012;13(4):279. 30. Radeloff D, et al. High-fat taste challenge reveals altered striatal 8. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Preliminary validation response in women recovered from bulimia nervosa: a pilot study. of the Yale food addiction scale. Appetite. 2009;52(2):430–6. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2014;15(4):307–16. 264 Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 31. Haedt-Matt AA, Keel PK. Revisiting the affect regulation model of the natural environment. Journal of consulting and clinical psychol- ogy. 2007;75(4):629. binge eating: a meta-analysis of studies using ecological momen- tary assessment. Psychological bulletin. 2011;137(4):660. 52. Selby EA, et al. Momentary emotion surrounding bulimic behav- 32. Halmi KA, Sunday S, Puglisi A, Marchi P. Hunger and satiety in iors in women with bulimia nervosa and borderline personality anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Annals of the New York Academy of disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2012;46(11):1492–500. Sciences. 1989;575(1):431–45. 53. Braun DL, Sunday S, Halmi K. Psychiatric comorbidity in patients 33. Ely AV, et al. Response in taste circuitry is not modulated by hunger with eating disorders. Psychological medicine. 1994;24(4):859–67. and satiety in women remitted from bulimia nervosa. Journal of 54. Pursey KM, Davis C, Burrows TL. Nutritional aspects of food abnormal psychology. 2017;126(5):519. addiction. Current Addiction Reports. 2017;4(2):142–50. 34. Sun X, et al. Basolateral amygdala response to food cues in the 55. Curtis C, Davis C. A qualitative study of binge eating and obesity absence of hunger is associated with weight gain susceptibility. from an addiction perspective. Eating disorders. 2014;22(1):19–32. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35(20):7964–76. 56. Schulte EM, Avena NM, Gearhardt AN. Which foods may be ad- 35. Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Evidence for sugar addiction: dictive? The roles of processing, fat content, and glycemic load. behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sug- PloS one. 2015;10(2):e0117959. ar intake. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32(1):20– 57. Avena NM, Rada P, Moise N, Hoebel B. Sucrose sham feeding on a binge schedule releases accumbens dopamine repeatedly and elim- 36. Iemolo A, et al. Withdrawal from chronic, intermittent access to a inates the acetylcholine satiety response. Neuroscience. highly palatable food induces depressive-like behavior in compul- 2006;139(3):813–20. sive eating rats. Behavioural pharmacology. 2012;23:593. 58. Berridge KC, Ho C-Y, Richard JM, DiFeliceantonio AG. The 37. Wideman C, Nadzam G, Murphy H. Implications of an animal tempted brain eats: pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating model of sugar addiction, withdrawal and relapse for human health. disorders. Brain research. 2010;1350:43–64. Nutritional neuroscience. 2005;8(5–6):269–76. 59. Zhou Q-Y, Palmiter RD. Dopamine-deficient mice are severely 38. Bocarsly ME, Berner LA, Hoebel BG, Avena NM. Rats that binge hypoactive, adipsic, and aphagic. Cell. 1995;83(7):1197–209. eat fat-rich food do not show somatic signs or anxiety associated 60. Berridge KC, Valenstein ES. What psychological process mediates with opiate-like withdrawal: implications for nutrient-specific food feeding evoked by electrical stimulation of the lateral hypothala- addiction behaviors. Physiology & behavior. 2011;104(5):865–72. mus? Behavioral neuroscience. 1991;105(1):3. 39.� Schulte EM, Smeal JK, Lewis J, Gearhardt AN. Development of 61. Woods SC, Lotter EC, McKay LD, Porte D Jr. Chronic intra- the highly processed food withdrawal scale. Appetite. 2018;131: cerebroventricular infusion of insulin reduces food intake and body 148–54 This paper describes the development and validation of weight of baboons. Nature. 1979;282(5738):503. the Highly Processed Food Withdrawal Scale (ProWS), which 62. Figlewicz D, Evans S, Murphy J, Hoen M, Baskin D. Expression of is the first validated psychometric scale to measure withdrawal receptors for insulin and leptin in the ventral tegmental symptoms in the context of food addiction. This paper presents area/substantia nigra (VTA/SN) of the rat. Brain research. data supporting the convergent validity of the ProWS with 2003;964(1):107–15. addictive-like eating and the predictive validity of the ProWS 63. Pardini AW, et al. Distribution of insulin receptor substrate-2 in for dieting success. brain areas involved in energy homeostasis. Brain research. 40. Ziauddeen H, Fletcher PC. Is food addiction a valid and useful 2006;1112(1):169–78. concept? obesity reviews. 2013;14(1):19–28. 64. Figlewicz DP, Szot P, Chavez M, Woods SC, Veith RC. 41. Hansen S. Conceptualizing bulimia as addiction: a resident’sper- Intraventricular insulin increases dopamine transporter mRNA in sonal experience. American Journal of Psychiatry Residents' rat VTA/substantia nigra. Brain research. 1994;644(2):331–4. Journal. 2016;11(08):6–8. 65. Williams JM, et al. Hypoinsulinemia regulates amphetamine- 42. Davis C, Carter JC. Compulsive overeating as an addiction disor- induced reverse transport of dopamine. PLoS biology. der. A review of theory and evidence. Appetite. 2009;53(1):1–8. 2007;5(10):e274. 43. Thornley S, McRobbie H. Carbohydrate withdrawal: is recognition 66. Carvelli L, et al. PI 3-kinase regulation of dopamine uptake. Journal the first step to recovery? The New Zealand Medical Journal of neurochemistry. 2002;81(4):859–69. (Online). 2009;122(1290). 67. Labouèbe G, et al. Insulin induces long-term depression of ventral 44. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Development of the tegmental area dopamine neurons via endocannabinoids. Nature Yale Food Addiction Scale Version 2.0. Psychology of Addictive neuroscience. 2013;16(3):300. Behaviors. 2016;30(1):113. 68. Bruijnzeel AW, Corrie LW, Rogers JA, Yamada H. Effects of insu- 45. Ifland J, et al. Refined food addiction: a classic substance use dis- lin and leptin in the ventral tegmental area and arcuate hypothalam- order. Medical hypotheses. 2009;72(5):518–26. ic nucleus on food intake and brain reward function in female rats. 46. Lingswiler VM, Crowther JH, Stephens M. Affective and cognitive Behavioural brain research. 2011;219(2):254–64. antecedents to eating episodes in bulimia and binge eating. 69. Mebel DM, Wong JC, Dong YJ, Borgland SL. Insulin in the ventral International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1989;8(5):533–9. tegmental area reduces hedonic feeding and suppresses dopamine 47. Gilbert DG, Gilbert BO, Schultz VL. Withdrawal symptoms: indi- concentration via increased reuptake. European Journal of vidual differences and similarities across addictive behaviors. Neuroscience. 2012;36(3):2336–46. Personality and individual Differences. 1998;24(3):351–6. 70. Figlewicz DP, Bennett J, Evans SB, Kaiyala K, Sipols AJ, Benoit 48. Farrell M. Opiate withdrawal. Addiction. 1994;89(11):1471–5. SC. Intraventricular insulin and leptin reverse place preference con- 49. Tekol Y. Salt addiction: a different kind of drug addiction. Medical ditioned with high-fat diet in rats. Behavioral neuroscience. hypotheses. 2006;67(5):1233–4. 2004;118(3):479. 50. Anestis MD, Selby EA, Crosby RD, Wonderlich SA, Engel SG, 71. Dunn JP, et al. Relationship of dopamine type 2 receptor binding Joiner TE. A comparison of retrospective self-report versus ecolog- potential with fasting neuroendocrine hormones and insulin sensi- ical momentary assessment measures of affective lability in the tivity in human obesity. Diabetes care. 2012:DC_112250. examination of its relationship with bulimic symptomatology. 72. Anthony K, et al. Attenuation of insulin-evoked responses in brain Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2010;48(7):607–13. networks controlling appetite and reward in insulin resistance: the 51. Smyth JM, et al. Daily and momentary mood and stress are associ- cerebral basis for impaired control of food intake in metabolic syn- ated with binge eating and vomiting in bulimia nervosa patients in drome? Diabetes. 2006;55(11):2986–92. Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 265 73. Volkow ND, et al. “Nonhedonic” food motivation in humans in- 82. Wang G-J, et al. BMI modulates calorie-dependent dopamine changes in accumbens from glucose intake. PloS one. 2014;9(7): volves dopamine in the dorsal striatum and methylphenidate am- plifies this effect. Synapse. 2002;44(3):175–80. e101585. 83. Burger KS, Stice E. Greater striatopallidal adaptive coding during 74. Chechlacz M, et al. Diabetes dietary management alters responses cue–reward learning and food reward habituation predict future to food pictures in brain regions associated with motivation and weight gain. Neuroimage. 2014;99:122–8. emotion: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. 84. Stice E, Yokum S, Blum K, Bohon C. Weight gain is associated Diabetologia. 2009;52(3):524. with reduced striatal response to palatable food. Journal of 75. MacDonald IA. A review of recent evidence relating to sugars, Neuroscience. 2010;30(39):13105–9. insulin resistance and diabetes. European journal of nutrition. 85.�� Ilyas A, et al. The metabolic underpinning of eating disorders: a 2016;55(2):17–23. systematic review and meta-analysis of insulin sensitivity. 76. van Golen LW, et al. Effects of insulin detemir and NPH insulin on Molecular and cellular endocrinology. 2018; This paper presents body weight and appetite-regulating brain regions in human type 1 a systematic review and meta-analysis of variations in insulin diabetes: a randomized controlled trial. Plos one. 2014;9(4):e94483. sensitivity across the spectrum of eating disorders. The meta- 77. Hermansen K, Fontaine P, Kukolja K, Peterkova V, Leth G, Gall M- analysis supported greater insulin sensitivity in anorexia A. Insulin analogues (insulin detemir and insulin aspart) versus nervosa and decreased insulin sensitivity in populations with traditional human insulins (NPH insulin and regular human insulin) bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder. in basal-bolus therapy for patients with type 1 diabetes. 86. Monteleone P, Bortolotti F, Fabrazzo M, La Rocca A, Fuschino A, Diabetologia. 2004;47(4):622–9. Maj M. Plasma leptin response to acute fasting and refeeding in 78. Jastreboff AM, Sinha R, Lacadie C, Small DM, Sherwin RS, untreated women with bulimia nervosa. The Journal of Clinical Potenza MN. Neural correlates of stress-and food cue-induced food Endocrinology & Metabolism. 2000;85(7):2499–503. craving in obesity: association with insulin levels. Diabetes care. 87. Frank GK, et al. Altered brain activity in women recovered from 2013;36(2):394–402. bulimic-type eating disorders after a glucose challenge: a pilot 79. Haltia LT, et al. Effects of intravenous glucose on dopaminergic study. Int J Eat Disord. 2006;39(1):76–9. function in the human brain in vivo. Synapse. 2007;61(9):748–56. 80. Vilsbøll T, Krarup T, Madsbad S, Holst JJ. Both GLP-1 and GIP are Publisher’sNote Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to insulinotropic at basal and postprandial glucose levels and contrib- jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. ute nearly equally to the incretin effect of a meal in healthy subjects. Regulatory peptides. 2003;114(2–3):115–21. 81. Flint A, Raben A, Astrup A, Holst JJ. Glucagon-like peptide 1 promotes satiety and suppresses energy intake in humans. The Journal of clinical investigation. 1998;101(3):515–20. http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png Current Addiction Reports Springer Journals

Towards a Translational Approach to Food Addiction: Implications for Bulimia Nervosa

Loading next page...
 
/lp/springer-journals/towards-a-translational-approach-to-food-addiction-implications-for-ul7EggZgiC

References (95)

Publisher
Springer Journals
Copyright
Copyright © 2019 by The Author(s)
Subject
Medicine & Public Health; Psychiatry; Neurology
eISSN
2196-2952
DOI
10.1007/s40429-019-00264-0
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

Purpose of Review In recent years, the food addiction hypothesis of loss-of-control eating has gained traction in the field of eating disorders. In particular, the neural process of food addiction plays a dominant role in the recently formulated “addictive appetite” model of bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder. Nonetheless, several components of the food addiction hypothesis, including the presence of withdrawal and tolerance effects, as well as the proposition that some foods possess “addicting” properties, remain highly controversial. In response, the current review synthesises existing evidence for withdrawal and tolerance effects in people with bulimia nervosa. Recent Findings The recent development of a validated tool to measure withdrawal from highly processed foods will aid in measuring withdrawal symptoms and testing hypotheses related to withdrawal in the context of food addiction. We subsequently describe preclinical and human evidence for a central insulin- and dopamine-mediated pathway by which recurrent loss-of- control binge eating is maintained in bulimia nervosa. Summary Evidence in populations with bulimia nervosa and loss-of-control eating provides preliminary support for the role of food addiction in the maintenance of bulimia nervosa. Future longitudinal research is needed to develop a clearer profile of illness progression and to clarify the extent to which dysregulation in glucose metabolism contributes to food craving and symptom maintenance in bulimia nervosa. . . . . Keywords Food addiction Bulimia nervosa Eating disorders Sugar Dopamine Background In this article, we will use bulimia nervosa (BN) as the exem- plar. The “Addictive Appetite Model” proposes that three pri- The concept of “food addiction” has received increasing at- mary processes maintain psychopathology in BN: (1) the high tention in the scientific literature of recent years. While cogent salience of palatable foods [3], which is moderated by a ge- arguments have been made against the establishment of food netic susceptibility to food approach tendencies, reduced effi- addiction as a psychiatric diagnosis in its own right [1], there ciency in satiation processes [4] and/or episodes of food re- is substantial evidence to suggest that processes similar to striction; (2) chronic stress and interpersonal difficulties those observed in substance abuse disorders play a significant resulting in a deficiency of alternative rewards and a primed role in the maintenance of eating disorders in which loss of stress system [5]; and (3) large swings in blood glucose, control of eating is a feature (e.g. anorexia nervosa-binge caused by the consumption of foods with a high glycaemic purge type, bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder) [2� ]. index, self-induced vomiting or insulin resistance (and insulin omission in diabetes mellitus). These pathways may contrib- ute to compulsive binge eating behaviour through aberrations This article is part of the Topical Collection on Food Addiction in dopaminergic function in a similar way to substance addictions. * Monica Leslie The current review presents a synthesis of the literature monica.leslie@kcl.ac.uk investigating some of the controversial aspects of applying the food addiction paradigm to eating disorders. For instance, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience (IoPPN), there is uncertainty as to whether tolerance and withdrawal Section of Eating Disorders, King’s College London (KCL), 103 criteria for an addictive disorder as specified within the Denmark Hill, London SE5 8AF, UK Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 259 DSM-5 are met. We also present a synthesis of molecular, consumption [13], a finding which may be of particular im- preclinical, clinical and neuroimaging evidence illustrating portance to BN as it is characterised by intermittent fasting how fluxes in glucose and insulin moderate central dopami- and binge episodes [14]. A similar downregulation of dopa- nergic functioning. mine D2 receptors is found in humans addicted to drugs of abuse [15] and is thought to be a key driver of compensatory overconsumption in the Reward Hyposensitivity Theory [16, Does Bulimia Nervosa Meet DSM-5 Criteria 17]. Behavioural observations in people with substance use for an Addictive Disorder? disorders mirror these findings. Individuals with BN endorse higher levels of tolerance-like The DSM-5 criteria for an addictive disorder are presented in symptoms measured using the Yale Food Addiction Scale (YFAS) [8], compared with healthy controls [18, 19]. There Table 1. The extent to which BN meets these criteria has been reviewed extensively elsewhere [6]. In his 2014 review, are clinical reports of subthreshold BN patients initiating larg- er and more frequent binge episodes over time [20]. Brewerton acknowledges significant phenotypic overlap be- tween disorders of substance dependence and BN but high- Consistent with these accounts is cross-sectional evidence of the correlation between higher body weight and frequency and lights a paucity of systematic clinical evidence for tolerance and withdrawal in the latter, a point frequently cited as a major severity of binge eating episodes [21]. Individuals with binge- weakness of the food addiction hypothesis [7]. Much research type eating disorders endorse significantly greater levels of on tolerance and withdrawal symptoms in humans to date has eating for purposes of reward enhancement compared with been largely anecdotal [8]. However, herein we synthesise the weight-matched controls [22]. Such evidence, although com- evidence from new assessment methods. pelling, remains indirect and is insufficient to prove the exis- tence of tolerance in humans. A preference for intensely sweet food and larger quan- Tolerance tities of sweeteners in individuals with BN is a character- istic often presented as an indicator of tolerance [3, The most compelling evidence for food tolerance is demon- strated in animal models, as previously reviewed by Murray 23–25]. Furthermore, this preference remains after ingesting a glucose load [26]. Magnetic resonance imag- et al. [9]. Rats who voluntarily overeat highly palatable food exhibit evidence of a neural reward deficit due to downregu- ing (MRI) studies indicate hypofunctioning of gustatory and limbic circuitry in BN patients when tasting palatable lated dopamine D2 receptors, which worsens as weight is gained [10–12]. This decreased sensitivity to reward is direct- food compared with controls [27, 28] and compared with individuals recovered from BN [29, 30]. This evidence is ly linked to the onset of compulsive food seeking in rats [11]. Repetitive bingeing on sucrose interspersed with periods of consistent with the idea that individuals with BN ingest dietary restriction causes rats to triple their overall daily sugar more food over time because of a decreased sensitivity to Table 1 DSM-5 criteria for an addictive disorder (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) Criteria 1 The substance is often taken in larger amounts or over a longer period than was intended Criteria 2 There is a persistent desire or unsuccessful efforts to cut down or control use of the substance Criteria 3 A great deal of time is spent in activities necessary to obtain the substance, use the substance or recover from its effects Criteria 4 Craving or a strong desire or urge to use the substance Criteria 5 Recurrent use of the substance despite having persistent or recurrent social or interpersonal problems caused or exacerbated by the effects of its use Criteria 6 Continued use of the substance despite having persistent or recurrent social or interpersonal problems caused or exacerbated by the effects of its use Criteria 7 Important social, occupational, or recreational activities are given up or reduced because of use of the substance Criteria 8 Recurrent use of the substance in situations in which it is physically hazardous Criteria 9 Tolerance, as defined by either of the following: A. A need for markedly increased amounts of the substance to achieve intoxication or desired effect B. A markedly diminished effect with continued use of the same amount of the substance Criteria 10 Withdrawal, as manifested by either of the following: A. The characteristic withdrawal syndrome for other substance B. The substance (or a closely related substance) is taken to relieve or avoid withdrawal symptoms Bold text indicate criteria for an addictive disorder which are discussed in depth in the current manuscript with reference to their relevance to bulimia nervosa 260 Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 sweet taste resulting from repeated bingeing on abstaining from salted food [49]. Furthermore, tiredness and hyperpalatable foods [27]. irritability have been cited as motivating factors for eating The development of impaired satiety mechanisms may be [45], providing some suggestion of food being used as a an indirect indicator of tolerance [6, 31, 32]. For example, a “pick-me-up”, or to avoid experiencing negative feelings recent functional MRI (fMRI) study found that women in stemming from withdrawal. remission from BN exhibited the same response to taste stim- Symptoms of psychological withdrawal are also widely uli in brain regions implicated in translating sensory informa- reported. Cross-sectional self-report accounts from patients tion about taste into motivated behaviour, regardless of wheth- with BN reveal that most feel tension, loneliness and physical er the individuals were hungry or sated, whilst healthy con- symptoms of anxiety before a binge, and the majority feel that trols showed an increased response to taste stimuli when hun- their negative psychological states are alleviated whilst en- gry versus when fed [33]. The authors also found an increased gaged in a binge [39� ]. Longitudinal studies using amygdala response in their remitted BN sample when fed Ecological Momentary Assessment technology also report compared to healthy controls, which they propose might pro- that binge eating and subsequent purging are usually preceded ject to the hypothalamus and motivate eating in the absence of by dysphoric mood states [50–52]. However, there is high hunger [33, 34]. It is possible that brain circuitry in BN fails to prevalence of depression and emotion dysregulation in BN de-value food reward when in a fed state, leading to eating populations [53], so it is not clear whether such presentations beyond metabolic need. of low mood represent psychological withdrawal from food. Five years on from Brewerton’s 2014 review, there remains Future research should aim to elucidate whether dysphoric a paucity of direct evidence of tolerance in humans in relation mood states before bingeing are distinct from more permanent to food intake and prospective, longitudinal studies are mood-related comorbidities, perhaps by comparing depressed needed. versus non-depressed individuals with BN. The first and only tool to evaluate withdrawal in the context Withdrawal Syndromes of addictive-like eating has been developed recently: the Highly Processed Food Withdrawal Scale—ProWS [39� ]. Preliminary evidence for a withdrawal syndrome in relation- This in part is derived from the premise that specific nutrition- ship to palatable food comes from animal models. There are al ingredients are capable of triggering addictive-like re- consistent observations of strong physical (e.g. forepaw trem- sponses [54]. In a pilot study, the ProWS was found to be or, teeth chattering) and psychological (e.g. aggression, anxi- positively associated with elevated YFAS symptoms, BMI ety) withdrawal responses in rats during periods of withdrawal and weight cycling in a community sample, and responses from sucrose [35–37]. The same observations, however, are on the ProWS explained an additional 11.2% of the variance not found with removal of high-fat foods [38] and have not yet in self-reported dieting success [39� ]. This tool may help dif- been studied with removal of highly processed foods [39� ]. ferentiate between the withdrawal effects from different types Neuroimaging studies show patterns consistent with this be- of palatable food [55, 56]. havioural data. Sugar-dependent rats show a significant in- crease in extracellular acetylcholine and a decrease in dopa- mine release in the nucleus accumbens shell, as compared to The Impact of Glucose Metabolism control groups, during a 36-h period of food deprivation [35], on Hedonic Eating Behaviour effects which are similar to withdrawal from morphine, nico- tine and alcohol. Preclinical Evidence Traditionally, withdrawal symptoms have not been clearly defined in the context of addictive-like eating, prompting crit- Sweet, pa latable foods act as unconditioned rewarding stimuli icism of the food addiction framework [40]. To date, the food in humans and rodent models, with evidence suggesting that addiction field has largely relied on observational and anec- merely tasting sucrose without digestion produces activation dotal clinical reports based on small cohorts or single case of dopaminergic circuits within the striatum [57]. However, studies [41–46] and on self-reported endorsement of with- there is evidence to suggest that palatable foods with a high drawal symptoms on the YFAS [18] and other withdrawal glycaemic index further contribute to the development of scales [47]. Cross-sectional self-report accounts are consistent compulsive binge eating behaviour through changes in dopa- in describing physiological symptoms of withdrawal similar minergic functioning triggered by wide swings in blood glu- to those experienced during opiate withdrawal [8, 48]. cose. One candidate mechanism for this effect relates to the Headaches, irritability and flu-like symptoms are reported by interaction between insulin and dopaminergic functioning. individuals abstaining from sugar [42], stomach pains, muscle The role of mesolimbic dopaminergic functioning in food spasms and shakiness by individuals abstaining or reducing approach behaviours has been reviewed extensively else- intake of carbohydrates [43, 45] and nausea by individuals where [58]. Dopamine-deficient mouse models exhibit severe Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 261 aphagia leading to weight loss and death [59]. Conversely, the threshold for intracranial self-stimulation, thus indicating a stimulation of dopaminergic activity within the striatum trig- reduction of reward functioning. gers food consumption in rats without enhancing “liking” re- sponses (e.g. lip-licking and paw licking). Thus, dopaminergic Evidence in Humans functioning is thought to hold a role in food approach behav- iours (wanting) that is discrete from the hedonic response to Insulin resistance impacts on central dopaminergic systems in food receipt [60]. In contrast, central insulin suppresses feed- humans. For example, Dunn et al. [71] conducted a positron ing [61]. It is thought that the effects of central insulin and emission tomography (PET) study using the dopamine D2/D3 dopamine on food intake are not independent but rather inter- receptor radioligand [18F] fallypride and found that insulin act to regulate hedonic eating behaviour. sensitivity is negatively correlated with dopamine type 2 re- Dopaminergic neurons within the ventral tegmental area ceptor availability in the ventral striatum in a heterogeneous (VTA) express insulin receptors [62, 63], presenting a possible sample of lean and obese women. In men, Anthony et al. [72] mechanism by which insulin might influence the dopaminer- found that exogenously administered insulin increases metab- gic induction of feeding behaviour. Furthermore, central insu- olism in the ventral striatum and prefrontal cortex, whilst de- lin enhances the expression of dopamine transporter protein creasing metabolism in the right amygdala, hippocampus and within the VTA via a protein kinase B (Akt) signalling system cerebellar vermis. Furthermore, the effect of insulin in increas- [64, 65]. The enhanced expression of dopamine transporters ing metabolism in the ventral striatum and prefrontal cortex on the cell surface induced by insulin exposure is associated was lower in insulin-resistant versus insulin-sensitive partici- with greater dopamine uptake [66], thus reducing levels of pants [72]. This pattern of findings is thus indicative of trait- synaptic dopamine. level differences in the effects of central insulin on dopami- With regard to the effects of insulin on postsynaptic dopa- nergic mesolimbic regions, known to be critical for food crav- minergic signalling, in vitro studies have found that insulin ing and food approach behaviour [59, 73]. exposure invokes long-term depression of excitatory signal- Interactions between insulin resistance and central do- ling within VTA dopamine neurons extracted from male paminergic functioning may have functional significance C57BL/6J mice [67]. This effect appears to be long-lasting for food craving in humans. Chechlacz et al. [74], in an as, once induced, the long-term depression of VTA dopamine fMRI study, found that people with type II diabetes cells is not reversed by application of the insulin receptor mellitus (characterised by insulin resistance) exhibit great- antagonist S961 or through a tyrosine kinase inhibitor, which er blood oxygenated level dependent (BOLD) response to suppresses insulin receptor functioning [67]. food versus non-food images in the insula, orbitofrontal The effects of central insulin on dopaminergic functioning cortex (OFC) and basal ganglia, when compared to people within the VTA likely have downstream effects in suppressing without diabetes mellitus. Moreover, this increased activa- feeding and particularly hedonic feeding. For example, tion within the insula and OFC is positively correlated with Bruijnzeel et al. [68] found that injecting insulin directly into self-reported external eating. These findings, taken togeth- the VTA of female rats decreased 24-h food intake. Mebel er, thus provide evidence that insulin resistance, commonly et al. [69] have similarly found that injecting insulin directly observed following repeated excess consumption of fruc- into the VTA suppresses subsequent feeding in male C57BL/ tose in combination with an overall excessive energy in- 6J mice; however, this effect was dependent on the hunger take [75], is positively correlated with a pattern of neural status of the animals. That is, insulin in the VTA suppressed response to food stimuli which is associated with greater the quantity of sweetened high-fat food consumed by sated external cue-driven eating. However, it should be noted mice but did not affect normal chow intake in hungry mice. that the correlational nature of these findings limits the This pattern of effects therefore suggests that insulin activity ability to draw firm causal inferences. Although there is in the VTA acts selectively to suppress subsequent hedonic relatively less evidence regarding food craving in type I feeding, with weaker evidence for effects on homeostatic diabetes, an fMRI study has found that insulin detemir, feeding behaviour. which more readily enters the brain compared to standard Central insulin functioning may play a role in blocking the forms of insulin, is associated with reduced BOLD re- memory of palatable food reward or attenuating the incentive sponse to food images in the bilateral insula, a brain region salience of cues associated with palatable food. Evidence for associated with the regulation of appetite [76]. The authors this hypothesis comes from studies demonstrating that have speculated that insulin detemir may therefore induce a injecting insulin either into the cerebral ventricles [70]or more effective satiety reaction, thus explaining the reduced VTA [67] of rats at the time of memory retrieval reduces levels of weight gain observed in people with type I dia- conditioned place preference for palatable food. betes mellitus taking insulin detemir [77]. Furthermore, Bruijnzeel et al. [68] found that injecting insulin In a related study, Jastreboff et al. [78] recruited 25 men and at a dose of 0.005 mU/side into the VTA elevated the reward women in the obese weight range and 25 lean controls. 262 Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 Fasting insulin and glucose were taken to measure insulin ventral striatum, whilst the opposite was observed in the resistance. During a subsequent fMRI task, audio scripts de- obese participant group [82]. Thus, there is evidence that signed to provoke relaxing imagery or favourite food imagery glucose impacts upon dopaminergic functioning separate- were played. Food craving was assessed before and after each ly from the effects of sweet taste alone, with BMI modu- imagery trial. The degree of food craving following food im- lating the direction of that effect. The reduced activation agery trials was positively associated with insulin resistance in stimulated by sugar consumption in obese participants is the obese, but not lean, participant group. Furthermore, the in line with previous evidence of downregulated striatal relationship between insulin resistance and food craving with- response to the receipt of sugar solutions, including choc- in the obese participant group was mediated by BOLD re- olate milk and milkshakes [83, 84]. sponses in dopaminergic regions including the VTA and There is a relative paucity of research investigating the substantia nigra. These studies suggest that insulin resistance effects of glucose metabolism on dopamine-mediated moderates craving and associated neural circuits in response feeding in BN and binge eating disorder without obesity. to food-related imagery. A recent meta-analysis of studies analysing insulin sensi- Thus, the above studies illustrate that interactions be- tivity in BN and binge eating disorders has found signif- tween central insulin and dopaminergic systems, known icantly reduced insulin sensitivity in both disorders [85��]. to impact on feeding behaviour in animals, also regulate Such insulin resistance therefore leads to greater flux in food craving in humans. Whilst this evidence therefore blood glucose following the consumption of foods with a supports a potential mechanism linking the short- and high glycaemic index, thus potentially contributing to long-term physiological effects of sugar consumption to food craving in a similar manner to that described above food craving in humans, it is also of interest to disentangle for populations with obesity [78]. The effect of insulin the effects of sweet taste on dopaminergic incentive sensi- resistance on glucose flux is further exacerbated by the tisation from the physiological effects of sugar in food wide swings in blood glucose induced by intermittent approach behaviour in humans. In support of the physio- fasting followed by objectively large binge eating epi- logical effects of glucose consumption on central dopami- sodes in people with BN [86]. Frank et al. [87] found nergic functioning, regardless of sweet taste, Haltia et al evidence of some trait differences in brain response to [79] found that the intravenous administration of glucose, glucose, with participants recovered from BN exhibiting versus placebo, was associated with increased D2 receptor suppressed BOLD response to a glucose, versus artificial binding potential in the right caudate nucleus and bilateral saliva solution, in the anterior cingulate cortex and left putamen in both lean and overweight women. However, cuneus in comparison to the control group. However, this intravenous glucose administration was rather associated study is confounded by the difference in sweet taste as well as nutritional content (glucose versus calorie-free with reductions in D2 receptor binding potential in the bilateral caudate nucleus, left putamen and right thalamus liquid). It will therefore be critical to carry out similar in men. It should be noted that the intravenous method of research to that described above for obesity in populations administration employed in this study bypassed the gastro- with bulimia-spectrum disorders in order to clarify the intestinal system, thus failing to stimulate the production of functional role of a glucose metabolism pathway in loss- gastrointestinal hormones, such as glucagon-like peptide 1, of-control binge eating versus the chronic overeating whichalsoimpactonappetitivefunctioning[80, 81]. This which commonly characterises overweight and obesity. study is therefore limited in the extent to which it directly bears upon the oral consumption of glucose versus calorie- free sweet taste. Nonetheless, these findings provide evi- Conclusion dence for the impact of glucose on mesolimbic dopaminer- gic functioning in the absence of sweet taste. The function- The current literature review has thus far served to illustrate al significance of the sexual dimorphism in brain response the existing state of the evidence with regard to food tolerance is not yet clear and would be an interesting avenue for and withdrawal effects in BN. Additionally, preclinical and future research. preliminary evidence in human studies has elucidated an In another PET study conducted in 19 participants with insulin-dependent mechanism whereby foods with a high BMIs ranging from the lean to obese weight range, dopa- glycaemic index interact with mesolimbic dopamine systems mine functioning was measured following the consump- and heighten food craving in cases of insulin dysregulation. tion of a 75-g oral glucose drink versus a calorie-free Nonetheless, there are several lines of evidence that should be sucralose drink of equal volume and sweetness. Within explored further before definite conclusions can be drawn the lean participant group, consuming the glucose drink, with regard to withdrawal and tolerance in BN and the phys- versus the calorie-free sucralose drink, was associated iological mechanisms which maintain addictive responses to with increased dopaminergic binding potential within the palatable food. Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 263 Funding ML was supported by grants from the Swiss Fund for Anorexia 9. Murray S, Gordillo M, Avena NM. Animal models of eating disor- Nervosa. JT was supported by the National Institute for Health Research ders, substance use disorders, and addictions. In: Eating disorders, (NIHR) Mental Health Biomedical Research Centre at South London and addictions and substance use disorders. Berlin: Springer; 2014. p. Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London. 3–21. 10. Colantuoni C, et al. Excessive sugar intake alters binding to dopa- mine and mu-opioid receptors in the brain. Neuroreport. Compliance with Ethical Standards 2001;12(16):3549–52. 11. Johnson PM, Kenny PJ. Dopamine D2 receptors in addiction-like Conflict of Interest Ms Leslie reports grants from the Swiss Fund for reward dysfunction and compulsive eating in obese rats. Nature Anorexia Nervosa, from null, during the conduct of the study; other from neuroscience. 2010;13(5):635. Opiant Pharmaceuticals Inc., outside the submitted work. Dr. Treasure 12. Robinson MJ, et al. Individual differences in cue-induced motiva- has nothing to disclose. Ms Lambert has nothing to disclose. tion and striatal systems in rats susceptible to diet-induced obesity. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2015;40(9):2113. Human and Animal Rights and Informed Consent This article does not 13. Rada P, avena N, Hoebel B. Daily bingeing on sugar repeatedly contain any studies with human or animal subjects performed by any of releases dopamine in the accumbens shell. Neuroscience. the authors. 2005;134(3):737–44. 14. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical man- Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative ualofmentaldisorders. Arlington: American Psychiatric Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http:// Publishing; 2013. creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, 15. Koob GF, Volkow ND. Neurobiology of addiction: a neurocircuitry distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appro- analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(8):760–73. priate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the 16. Blum K, et al. The reward deficiency syndrome: a biogenetic model Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive and compul- sive behaviors. Journal of psychoactive drugs. 2000;32(sup1):1– 17. Rogers PJ. Food and drug addictions: similarities and differences. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior. 2017;153:182–90. 18. Meule A, Gearhardt AN. Five years of the Yale Food Addiction References Scale: taking stock and moving forward. Current Addiction Reports. 2014;1(3):193–205. 19. de Vries SK, Meule A. Food addiction and bulimia nervosa: new Papers of particular interest, published recently, have been data based on the Yale Food Addiction Scale 2.0. European Eating highlighted as: Disorders Review. 2016;24(6):518–22. � Of importance 20. Stice E, Marti CN, Shaw H, Jaconis M. An 8-year longitudinal study of the natural history of threshold, subthreshold, and partial �� Of major importance eating disorders from a community sample of adolescents. Journal of abnormal psychology. 2009;118(3):587. 1. Finlayson G. Food addiction and obesity: unnecessary medicaliza- 21. Picot AK, Lilenfeld LR. The relationship among binge severity, tion of hedonic overeating. Nature Reviews Endocrinology. personality psychopathology, and body mass index. International 2017;13(8):493. Journal of eating disorders. 2003;34(1):98–107. 2� Treasure J, Leslie M, Chami R, Fernández-Aranda F. Are trans 22. Leslie M, Turton R, Burgess E, Nazar BP, Treasure J. Testing the diagnostic models of eating disorders fit for purpose? A consider- addictive appetite model of binge eating: The importance of crav- ation of the evidence for food addiction. European Eating Disorders ing, coping, and reward enhancement. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2018. Review. 2018;26(2):83–91 This paper presents a new mainte- 23. Franko DL, Wolfe BE, Jimerson DC. Elevated sweet taste pleas- nance model of recurrent loss-of-control binge eating: the ad- antness ratings in bulimia nervosa. Physiology & behavior. dictive appetite model of bulimia and binge eating disorder. 1994;56(5):969–73. This paper posits the existence of specific maintenance process- 24. Drewnowski A, Bellisle F, Aimez P, Remy B. Taste and bulimia. es related to food addiction, which can be formulated as falsi- Physiology & behavior. 1987;41(6):621–6. fiable hypotheses. 25. Drewnowski A, Shrager EE, Lipsky C, Stellar E, Greenwood M. 3. Berridge KC, Robinson TE. What is the role of dopamine in re- Sugar and fat: sensory and hedonic evaluation of liquid and solid ward: hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain foods. Physiology & behavior. 1989;45(1):177–83. research reviews. 1998;28(3):309–69. 26. Rodin J, Bartoshuk L, Peterson C, Schank D. Bulimia and taste: pos- 4. Carnell S, Wardle J. Appetite and adiposity in children: evidence for sible interactions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 1990;99(1):32. a behavioral susceptibility theory of obesity. The American journal 27. Bohon C, Stice E. Reward abnormalities among women with full of clinical nutrition. 2008;88(1):22–9. and subthreshold bulimia nervosa: a functional magnetic resonance 5. Turton R, Chami R, Treasure J. Emotional eating, binge eating and imaging study (in English). International Journal of Eating animal models of binge-type eating disorders. Current obesity re- Disorders. 2011;44(7):585–95. ports. 2017;6(2):217–28. 28. Frank GK, Reynolds JR, Shott ME, O'Reilly RC. Altered temporal 6. Brewerton TD. Are eating disorders addictions? In: Eating disor- difference learning in bulimia nervosa. Biological psychiatry. ders, addictions and substance use disorders. Berlin: Springer; 2011;70(8):728–35. 2014. p. 267–99. 29. Oberndorfer TA, et al. Altered insula response to sweet taste pro- 7. Ziauddeen H, Farooqi IS, Fletcher PC. Obesity and the brain: how cessing after recovery from anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Am J convincing is the addiction model? Nature Reviews Neuroscience. Psychiatry. 2013;170(10):1143–51. 2012;13(4):279. 30. Radeloff D, et al. High-fat taste challenge reveals altered striatal 8. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Preliminary validation response in women recovered from bulimia nervosa: a pilot study. of the Yale food addiction scale. Appetite. 2009;52(2):430–6. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2014;15(4):307–16. 264 Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 31. Haedt-Matt AA, Keel PK. Revisiting the affect regulation model of the natural environment. Journal of consulting and clinical psychol- ogy. 2007;75(4):629. binge eating: a meta-analysis of studies using ecological momen- tary assessment. Psychological bulletin. 2011;137(4):660. 52. Selby EA, et al. Momentary emotion surrounding bulimic behav- 32. Halmi KA, Sunday S, Puglisi A, Marchi P. Hunger and satiety in iors in women with bulimia nervosa and borderline personality anorexia and bulimia nervosa. Annals of the New York Academy of disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 2012;46(11):1492–500. Sciences. 1989;575(1):431–45. 53. Braun DL, Sunday S, Halmi K. Psychiatric comorbidity in patients 33. Ely AV, et al. Response in taste circuitry is not modulated by hunger with eating disorders. Psychological medicine. 1994;24(4):859–67. and satiety in women remitted from bulimia nervosa. Journal of 54. Pursey KM, Davis C, Burrows TL. Nutritional aspects of food abnormal psychology. 2017;126(5):519. addiction. Current Addiction Reports. 2017;4(2):142–50. 34. Sun X, et al. Basolateral amygdala response to food cues in the 55. Curtis C, Davis C. A qualitative study of binge eating and obesity absence of hunger is associated with weight gain susceptibility. from an addiction perspective. Eating disorders. 2014;22(1):19–32. Journal of Neuroscience. 2015;35(20):7964–76. 56. Schulte EM, Avena NM, Gearhardt AN. Which foods may be ad- 35. Avena NM, Rada P, Hoebel BG. Evidence for sugar addiction: dictive? The roles of processing, fat content, and glycemic load. behavioral and neurochemical effects of intermittent, excessive sug- PloS one. 2015;10(2):e0117959. ar intake. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2008;32(1):20– 57. Avena NM, Rada P, Moise N, Hoebel B. Sucrose sham feeding on a binge schedule releases accumbens dopamine repeatedly and elim- 36. Iemolo A, et al. Withdrawal from chronic, intermittent access to a inates the acetylcholine satiety response. Neuroscience. highly palatable food induces depressive-like behavior in compul- 2006;139(3):813–20. sive eating rats. Behavioural pharmacology. 2012;23:593. 58. Berridge KC, Ho C-Y, Richard JM, DiFeliceantonio AG. The 37. Wideman C, Nadzam G, Murphy H. Implications of an animal tempted brain eats: pleasure and desire circuits in obesity and eating model of sugar addiction, withdrawal and relapse for human health. disorders. Brain research. 2010;1350:43–64. Nutritional neuroscience. 2005;8(5–6):269–76. 59. Zhou Q-Y, Palmiter RD. Dopamine-deficient mice are severely 38. Bocarsly ME, Berner LA, Hoebel BG, Avena NM. Rats that binge hypoactive, adipsic, and aphagic. Cell. 1995;83(7):1197–209. eat fat-rich food do not show somatic signs or anxiety associated 60. Berridge KC, Valenstein ES. What psychological process mediates with opiate-like withdrawal: implications for nutrient-specific food feeding evoked by electrical stimulation of the lateral hypothala- addiction behaviors. Physiology & behavior. 2011;104(5):865–72. mus? Behavioral neuroscience. 1991;105(1):3. 39.� Schulte EM, Smeal JK, Lewis J, Gearhardt AN. Development of 61. Woods SC, Lotter EC, McKay LD, Porte D Jr. Chronic intra- the highly processed food withdrawal scale. Appetite. 2018;131: cerebroventricular infusion of insulin reduces food intake and body 148–54 This paper describes the development and validation of weight of baboons. Nature. 1979;282(5738):503. the Highly Processed Food Withdrawal Scale (ProWS), which 62. Figlewicz D, Evans S, Murphy J, Hoen M, Baskin D. Expression of is the first validated psychometric scale to measure withdrawal receptors for insulin and leptin in the ventral tegmental symptoms in the context of food addiction. This paper presents area/substantia nigra (VTA/SN) of the rat. Brain research. data supporting the convergent validity of the ProWS with 2003;964(1):107–15. addictive-like eating and the predictive validity of the ProWS 63. Pardini AW, et al. Distribution of insulin receptor substrate-2 in for dieting success. brain areas involved in energy homeostasis. Brain research. 40. Ziauddeen H, Fletcher PC. Is food addiction a valid and useful 2006;1112(1):169–78. concept? obesity reviews. 2013;14(1):19–28. 64. Figlewicz DP, Szot P, Chavez M, Woods SC, Veith RC. 41. Hansen S. Conceptualizing bulimia as addiction: a resident’sper- Intraventricular insulin increases dopamine transporter mRNA in sonal experience. American Journal of Psychiatry Residents' rat VTA/substantia nigra. Brain research. 1994;644(2):331–4. Journal. 2016;11(08):6–8. 65. Williams JM, et al. Hypoinsulinemia regulates amphetamine- 42. Davis C, Carter JC. Compulsive overeating as an addiction disor- induced reverse transport of dopamine. PLoS biology. der. A review of theory and evidence. Appetite. 2009;53(1):1–8. 2007;5(10):e274. 43. Thornley S, McRobbie H. Carbohydrate withdrawal: is recognition 66. Carvelli L, et al. PI 3-kinase regulation of dopamine uptake. Journal the first step to recovery? The New Zealand Medical Journal of neurochemistry. 2002;81(4):859–69. (Online). 2009;122(1290). 67. Labouèbe G, et al. Insulin induces long-term depression of ventral 44. Gearhardt AN, Corbin WR, Brownell KD. Development of the tegmental area dopamine neurons via endocannabinoids. Nature Yale Food Addiction Scale Version 2.0. Psychology of Addictive neuroscience. 2013;16(3):300. Behaviors. 2016;30(1):113. 68. Bruijnzeel AW, Corrie LW, Rogers JA, Yamada H. Effects of insu- 45. Ifland J, et al. Refined food addiction: a classic substance use dis- lin and leptin in the ventral tegmental area and arcuate hypothalam- order. Medical hypotheses. 2009;72(5):518–26. ic nucleus on food intake and brain reward function in female rats. 46. Lingswiler VM, Crowther JH, Stephens M. Affective and cognitive Behavioural brain research. 2011;219(2):254–64. antecedents to eating episodes in bulimia and binge eating. 69. Mebel DM, Wong JC, Dong YJ, Borgland SL. Insulin in the ventral International Journal of Eating Disorders. 1989;8(5):533–9. tegmental area reduces hedonic feeding and suppresses dopamine 47. Gilbert DG, Gilbert BO, Schultz VL. Withdrawal symptoms: indi- concentration via increased reuptake. European Journal of vidual differences and similarities across addictive behaviors. Neuroscience. 2012;36(3):2336–46. Personality and individual Differences. 1998;24(3):351–6. 70. Figlewicz DP, Bennett J, Evans SB, Kaiyala K, Sipols AJ, Benoit 48. Farrell M. Opiate withdrawal. Addiction. 1994;89(11):1471–5. SC. Intraventricular insulin and leptin reverse place preference con- 49. Tekol Y. Salt addiction: a different kind of drug addiction. Medical ditioned with high-fat diet in rats. Behavioral neuroscience. hypotheses. 2006;67(5):1233–4. 2004;118(3):479. 50. Anestis MD, Selby EA, Crosby RD, Wonderlich SA, Engel SG, 71. Dunn JP, et al. Relationship of dopamine type 2 receptor binding Joiner TE. A comparison of retrospective self-report versus ecolog- potential with fasting neuroendocrine hormones and insulin sensi- ical momentary assessment measures of affective lability in the tivity in human obesity. Diabetes care. 2012:DC_112250. examination of its relationship with bulimic symptomatology. 72. Anthony K, et al. Attenuation of insulin-evoked responses in brain Behaviour Research and Therapy. 2010;48(7):607–13. networks controlling appetite and reward in insulin resistance: the 51. Smyth JM, et al. Daily and momentary mood and stress are associ- cerebral basis for impaired control of food intake in metabolic syn- ated with binge eating and vomiting in bulimia nervosa patients in drome? Diabetes. 2006;55(11):2986–92. Curr Addict Rep (2019) 6:258–265 265 73. Volkow ND, et al. “Nonhedonic” food motivation in humans in- 82. Wang G-J, et al. BMI modulates calorie-dependent dopamine changes in accumbens from glucose intake. PloS one. 2014;9(7): volves dopamine in the dorsal striatum and methylphenidate am- plifies this effect. Synapse. 2002;44(3):175–80. e101585. 83. Burger KS, Stice E. Greater striatopallidal adaptive coding during 74. Chechlacz M, et al. Diabetes dietary management alters responses cue–reward learning and food reward habituation predict future to food pictures in brain regions associated with motivation and weight gain. Neuroimage. 2014;99:122–8. emotion: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. 84. Stice E, Yokum S, Blum K, Bohon C. Weight gain is associated Diabetologia. 2009;52(3):524. with reduced striatal response to palatable food. Journal of 75. MacDonald IA. A review of recent evidence relating to sugars, Neuroscience. 2010;30(39):13105–9. insulin resistance and diabetes. European journal of nutrition. 85.�� Ilyas A, et al. The metabolic underpinning of eating disorders: a 2016;55(2):17–23. systematic review and meta-analysis of insulin sensitivity. 76. van Golen LW, et al. Effects of insulin detemir and NPH insulin on Molecular and cellular endocrinology. 2018; This paper presents body weight and appetite-regulating brain regions in human type 1 a systematic review and meta-analysis of variations in insulin diabetes: a randomized controlled trial. Plos one. 2014;9(4):e94483. sensitivity across the spectrum of eating disorders. The meta- 77. Hermansen K, Fontaine P, Kukolja K, Peterkova V, Leth G, Gall M- analysis supported greater insulin sensitivity in anorexia A. Insulin analogues (insulin detemir and insulin aspart) versus nervosa and decreased insulin sensitivity in populations with traditional human insulins (NPH insulin and regular human insulin) bulimia nervosa and binge eating disorder. in basal-bolus therapy for patients with type 1 diabetes. 86. Monteleone P, Bortolotti F, Fabrazzo M, La Rocca A, Fuschino A, Diabetologia. 2004;47(4):622–9. Maj M. Plasma leptin response to acute fasting and refeeding in 78. Jastreboff AM, Sinha R, Lacadie C, Small DM, Sherwin RS, untreated women with bulimia nervosa. The Journal of Clinical Potenza MN. Neural correlates of stress-and food cue-induced food Endocrinology & Metabolism. 2000;85(7):2499–503. craving in obesity: association with insulin levels. Diabetes care. 87. Frank GK, et al. Altered brain activity in women recovered from 2013;36(2):394–402. bulimic-type eating disorders after a glucose challenge: a pilot 79. Haltia LT, et al. Effects of intravenous glucose on dopaminergic study. Int J Eat Disord. 2006;39(1):76–9. function in the human brain in vivo. Synapse. 2007;61(9):748–56. 80. Vilsbøll T, Krarup T, Madsbad S, Holst JJ. Both GLP-1 and GIP are Publisher’sNote Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to insulinotropic at basal and postprandial glucose levels and contrib- jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. ute nearly equally to the incretin effect of a meal in healthy subjects. Regulatory peptides. 2003;114(2–3):115–21. 81. Flint A, Raben A, Astrup A, Holst JJ. Glucagon-like peptide 1 promotes satiety and suppresses energy intake in humans. The Journal of clinical investigation. 1998;101(3):515–20.

Journal

Current Addiction ReportsSpringer Journals

Published: Jun 25, 2019

There are no references for this article.