Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 14-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

Can False Memories Prime Problem Solutions for Healthy Older Adults and Those With Alzheimer’s Disease?

Can False Memories Prime Problem Solutions for Healthy Older Adults and Those With Alzheimer’s... Abstract Objective Recent research has shown that false memories can have a positive consequence on human cognition in both children and young adults. The present experiment investigated whether false memories could have similar positive effects by priming solutions to insight-based problems in healthy older adults and people with Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Method Participants were asked to solve compound remote associate task (CRAT) problems, half of which had been preceded by the presentation of Deese/Roediger-McDermott (DRM) lists whose critical lures (CL) were also the solutions to those problems. Results The results showed that regardless of cognitive ability, when the CL was falsely recognized, CRAT problems were solved more often and reliably faster than problems that were not primed by a DRM list. When the CL was not falsely recognized, CRAT problem solution rates and times were no different from when there was no DRM priming. Discussion These findings are consistent with predictions from theories of associative activation and demonstrate the importance of automatic spreading activation processes in memory across the life span. Alzheimer’s disease, Compound remote associates task, DRM paradigm, False memory, Priming problem solving Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is characterized by a progressive deterioration of cognitive functioning, and memory disorders are considered to be the earliest and the more serious clinical symptoms of this disease. At the beginning of the disease people with AD commonly exhibit impairments in episodic memory and rapidly forget newly learned information (Baudic, Tzortzis, & Barba, 2004). However, numerous neuropsychological studies have reported evidence that semantic memory impairment may occur relatively early in the course of AD. For example, people with AD retrieve fewer items from a given semantic or letter category on timed verbal fluency tasks (Ober, Dronkers, Koss, Delis, & Friedland, 1986) and perform more poorly on object naming tasks, particularly those involving less common objects with lower frequency names (Kirshner, Webb, & Kelly, 1984). Despite the unequivocal evidence of semantic deficits in AD, a controversy remains as to whether the semantic deficit stems from a loss of information in the semantic store (Chertkow, Bub, & Seidenberg, 1989), or whether the store of semantic memory remains intact in AD, and the deficit is related to a disturbance in an AD patient’s ability to access and manipulate semantic information (Nebes, 1989). The semantic priming paradigm has been used to look at changes in semantic memory in AD. Most studies of semantic priming use a lexical decision task. The lexical decision task is a computerized task composed of a number of trials, each of which is composed of two events: a prime and a target. The prime is often a word for which no response is required. Later, when the target appears on the screen, the participant has to decide as quickly and accurately as possible if it is a real word (e.g., chair) or not (e.g., ignul). The temporal interval between the presentation of the prime and the presentation of the target is called the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). The SOA ideally is short, from a few tens to a few hundreds of milliseconds. When the targets are words, a few of them share a semantic relationship with the prime (e.g., the prime “table” followed by the target “chair”), whereas the others are not related to the prime (elephant-table). The priming effect corresponds to the reduction of the response time or the percentage of errors in the trials where the prime and the target are related compared to those where the words do not share a semantic relationship. Semantic priming effects are generally viewed within the framework of automatic spreading activation in the semantic network. That is, the related prime word activates the subsequently presented target word through their associative links in semantic memory (e.g., Collins & Loftus, 1975). Thus, the presentation of a prime automatically activates related nodes, increasing their accessibility. Therefore, when the prime and target are related, the target word is likely to have received this prior activation and will be recognized more quickly and accurately. This automatic preactivation of the related words in the semantic network is the cause of the observed facilitation. Semantic priming effects have been investigated in patients with AD, with conflicting results. For example, some researchers have found less-than-normal priming (Ober & Shenaut, 1988), other studies report equivalent priming for AD patients and healthy older adults, with researchers concluding that semantic memory structures are relatively intact in people with AD (Balota & Duchek, 1991; Chertkow et al., 1989; Nebes et al., 1989). Some studies have reported a combination of no priming and normal priming effects (Albert & Milberg, 1989), other studies have reported both normal priming and hyperpriming (greater than normal priming) with researchers concluding either that attentional abnormalities play a role in performance on semantic priming tasks (Hartman, 1991) or that degradation of the semantic network is responsible, where degraded concepts have more to gain from spread of activation than nondegraded concepts (e.g. Chertkow et al., 1989). In addition to semantic and episodic deficits, people with AD exhibit a higher incidence of memory distortions compared to their cognitively healthy older peers. These memory distortions can be severe, such as confabulation (Nedjam, Devouch, & Dalla Barba, 2004), though generally they are more mundane. For example, people with AD may have thought that they had turned off their stove when they simply misremembered that they turned off the stove. Organizational strategies, such as using pillboxes for medication, can help in the remembering of daily living activities. However, this type of strategy does not help when people with AD experience false memories—not looking in their pillbox, for example, because they falsely remember taking their medication. The Deese/Roediger-McDermott paradigm (DRM; Deese, 1959; Roediger & McDermott, 1995) is probably the most popular and commonly used paradigm to study false memories in a laboratory setting. Here, participants are presented with a list of words all of which are associatively related (e.g., table, sit, legs) and are also associated with a critical lure (CL) word that is never presented (i.e., chair). When memory is subsequently tested using recall or recognition tasks, healthy participants show a tendency to falsely recall and recognize the CLs as having been presented on the list (Balota et al., 1999; McDermott, 1999). Several studies have used the DRM paradigm with AD people and shown that they tend to produce fewer CLs than control participants (e.g., Balota et al., 1999; Budson et al., 2002; Gallo, Bell, Beier, & Schacter, 2006; Waldie & Kwong See, 2003), although the reverse has also been observed (see Watson, Balota, & Sergent-Marshall, 2001). Some studies have shown no reliable differences in CL production in people with AD and healthy controls (Roediger, Balota, & Watson, 2001). There are a number of theories that can explain the lower production of CLs in people with AD. The activation-monitoring theory (see Roediger, Watson, McDermott, & Gallo, 2001) is one of the more dominant explanations (see Gallo, 2010 for a review). According to this theory, false memories arise due to two distinct processes: an activation process and a source-monitoring process. For example, in the DRM task, because the presentation of each of the lists of words automatically activates the related but unpresented CL, the CL is activated multiple times via an automatic spread of activation within the associative network. The sum of this activation increases the feeling of familiarity for this item, while simultaneously reducing the ability to remember the source of its activation (source-monitoring process). Thus, the production of the CL may result from its erroneous attribution to an external source. In healthy aging, any increase in false memories is often explained by a source-monitoring failure (Schacter et al., 1997). The activation-monitoring theory can also be used as a framework to explain the low production of CLs in people with AD in the DRM task. Several studies have reported a failure of the source-monitoring process in AD people (Rosa, Deason, Budson, & Gutchess, 2015). Thus, it seems implausible to attribute their lower production of CLs, compared with healthy older adults, to a more efficient source monitoring. Budson, Daffner, Desikan, and Schacter (2000) and later Gallo (2010) proposed that the activation of target items in people with AD during the presentation of the DRM lists does not spread toward the CL, due either to a disturbance of the semantic network or an attentional overload. Therefore, if the CL has not been activated during list presentation, the CL could not later be falsely remembered. However, Evrard, Colombel, Gilet, and Corson (2016) suggested that the activation of the CL is preserved in people with AD, but that its mnemonic trace does not persist long enough in memory to enable its later production, due to a decline in episodic memory. By way of summary then, healthy older adults and those with AD both experience declines in episodic memory. However, semantic memory is thought to be better preserved in healthy older adults than it is in those with AD. Thus, on the one hand, differences between these groups in false memory production may be mediated more by differences in semantic than episodic memory (Madore, Addis, & Schacter, 2015; Madore, Jing, & Schacter, 2016). That is, perhaps people with AD experience greater difficulty than their healthy counterparts with associative activation of related information within semantic networks (Budson et al., 2000; Dewhurst, Thorley, Hammond, & Ormerod, 2011; Gallo et al., 2006). On the other hand, perhaps the CL does get activated, but the time course for the decline in this activation is faster for those with AD than those without. One way to test these proposals is to introduce a second task where performance improvements on that task are predicated on the earlier production of the CL from the DRM list presentation. By way of background, there are a number of studies that have found that false memories have positive consequences on subsequent task performance. For example, McKone and Murphy (2000) showed that false memories generated using the DRM paradigm could prime performance on related memory tasks (e.g., stem-cued recall). Studies like these have prompted researchers to examine the possible beneficial effects of false memories on cognitive tasks other than those related to memory (Howe, 2011; Schacter, Guerin, & St. Jacques, 2011). Howe, Garner, Dewhurst, and Ball (2010) were the first to carry out research investigating the role that false memories play in priming insight-based problem solving using compound remote associate tasks (CRATs) (see Mednick, 1962; Sio). CRAT problems, originally developed by Mednick (1962), involve the presentation of three words (e.g., apple, family, and house) and the task is to come up with a word (i.e., tree) which, when combined with each of the three original words, creates compound words or common phrases (i.e., apple tree, family tree, treehouse). Howe et al. (2010) presented adults with DRM lists whose CLs served as potential primes for half of the subsequent CRAT problems that participants had to solve. They found that when participants falsely recalled the CLs of the studied DRM lists, the corresponding CRATs were solved more frequently and significantly faster than CRATs that had not been primed or cases in which DRM lists had been presented but CLs were not falsely remembered. Howe, Garner, Charlesworth, and Knott (2011) extended this research to children and found exactly the same results. What this research shows that like true memories, false memories can successfully prime higher-order cognitive tasks (i.e., insight-based problem solving). In the current research, we wanted to see whether these priming effects also occur in healthy older adults and, more importantly, people with AD. If this finding can be extended to people with AD, this would extend our understanding of memory processes in this sub-group of memory-impaired people and importantly, could have positive implications for memory rehabilitation. If people with AD have greater problems with spreading activation than healthy older adults (Budson et al., 2000; Gallo et al., 2006), then they should be less likely to remember the CL than healthy older adults. In addition, if false memories have a shorter “life span” for people with AD, then even when CLs are produced on the memory test, priming effects on subsequently administered CRAT problems should be attenuated or absent. However, if as some previous research suggests (Roediger et al., 2001) both healthy older adults and people with AD do have intact semantic networks and CL longevity is not an issue, then it is expected that CLs will be falsely remembered and priming will occur on subsequent CRAT problems. In the present study, we used CRAT problems whose baseline solution rates were moderate (30%–80%) for older adults. Because the CRAT norms that are available were based on solutions provided by children and young adults, we created our own age-appropriate CRAT norms prior to conducting the priming experiment. Our rationale for this was that we wanted to eliminate differences of age due simply to knowledge base, a procedure consistent with previous studies (e.g., Howe et al., 2011), as these differences were not of interest in the current research. Experiment 1: Norming CRAT Problems for Use With Older Adults Before turning to the main experiment, we report a pilot study in which we collected norms for CRAT problems for use with older adults. In line with previous research, age-appropriate CRAT problems were developed in order to eliminate any potential age effects in problem-solving performance due to the use of extant norms which were developed using samples of children and young adults. Method Participants A total of 32 healthy older adults (13 males and 19 females) took part in this experiment; their mean age was 78.19 (SD = 5.67). The older adults had normal cognitive functioning (as assessed by the Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE]; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) with a mean score of 27.31 (SD = 2.52), normal activities of daily living, and most importantly, did not meet diagnostic criteria for dementia. These older adults were volunteers who were community dwelling and were tested in their own home or local community center. Materials Older adults were presented with 20 CRAT problems taken from the Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003) norms. The items on the CRATs required a solution that was associated with all three words of the triad through the construction of a compound word or common phrase (e.g., cream, skate, and water combined with the solution word ice, creating the compounds ice cream, ice skate, and ice water). (Note that only problems with solution rates above 30% and solved within 30 s were selected for subsequent use with older adults with AD.) All the solution words had a familiarity rating of 500 or above (with a maximum entry of 645 and a mean of 566 [Coltheart, 1981]) and a word frequency of 10 or above (with a maximum entry of 686 and a mean of 126 [Kucera & Francis. 1967]). Procedure Participants were tested individually in a quiet room. Instructions similar to those used by Howe et al. (2011) and by Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003) were given. Specifically, participants were told that they would see three words and that they should try and produce a fourth word that, when combined with each of the three items, would make up a common compound word or phrase. Participants were first given three demonstrations by the experimenter followed by two practice problems prior to the experiment itself. The three problem words were presented on a computer laptop screen simultaneously in a horizontal orientation, with one word above, below, and at the center fixation point. Participants were given 30 s to produce the solution (this was a verbal solution). If the solution was produced within the time limit, both the solution word and solution time were recorded and the next problem was presented. If participants did not produce the correct response within the time limit, the solution was provided by the experimenter and the program automatically moved to the next problem. Results Table 1 shows the average solution rates and times for the 20 problems separately. As can be seen, older adults were able to solve most of these CRAT problems. Importantly, for the next experiment, there was a good range of solution rates and times for these CRAT problems. What this means is that priming effects, should they exist, can be measured without constraints imposed by floor and ceiling effects. Table 1. CRAT Problems: Solution Rates and Times CRAT problem CRAT solution % Solved Solution time (s) Heart/potato/tooth Sweet 18.8 30.2 (12.24) Over/deep/Walk Sleep 21.9 23.7 (17.45) Skin/ball/tissue Soft 25 13.5 (10.02) Bike/top/goat Mountain 34.4 20.1 (17.68) Base/tank/territorial Army 40.6 21.3 (18.03) Cut/sore/war Cold 43.8 21.9 (18.86) Hold/print/stool Foot 43.8 18.6 (13.62) List/death/Bone Wish 46.9 19.2 (13.84) Polo/flannel/vest Shirt 46.9 24.5 (20.69) Bow/Haul/jump Long 53.2 18.6 (16.09) Bowl/Juice/Salad Fruit 53.2 11.6 (9.73) Measuring/cake/tea Cup 59.4 18.5 (13.97) Pal/tip/knife Pen 62.5 14.3 (12.50) Bite/vein/web Spider 68.8 17.1 (13.63) Jack/magic/board Black 75 13.5 (12.48) Crumbs/dough/Knife Bread 81.3 13.4 (12.54) Care/spa/mental Health 87 10.2 (7.73) Knitting/stick/pine Needle 87.5 16.2 (14.52) Sill/frame/cleaner Window 87.6 11.8 (14.09) School/Chair/Heels High 90.6 12.8 (13.63) Rocking/wheel/high Chair 93.7 9.8 (11.71) CRAT problem CRAT solution % Solved Solution time (s) Heart/potato/tooth Sweet 18.8 30.2 (12.24) Over/deep/Walk Sleep 21.9 23.7 (17.45) Skin/ball/tissue Soft 25 13.5 (10.02) Bike/top/goat Mountain 34.4 20.1 (17.68) Base/tank/territorial Army 40.6 21.3 (18.03) Cut/sore/war Cold 43.8 21.9 (18.86) Hold/print/stool Foot 43.8 18.6 (13.62) List/death/Bone Wish 46.9 19.2 (13.84) Polo/flannel/vest Shirt 46.9 24.5 (20.69) Bow/Haul/jump Long 53.2 18.6 (16.09) Bowl/Juice/Salad Fruit 53.2 11.6 (9.73) Measuring/cake/tea Cup 59.4 18.5 (13.97) Pal/tip/knife Pen 62.5 14.3 (12.50) Bite/vein/web Spider 68.8 17.1 (13.63) Jack/magic/board Black 75 13.5 (12.48) Crumbs/dough/Knife Bread 81.3 13.4 (12.54) Care/spa/mental Health 87 10.2 (7.73) Knitting/stick/pine Needle 87.5 16.2 (14.52) Sill/frame/cleaner Window 87.6 11.8 (14.09) School/Chair/Heels High 90.6 12.8 (13.63) Rocking/wheel/high Chair 93.7 9.8 (11.71) Note: Standard deviation is in parenthesis and solution times are presented. CRAT = Compound remote associate task. View Large Table 1. CRAT Problems: Solution Rates and Times CRAT problem CRAT solution % Solved Solution time (s) Heart/potato/tooth Sweet 18.8 30.2 (12.24) Over/deep/Walk Sleep 21.9 23.7 (17.45) Skin/ball/tissue Soft 25 13.5 (10.02) Bike/top/goat Mountain 34.4 20.1 (17.68) Base/tank/territorial Army 40.6 21.3 (18.03) Cut/sore/war Cold 43.8 21.9 (18.86) Hold/print/stool Foot 43.8 18.6 (13.62) List/death/Bone Wish 46.9 19.2 (13.84) Polo/flannel/vest Shirt 46.9 24.5 (20.69) Bow/Haul/jump Long 53.2 18.6 (16.09) Bowl/Juice/Salad Fruit 53.2 11.6 (9.73) Measuring/cake/tea Cup 59.4 18.5 (13.97) Pal/tip/knife Pen 62.5 14.3 (12.50) Bite/vein/web Spider 68.8 17.1 (13.63) Jack/magic/board Black 75 13.5 (12.48) Crumbs/dough/Knife Bread 81.3 13.4 (12.54) Care/spa/mental Health 87 10.2 (7.73) Knitting/stick/pine Needle 87.5 16.2 (14.52) Sill/frame/cleaner Window 87.6 11.8 (14.09) School/Chair/Heels High 90.6 12.8 (13.63) Rocking/wheel/high Chair 93.7 9.8 (11.71) CRAT problem CRAT solution % Solved Solution time (s) Heart/potato/tooth Sweet 18.8 30.2 (12.24) Over/deep/Walk Sleep 21.9 23.7 (17.45) Skin/ball/tissue Soft 25 13.5 (10.02) Bike/top/goat Mountain 34.4 20.1 (17.68) Base/tank/territorial Army 40.6 21.3 (18.03) Cut/sore/war Cold 43.8 21.9 (18.86) Hold/print/stool Foot 43.8 18.6 (13.62) List/death/Bone Wish 46.9 19.2 (13.84) Polo/flannel/vest Shirt 46.9 24.5 (20.69) Bow/Haul/jump Long 53.2 18.6 (16.09) Bowl/Juice/Salad Fruit 53.2 11.6 (9.73) Measuring/cake/tea Cup 59.4 18.5 (13.97) Pal/tip/knife Pen 62.5 14.3 (12.50) Bite/vein/web Spider 68.8 17.1 (13.63) Jack/magic/board Black 75 13.5 (12.48) Crumbs/dough/Knife Bread 81.3 13.4 (12.54) Care/spa/mental Health 87 10.2 (7.73) Knitting/stick/pine Needle 87.5 16.2 (14.52) Sill/frame/cleaner Window 87.6 11.8 (14.09) School/Chair/Heels High 90.6 12.8 (13.63) Rocking/wheel/high Chair 93.7 9.8 (11.71) Note: Standard deviation is in parenthesis and solution times are presented. CRAT = Compound remote associate task. View Large Experiment 2: Examining Priming Effects in Older Healthy Adults and Those With AD With these norms in hand, we now turn to the main question concerning the role of false memories in priming solutions to insight-based problem solving in healthy older adults and people with AD. Method Participants A new sample of 60 participants was recruited whose demographic and other characteristics are shown in Table 2. A statistical power analysis was performed for sample size estimation. The effect size in this study was considered to be medium using Cohen’s (1988) criteria. With an α = 0.05 and power = 0.80, our sample size of 60 (30 participants per group) is considered more than adequate. Thirty participants had a clinical diagnosis of probable or possible AD (McKhann et al., 1984). Patients were diagnosed as being demented with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, third edition (DSM III-R; American Psychiatric Association, 1987) criteria as having AD by the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorder and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS-ADRDA) criteria (McKhann et al., 1984). AD was diagnosed by a clinician using neuropsychological examination, MMSE (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975), family interview, laboratory screening (i.e., hematology; B12 and folate levels; renal, liver, and thyroid function; calcium and syphilis serology), and medical examination. If there was a suggestion of a psychiatric disorder, patients were also assessed by a psychiatrist. Patients with a history of stroke or depression were excluded from this study. Patients with a Hachinski score (Hachinski et al., 1975) that indicated they might have vascular component to their dementia were also excluded. Thirty participants made up an older adult control (OAC) group. These people were community dwelling and were recruited from a panel of older adults who had expressed an interest in participating in research (n = 18), or were recruited from Age Concern London (n = 12). All OAC participants had recently been screened for dementia using the MMSE, all scoring above the standard cutoff of 26/30. Table 2. Means (and SE) Demographic Characteristics of Participants AD OAC MMSE 19.54 (0.55) 27.39 (0.58) NART 106.54 (1.79) 116.78 (0.95) Education 10.02 (0.27) 12.32 (0.42) Age 76.43 (1.13) 77.32 (0.84) AD OAC MMSE 19.54 (0.55) 27.39 (0.58) NART 106.54 (1.79) 116.78 (0.95) Education 10.02 (0.27) 12.32 (0.42) Age 76.43 (1.13) 77.32 (0.84) Note: AD = Alzheimer’s disease; OAC = Older adult control; MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination Score. Predicted IQ from the National Adult Reading Tests (NART). Education = Years of formal education. View Large Table 2. Means (and SE) Demographic Characteristics of Participants AD OAC MMSE 19.54 (0.55) 27.39 (0.58) NART 106.54 (1.79) 116.78 (0.95) Education 10.02 (0.27) 12.32 (0.42) Age 76.43 (1.13) 77.32 (0.84) AD OAC MMSE 19.54 (0.55) 27.39 (0.58) NART 106.54 (1.79) 116.78 (0.95) Education 10.02 (0.27) 12.32 (0.42) Age 76.43 (1.13) 77.32 (0.84) Note: AD = Alzheimer’s disease; OAC = Older adult control; MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination Score. Predicted IQ from the National Adult Reading Tests (NART). Education = Years of formal education. View Large As noted, the demographic characteristics of the participants are shown in Table 2. One-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were used to analyze these demographic variables. These analyses showed that there were no significant differences between groups in mean age, F < 1. There was a significant difference in the National Adult Reading Test (NART) predicted IQ scores (Nelson, 1982) for the groups, F(1, 58) = 18.56, p < .001, with the AD group scoring reliably lower than the OACs. There was also a significant difference in the mean number of years of formal education between groups, F(1, 58) = 12.01, p < .001, with the OACs having reliably more years of formal education. For MMSE Score (Folstein et al., 1975), as expected, there was also a significant difference between groups, F(1,58) = 56.21, p < .001, with the people with AD performing reliably lower that the OACs. In addition, people with AD were also tested on the word list memory test from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) neuropsychology battery (Morris et al., 1993; Welsh, Butters, Hughes, Mohs, & Heyman, 1991). For delayed recall, they recalled an average of 0.8 (SD = 1.92) words. This level of performance on this particular task is in keeping with other reports for people with AD in the literature (e.g., Akhtar, Moulin, & Bowie, 2006; Moulin, James, Freeman, & Jones, 2004), where mean performance is typically less than one for people with AD. Mean normal performance is around six items (Moulin et al., 2004). Older healthy adults gave their written informed consent. For people with AD, written informed consent was given either by them or their primary caregivers. The OACs were community dwelling and people with AD were recruited from two day care centers in London. All participants were fluent in English. Design, materials, and procedure A 2 (Group: AD vs OAC) × 2 (Priming: primed vs unprimed) mixed design was used, where the first factor was between-participants and the second was a within-participant factor. For purposes of the analyses, primed items were further divided according to whether participants falsely remembered the CL (designated “primed/FM”) or did not falsely remember the CL (designated “primed/no-FM”). This resulted in a 2 (Group) × 3 (Priming) design. We followed the same procedure as Howe et al. (2010, 2011) such that each participant was primed on half the subsequent CRAT problems with preceding DRM lists whose CLs were also the solution to those CRAT problems. The order of both the DRM lists and CRAT problems was carefully counterbalanced to eliminate order effects. Ten CRAT problems were selected from the normative data in Experiment 1 (see Supplementary Appendix). Their nonprimed solution rates ranged between 30% and 80% in order to insure that they were neither too easy nor too hard. In addition, 10 DRM lists whose CLs were the corresponding solutions to these CRATs were selected. Each list contained 10 word associates of the CL and were taken from Roediger et al. (2001) norms. Lists were randomly divided into two groups of five with participants being primed using one of these sets. In order to prevent differences in false memory rates, the two sets of five DRM lists were equated on backward associative strength (BAS) (List Set 1 BAS = 0.777, List Set 2 BAS = 0.725). Participants were given one set of the five DRM lists in a randomized order. Each list was presented verbally, followed by a distractor task (counting backward by four from a 3-digit number for 30 s). This was followed by a recognition test similar to that in Howe, Wilkinson, Garner, and Ball (2016) whereby participants were verbally presented with the 5 CL words from the studied DRM lists, 5 unstudied and unrelated CLs, 32 true items from the studied DRM lists, 32 foils unrelated to studied DRM lists, and 8 filler items. A recognition test was implemented rather than a recall test to reduce effects of priming during retrieval (Olszewska & Ulatowska, 2013). For each word presented in the recognition task, participants had to select either [O], indicating that the word was Old and that they recognize the word from the previously presented lists, or [N] if they thought the current word presented was a new word that they did not hear in the previous word lists. Following completion of the DRM lists, all 10 CRAT problems were completed. The same procedure used for the CRAT pilot study was used for the main experiment and solution rates and solution times were collected for correctly solved CRAT problems. Results We controlled for the effects of educational level and NART statistically. In all of our analyses, we ran an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with these demographic variables as covariates. For all ANCOVAs, the pattern of findings for the main effect of group and interactions with group were unchanged from the analysis with ANOVAs. For simplicity, therefore, the ANOVAs are reported here. False memories were comparable to previous studies (Roediger, Balota, & Watson, 2001; Waldie & Kwong-See, 2003). The recognition task showed that both the OACs and people with AD created false memories for the CL words, with people with AD falsely recognizing the CL 61% (M = 3.05, SD = 1.31) of the time and the OAC group 60% (M = 3.01, SD = 0.91) of the time. There were no reliable differences. Overall recognition scores were analyzed using a 2 (Group: AD vs OAC) × 4 (list type: CLs, unstudied unrelated CLs, foils and list items) mixed model ANOVA. Analysis revealed a significant main effect of list type, F(3, 58) = 6.51, p < .001. Pairwise comparisons revealed greater recognition in list items (80%) compared to foil items (66.7%) (M = 25.63, SD = 2.51 vs M = 21.34, SD = 3.3) and greater recognition of CL words (60.6%) compared to unrelated CL words (40.4%) (M = 3.03, SD = 0.80 vs M = 2.02, SD = 1.08). There was no main effect for group (F < 1) and no interaction (F < 1). Because false alarm rates for recognition tests often require a correction for response bias, we analyzed discrimination and response bias scores using signal detection analysis. We used the Snodgrass and Corwin (1988) correction for signal detection theory (SDT) measures, whereby 0.5 was added to hit and false alarm rates and the corrected score was divided by N + 1. This was used in order to prevent values of 1.0 and 0. The Snodgrass and Corwin correction was conducted for SDT measures for all list items. For discriminability (d’), larger values indicate better memory performance, and for criterion value (C), values greater than 0 represent a conservative response bias and less than 0 represents a liberal response bias. The values of d’ and C are shown in Table 3. The calculation of d’ and C for used the false alarm rate for unrelated foils. Signal detection measures for hits and CLs were analyzed using separate independent t tests. The analysis of d’ for hits and CLs revealed no reliable differences (t < 1). Analysis of the criterion C, revealed no reliable differences for hits or CLs (t < 1). Table 3. Means and SD of Signal Detection Measures of Discriminability (d’) and Bias (C) for Hits and Critical Lures (CL) OAC AD d’ C d’ C Hits 1.42 (0.48) 0.23 (0.2) 1.13 (0.39) 0.18 (0.1) CL 0.93 (0.78) 0.22 (0.39) 0.84 (0.53) 0.18 (0.32) OAC AD d’ C d’ C Hits 1.42 (0.48) 0.23 (0.2) 1.13 (0.39) 0.18 (0.1) CL 0.93 (0.78) 0.22 (0.39) 0.84 (0.53) 0.18 (0.32) Note: AD = Alzheimer’s disease; CL = Critical lures; OAC = Older adult control. View Large Table 3. Means and SD of Signal Detection Measures of Discriminability (d’) and Bias (C) for Hits and Critical Lures (CL) OAC AD d’ C d’ C Hits 1.42 (0.48) 0.23 (0.2) 1.13 (0.39) 0.18 (0.1) CL 0.93 (0.78) 0.22 (0.39) 0.84 (0.53) 0.18 (0.32) OAC AD d’ C d’ C Hits 1.42 (0.48) 0.23 (0.2) 1.13 (0.39) 0.18 (0.1) CL 0.93 (0.78) 0.22 (0.39) 0.84 (0.53) 0.18 (0.32) Note: AD = Alzheimer’s disease; CL = Critical lures; OAC = Older adult control. View Large The mean CRAT solution rates (proportions) and the mean CRAT solution times (in seconds) were calculated for each participant and analyzed separately in a series of 2 (Group: AD vs OAC) × 3 (Priming: primed/FM vs primed/no-FM vs unprimed) ANOVAs. For primed CRAT problems, solution rates and solution times were conditionalized on whether participants had produced the CL during recall (i.e., primed/FM = CL produced and primed/no-FM = no CL produced). Thus, both solution rates and solution times were subjected to separate ANOVAs where the factors were solution type (unprimed, primed/no-FM, or primed/FM) and group. The data are shown in Table 4. Table 4. Mean CRAT Problem Solution Rates and Solution Times for Older Adults and Alzheimer’s Patients for FM Priming Priming Participant Unprimed Priming/FM Priming/NO-FM Solution times (seconds) Older Adults 36.97 (1.92) 21.20 (1.27) 37.24 (1.61) Alzheimer’s patients 44.06 (2.58) 21.98 (1.72) 39.70 (2.16) Solution rates (proportion) Older Adults 0.38 (0.24) 0.59 (0.39) 0.23 (0.15) Alzheimer’s patients 0.32 (0.31) 0.57 (0.51) 0.24 (0.31) Priming Participant Unprimed Priming/FM Priming/NO-FM Solution times (seconds) Older Adults 36.97 (1.92) 21.20 (1.27) 37.24 (1.61) Alzheimer’s patients 44.06 (2.58) 21.98 (1.72) 39.70 (2.16) Solution rates (proportion) Older Adults 0.38 (0.24) 0.59 (0.39) 0.23 (0.15) Alzheimer’s patients 0.32 (0.31) 0.57 (0.51) 0.24 (0.31) Note: Standard errors are in parenthesis. CRAT = Compound remote associate task; FM = False memory. View Large Table 4. Mean CRAT Problem Solution Rates and Solution Times for Older Adults and Alzheimer’s Patients for FM Priming Priming Participant Unprimed Priming/FM Priming/NO-FM Solution times (seconds) Older Adults 36.97 (1.92) 21.20 (1.27) 37.24 (1.61) Alzheimer’s patients 44.06 (2.58) 21.98 (1.72) 39.70 (2.16) Solution rates (proportion) Older Adults 0.38 (0.24) 0.59 (0.39) 0.23 (0.15) Alzheimer’s patients 0.32 (0.31) 0.57 (0.51) 0.24 (0.31) Priming Participant Unprimed Priming/FM Priming/NO-FM Solution times (seconds) Older Adults 36.97 (1.92) 21.20 (1.27) 37.24 (1.61) Alzheimer’s patients 44.06 (2.58) 21.98 (1.72) 39.70 (2.16) Solution rates (proportion) Older Adults 0.38 (0.24) 0.59 (0.39) 0.23 (0.15) Alzheimer’s patients 0.32 (0.31) 0.57 (0.51) 0.24 (0.31) Note: Standard errors are in parenthesis. CRAT = Compound remote associate task; FM = False memory. View Large Concerning solution times, there was a main effect for priming F(2, 58) =15.26, p < .001, η2p = .244, where post hoc tests (Tukey’s) showed that solution times were faster for primed/FM problems (M = 21.49) compared to primed/No-FM problems (M = 38.12, p < .01) and unprimed CRAT problems (M = 38.50, p = < .01), and the latter two conditions did not differ. Furthermore, solution time results showed no significant difference across participant groups, with OAC’s average problem-solving time being 31.39 s (SE = 1.132) and AD’s being 34.09 s (SE = 1.519). There was no interaction. Concerning solution rates, there was a main effect for priming F(2, 58) = 15.26, p < .001, η2p = .248, where post hoc tests (Tukey’s LSD) showed that solution rates were higher for primed/FM CRAT problems (M = 0.52) than for primed/no-FM (M = 0.23) and when participants were unprimed (M = 0.26), and the latter two did not differ. There was no main effect for group, where OACs (M = 0.42) and AD (M = 0.39) solved similar numbers of CRAT problems and no interaction. Discussion The present study set out to investigate whether false memories can have a positive consequence on human cognition with older healthy adults and those with AD, as has been shown in children and young adults (Howe et al., 2010, 2011). To investigate this, participants were asked to solve CRAT problems, half of which had been preceded by the presentation of DRM lists whose CLs were also the solutions to those problems. Consistent with previous research, our study showed no reliable differences in the number of false memories produced in the recognition task (Roediger, Balota, & Watson, 2001; Waldie & Kwong-See, 2003). This finding can be explained by the fact that both older healthy adults and those with AD have intact semantic networks that automatically activate CLs upon DRM list presentation. Our findings support existing evidence regarding the underlying mechanisms in the production of false memories (Roediger et al., 2001). Previous research has shown the generation of false memories from the automatic spread of activation within the semantic networks and the corresponding activations of word associations. The findings from the present study further extend this notion, providing evidence that not only are false memories associated with the spreading activation among semantic associates but essentially act similarly to true memories when it comes to priming subsequent task performance (McDermott, 1999). Furthermore, when a recognition test is administered in this priming paradigm, endorsement of the false memory item versus no endorsement is an index of the strength of activation of the CL in memory. That is, no recognition = below threshold activation and recognition = above threshold activation. Although false memories arise at encoding, test performance reveals the strength of that activation. It also turns out that presenting the CL at test has little to no effect on memory strength of the CL because, as already mentioned, false memories arise during the encoding not retrieval process (see Howe et al., 2016). Our findings are the first to show that false memories can successfully prime insight-based problem solving in both AD and OACs. Just like in Howe et al. (2011), we propose when problem solutions were primed by the prior presentation of DRM lists whose CLs were falsely remembered and were solutions to those problems, critically both the probability of such problems being solved and the speed with which they were solved improved significantly. This was true regardless of whether the problem solvers were people with AD or OACs. These findings strongly suggest that false memories do not “fade” more rapidly for people with AD than for OACs and they are capable of priming and facilitating performance on a subsequent problem-solving task. What is important to consider here, is the DRM lists can prime and facilitate performance on problem-solving tasks both in terms of the rate and the speed which they are solved. However, one can only make this conclusion when the CL is falsely recognized. Such facilitation is not found when the false CL has not been remembered. Interestingly, priming with no recognition of the CL resulted in problem-solving rates and times identical to conditions in which there was no priming. This adds to the growing view that false memories, like true memories, can successfully prime higher cognitive processes, at least in terms of problems involving insight-based solutions (Diliberto-Macaluso, 2005; Howe et al., 2010, 2011, 2016). Our research is the first to demonstrate that false memory priming effects occur regardless of cognitive abilities. In the present study, priming effects were equally robust in OACs and people with AD. This cognitive invariance has important theoretical implications. We suggest intact semantic networks exist in both these groups of older adults. To add strength to this argument, we compared our findings to previous research with younger adults (e.g., Howe et al., 2011, 2016). What this comparison shows is that rates of priming for younger adults in those studies is similar to those same rates for the older adults in the present study. Specifically, regardless of differences in materials and overall false memory rates, when young adults remembered the CL, their priming power for solving subsequent CRAT problems was similar to the rate for when older adults got the CL in the present research. That is, average solution times for young adults (19.22) was similar to that for older adults (21.20) as were the average solution rates for younger adults (0.76) and older adults (0.6). Thus, what our study shows is that semantic networks are relatively well preserved in people with AD and OACs, at least when compared to those same rates for younger adults in earlier research. From all of this research, it is clear that false memories generated from the DRM word lists and CRAT problem solutions arise from the nonconscious and automatic spreading of activation among semantic concepts (Balota et al., 1999; Roediger et al., 2001). Therefore, as a result of priming occurring in both of the populations studied here, spreading of activation between nodes in the semantic networks must be intact. In the extant literature, decline in performance on tasks such as verbal fluency have been attributed to the breakdown in semantic networks, particularly for people with AD (Balota et al., 1999; Watson et al., 2001). What our findings suggest is that these breakdowns are not due to the deterioration of semantic networks but rather, due to possible failures in source monitoring. Although further research is needed to confirm this hypothesis, what our study shows is that there was no decline in spreading activation within semantic networks; false memories were as frequent in people with AD as in those without AD (our OACs) and they served as equally powerful primes for both groups when solving CRAT problems. Another hypothesis worth considering could be that CRAT problems were solved via insight-like (perhaps automatic, nonconscious) strategy or via a more analytic (perhaps deliberate) strategy (e.g., Kounios et al., 2006; Kounios & Beeman 2009). These studies show that distinct brain mechanisms are involved for the two types of solutions. Although in the present study there were no differences between OACs and people with AD in using primes for solving CRAT problems, the mechanisms through which the two groups of participants reached the solution could have differed. Of course, this hypothesis would require additional research. Previous research has shown the positive consequences that false memories have problem-solving tasks in both children and adults, yet this has not been fully examined in individuals with associated cognitive decline (Howe et al., 2010). The results from the current study demonstrate for the first time the priming effects false memories have on complex insight-based problem-solving tasks such as CRATs on OACs and people with AD. Additionally, our findings add to the recent literature on the positive consequences that false memories have on human cognition, particularly in the way they facilitate performance on higher-order cognitive tasks such as the CRAT. Given that these significant results were found in both older adults and people with AD, our findings strongly suggest that significant differences that may arise in memory functioning are not the result of deterioration in spreading activating in semantic networks, at least not in the DRM/CRAT tasks. Finally, our findings have a number of important theoretical and practical implications. First, we propose that OACs and people with AD have intact semantic networks. Second, although there are clear differences between true and false memories (Roediger & McDermott, 1995) our findings add to the growing literature suggesting that false memories can work in a very similar way to those observed for true memories (Diliberto-Macaluso, 2005). Third, our findings add to an emerging consensus that false memories, just like false beliefs (Howe & Derbish, 2010), can have beneficial effects in human cognition and not simply the negative consequences we are all familiar with (see Howe & Knott, 2015). We are aware that some may interpret false memories as negative regardless of their benefits as outlined in this paper, we believe that this by-product of a powerful reconstructive memory system is positive (see Howe et al., 2010). Our findings have taken us a step closer to realizing at least one beneficial aspect of false recollection in that it helps to establish that false memories, like true memories, can and do provide significant advantages when it comes to more complex cognitive processes, specifically insight-based problem solving for both OACs, people with AD, children, and adults (Howe et al., 2011). Supplementary Material Supplementary data is available at The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences online. Funding This research was supported by a grant to MLH from the Economic and Social Research Council of Great Britain (RES-062-23-3327). Conflict of Interest None reported. References Akhtar , S. , Moulin , C. J. , & Bowie , P. C . ( 2006 ). Are people with mild cognitive impairment aware of the benefits of errorless learning ? Neuropsychological Rehabilitation , 16 , 329 – 346 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/09602010500176674 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Albert , M. , & Milberg , W . ( 1989 ). Semantic processing in patients with Alzheimer’s disease . Brain and Language , 37 , 163 – 171 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(89)90106-5 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed American Psychiatric Association . ( 1987 ). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders (DSM-III-R) . Washington DG . Anderson , J. R . ( 1983 ). A spreading activation theory of memory . Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior , 22 , 261 – 295 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90201–3 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Balota , D. A. , Cortese , M. J. , Duchek , J. M. , Adams , D. , Roediger III, H. L. , McDermott , K. B. , & Yerys , B. E . ( 1999 ). Veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 16 , 361 – 384 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/026432999380834 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Balota , D. A. , & Duchek , J. M . ( 1991 ). Semantic priming effects, lexical repetition effects, and contextual disambiguation effects in healthy aged individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type . Brain and Language , 40 , 181 – 201 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(91)90124-J Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Baudic , S. , Tzortzis , C. , Barba , G. D. , & Traykov , L . ( 2004 ). Executive deficits in elderly patients with major unipolar depression . Journal of geriatric psychiatry and neurology , 17 , 195 – 201 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0891988704269823 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Bowden , E. M. , & Jung-Beeman , M . ( 2003 ). Normative data for 144 compound remote associate problems . Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers , 35 , 634 – 639 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Budson , A. E. , Daffner , K. R. , Desikan , R. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 2000 ). When false recognition is unopposed by true recognition: Gist-based memory distortion in Alzheimer’s disease . Neuropsychology , 14 , 277 – 287 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Budson , A. E. , & Solomon , P. R . ( 2012 ). New diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment for the practical neurologist . Practical Neurology , 12 , 88 – 96 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/practneurol-2011-000145 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Budson , A. E. , Sullivan , A. L. , Mayer , E. , Daffner , K. R. , Black , P. M. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 2002 ). Suppression of false recognition in Alzheimer’s disease and in patients with frontal lobe lesions . Brain , 125 , 2750 – 2765 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Chertkow , H. , Bub , D. , & Seidenberg , M . ( 1989 ). Priming and semantic memory loss in Alzheimer’s disease . Brain and Language , 36 , 420 – 446 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(89)90078-3 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Collins , A. M. , & Loftus , E. F . ( 1975 ). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing . Psychological Review , 82 , 407 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Coltheart , M . ( 1981 ). The MRC psycholinguistic database . The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 33 , 497 – 505 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Deese , J . ( 1959 ). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall . Journal of Experimental Psychology , 58 , 17 – 22 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046671 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Dewhurst , S. A. , Thorley , C. , Hammond , E. R. , & Ormerod , T. C . ( 2011 ). Convergent, but not divergent, thinking predicts susceptibility to associative memory illusions . Personality and Individual Differences , 51 , 73 – 76 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.018 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Diliberto-Macaluso , K. A . ( 2005 ). Priming and false memories from Deese-Roediger-McDermott lists on a fragment completion test with children . The American Journal of Psychology , 118 , 13 – 28 . Google Scholar PubMed Evrard , C. , Colombel , F. , Gilet , A. L. , & Corson , Y . ( 2016 ). Intact semantic priming of critical lures in alzheimer’s disease: Implications for false memory . The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences , 71 , 671 – 674 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbv006 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Folstein , M. F. , Folstein , S. E. , & McHugh , P. R . ( 1975 ). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician . Journal of Psychiatric Research , 12 , 189 – 198 . doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Gallo , D. A . ( 2010 ). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion . Memory & Cognition , 38 , 833 – 848 . doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.7.833 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Gallo , D. A. , Bell , D. M. , Beier , J. S. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 2006 ). Two types of recollection-based monitoring in younger and older adults: Recall-to-reject and the distinctiveness heuristic . Memory (Hove, England) , 14 , 730 – 741 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210600648506 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Hachinski , V. C. , Iliff , L. D. , Zilhka , E. , Du Boulay , G. H. , McAllister , V. L. , Marshall , J. ,… Symon , L . ( 1975 ). Cerebral blood flow in dementia . Archives of Neurology , 32 , 632 – 637 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1975.00490510088009 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Hartman , M. , & Hasher , L. ( 1991 ). Aging and suppression: Memory for previously relevant information . Psychology and aging , 6 , 587 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Howe , M. L . ( 2011 ). The adaptive nature of memory and its illusions . Current Directions in Psychological Science , 20 , 312 – 315 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411416571 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Howe , M. L. , & Derbish , M. H . ( 2010 ). On the susceptibility of adaptive memory to false memory illusions . Cognition , 115 , 252 – 267 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.016 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Howe , M. L. , Garner , S. R. , Charlesworth , M. , & Knott , L . ( 2011 ). A brighter side to memory illusions: False memories prime children’s and adults’ insight-based problem solving . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 108 , 383 – 393 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.012 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Howe , M. L. , Garner , S. R. , Dewhurst , S. A. , & Ball , L. J . ( 2010 ). Can false memories prime problem solutions ? Cognition , 117 , 176 – 181 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.009 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Howe , M. L. , & Knott , L. M . ( 2015 ). The fallibility of memory in judicial processes: Lessons from the past and their modern consequences . Memory (Hove, England) , 23 , 633 – 656 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2015.1010709 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Howe , M. L. , Wilkinson , S. , Garner , S. R. , & Ball , L. J . ( 2016 ). On the adaptive function of children’s and adults’ false memories . Memory , 24 , 1062 – 1077 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2015.1068335 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Kirshner , H. S. , Webb , W. G. , & Kelly , M. P . ( 1984 ). The naming disorder of dementia . Neuropsychologia , 22 , 23 – 30 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(84)90004–6 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Kounios , J. , & Beeman , M . ( 2009 ). The Aha! moment: The cognitive neuroscience of insight . Current Directions in Psychological Science , 18 , 210 – 216 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01638.x Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Kounios , J. , Frymiare , J. L. , Bowden , E. M. , Fleck , J. I. , Subramaniam , K. , Parrish , T. B. , & Jung-Beeman , M . ( 2006 ). The prepared mind: Neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight . Psychological Science , 17 , 882 – 890 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01798.x Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Kučera , H. , & Francis , W. N . ( 1967 ). Computational analysis of present-day American English . Brown University Press . Madore , K. P. , Addis , D. R. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 2015 ). Creativity and memory: Effects of an episodic-specificity induction on divergent thinking . Psychological Science , 26 , 1461 – 1468 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615591863 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Madore , K. P. , Jing , H. G. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 2016 ). Divergent creative thinking in young and older adults: Extending the effects of an episodic specificity induction . Memory & Cognition , 44 , 974 – 988 . doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-016-0605-z Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed McDermott , R . ( 1999 ). Why information technology inspired but cannot deliver knowledge management . California Management Review , 41 , 103 – 117 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Meade , M. L. , Watson , J. M. , Balota , D. A. , & Roediger , H. L . ( 2007 ). The roles of spreading activation and retrieval mode in producing false recognition in the DRM paradigm . Journal of Memory and Language , 56 , 305 – 320 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.07.007 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Mednick , S. A . ( 1962 ). The associative basis of the creative process . Psychological Review , 69 , 220 – 232 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed McKhann , G. , Drachman , D. , Folstein , M. , Katzman , R. , Price , D. , & Stadlan , E. M . ( 1984 ). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease . Neurology , 34 , 939 – 944 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed McKone , E. , & Murphy , B . ( 2000 ). Implicit false memory: Effects of modality and multiple study presentations on long-lived semantic priming . Journal of Memory and Language , 43 , 89 – 109 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2702 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Morris , J. C. , Edland , S. , Clark , C. , Galasko , D. , Koss , E. , Mohs , R. ,… Heyman , A . ( 1993 ). The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD). Part IV. Rates of cognitive change in the longitudinal assessment of probable Alzheimer’s disease . Neurology , 43 , 2457 – 2465 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Moulin , C. J. , James , N. , Freeman , J. E. , & Jones , R. W . ( 2004 ). Deficient acquisition and consolidation: Intertrial free recall performance in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment . Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology , 26 , 1 – 10 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.26.1.1.23940 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Nebes , R. D . ( 1989 ). Semantic memory in Alzheimer’s disease . Psychological Bulletin , 106 , 377 – 394 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Nedjam , Z. , Devouche , E. , & Dalla Barba , G . ( 2004 ). Confabulation, but not executive dysfunction discriminate AD from frontotemporal dementia . European Journal of Neurology , 11 , 728 – 733 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-1331.2004.00981.x Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Nelson , H. E . ( 1982 ). National Adult Reading Test (NART): For the assessment of premorbid intelligence in patients with dementia: Test manual . NFER-Nelson . Nichols , R. M. , Bogart , D. , & Loftus , E. F . ( 2015 ). False Memories. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences ( 2nd ed ., pp. 709 – 714 ). Oxford : Elsevier . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Norman , K. A. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 1997 ). False recognition in younger and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories . Memory & Cognition , 25 , 838 – 848 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Ober , B. A. , Dronkers , N. F. , Koss , E. , Delis , D. C. , & Friedland , R. P . ( 1986 ). Retrieval from semantic memory in Alzheimer-type dementia . Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology , 8 , 75 – 92 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638608401298 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Ober , B. A. , & Shenaut , G. K . ( 1988 ). Lexical decision and priming in Alzheimer’s disease . Neuropsychologia , 26 , 273 – 286 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(88)90080–2 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Olszewska , J. , & Ulatowska , J . ( 2013 ). Encoding strategy affects false recall and recognition: Evidence from categorical study material . Advances in Cognitive Psychology , 9 , 44 – 52 . doi: https://doi.org/10.2478/v10053-008-0130-0 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Roediger III, H. L. , Balota , D. A. , & Watson , J. M . ( 2001 ). Spreading activation and arousal of false memories . In H. L. Roediger , J. S. Nairne , I. Neath , & A. M. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 95 – 115 ). Washington, DC : American Psychological Association . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Roediger , H. L. , & McDermott , K. B . ( 1995 ). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 21 , 803 – 814 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Roediger , H. L. 3rd , Watson , J. M. , McDermott , K. B. , & Gallo , D. A . ( 2001 ). Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis . Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , 8 , 385 – 407 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Rosa , N. M. , Deason , R. G. , Budson , A. E. , & Gutchess , A. H . ( 2015 ). Self-referencing and false memory in mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease . Neuropsychology , 29 , 799 – 805 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000186 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Schacter , D. L. , Guerin , S. A. , & St Jacques , P. L . ( 2011 ). Memory distortion: An adaptive perspective . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 15 , 467 – 474 .doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612457377 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Schacter , D. L. , Koutstaal , W. , Johnson , M. K. , Gross , M. S. , & Angell , K. E . ( 1997 ). False recollection induced by photographs: A comparison of older and younger adults . Psychology and Aging , 12 , 203 – 215 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Snodgrass , J. G. , & Corwin , J . ( 1988 ). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia . Journal of Experimental Psychology. General , 117 , 34 – 50 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Spieler , D. H. , Balota , D. A. , & Faust , M. E . ( 1996 ). Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type . Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance , 22 , 461 – 479 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Tun , P. A. , Wingfield , A. , Rosen , M. J. , & Blanchard , L . ( 1998 ). Response latencies for false memories: Gist-based processes in normal aging . Psychology and Aging , 13 , 230 – 241 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Waldie , B. D. , & Kwong See , S. T . ( 2003 ). Remembering words never presented: False memory effects in dementia of the Alzheimer type . Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition , 10 , 281 – 297 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1076/anec.10.4.281.28969 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Watson , J. M. , Balota , D. A. , & Sergent-Marshall , S. D . ( 2001 ). Semantic, phonological, and hybrid veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer type . Neuropsychology , 15 , 254 – 267 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1037//0894-4105.15.2.254 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Welsh , K. , Butters , N. , Hughes , J. , Mohs , R. , & Heyman , A . ( 1991 ). Detection of abnormal memory decline in mild cases of Alzheimer’s disease using CERAD neuropsychological measures . Archives of Neurology , 48 , 278 – 281 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1991.00530150046016 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed © The Author(s) 2018. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com. This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/about_us/legal/notices) http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences Oxford University Press

Can False Memories Prime Problem Solutions for Healthy Older Adults and Those With Alzheimer’s Disease?

Loading next page...
 
/lp/ou_press/can-false-memories-prime-problem-solutions-for-healthy-older-adults-M9xSbEikWC
Publisher
Oxford University Press
Copyright
© The Author(s) 2018. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.
ISSN
1079-5014
eISSN
1758-5368
DOI
10.1093/geronb/gby064
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

Abstract Objective Recent research has shown that false memories can have a positive consequence on human cognition in both children and young adults. The present experiment investigated whether false memories could have similar positive effects by priming solutions to insight-based problems in healthy older adults and people with Alzheimer’s disease (AD). Method Participants were asked to solve compound remote associate task (CRAT) problems, half of which had been preceded by the presentation of Deese/Roediger-McDermott (DRM) lists whose critical lures (CL) were also the solutions to those problems. Results The results showed that regardless of cognitive ability, when the CL was falsely recognized, CRAT problems were solved more often and reliably faster than problems that were not primed by a DRM list. When the CL was not falsely recognized, CRAT problem solution rates and times were no different from when there was no DRM priming. Discussion These findings are consistent with predictions from theories of associative activation and demonstrate the importance of automatic spreading activation processes in memory across the life span. Alzheimer’s disease, Compound remote associates task, DRM paradigm, False memory, Priming problem solving Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is characterized by a progressive deterioration of cognitive functioning, and memory disorders are considered to be the earliest and the more serious clinical symptoms of this disease. At the beginning of the disease people with AD commonly exhibit impairments in episodic memory and rapidly forget newly learned information (Baudic, Tzortzis, & Barba, 2004). However, numerous neuropsychological studies have reported evidence that semantic memory impairment may occur relatively early in the course of AD. For example, people with AD retrieve fewer items from a given semantic or letter category on timed verbal fluency tasks (Ober, Dronkers, Koss, Delis, & Friedland, 1986) and perform more poorly on object naming tasks, particularly those involving less common objects with lower frequency names (Kirshner, Webb, & Kelly, 1984). Despite the unequivocal evidence of semantic deficits in AD, a controversy remains as to whether the semantic deficit stems from a loss of information in the semantic store (Chertkow, Bub, & Seidenberg, 1989), or whether the store of semantic memory remains intact in AD, and the deficit is related to a disturbance in an AD patient’s ability to access and manipulate semantic information (Nebes, 1989). The semantic priming paradigm has been used to look at changes in semantic memory in AD. Most studies of semantic priming use a lexical decision task. The lexical decision task is a computerized task composed of a number of trials, each of which is composed of two events: a prime and a target. The prime is often a word for which no response is required. Later, when the target appears on the screen, the participant has to decide as quickly and accurately as possible if it is a real word (e.g., chair) or not (e.g., ignul). The temporal interval between the presentation of the prime and the presentation of the target is called the stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA). The SOA ideally is short, from a few tens to a few hundreds of milliseconds. When the targets are words, a few of them share a semantic relationship with the prime (e.g., the prime “table” followed by the target “chair”), whereas the others are not related to the prime (elephant-table). The priming effect corresponds to the reduction of the response time or the percentage of errors in the trials where the prime and the target are related compared to those where the words do not share a semantic relationship. Semantic priming effects are generally viewed within the framework of automatic spreading activation in the semantic network. That is, the related prime word activates the subsequently presented target word through their associative links in semantic memory (e.g., Collins & Loftus, 1975). Thus, the presentation of a prime automatically activates related nodes, increasing their accessibility. Therefore, when the prime and target are related, the target word is likely to have received this prior activation and will be recognized more quickly and accurately. This automatic preactivation of the related words in the semantic network is the cause of the observed facilitation. Semantic priming effects have been investigated in patients with AD, with conflicting results. For example, some researchers have found less-than-normal priming (Ober & Shenaut, 1988), other studies report equivalent priming for AD patients and healthy older adults, with researchers concluding that semantic memory structures are relatively intact in people with AD (Balota & Duchek, 1991; Chertkow et al., 1989; Nebes et al., 1989). Some studies have reported a combination of no priming and normal priming effects (Albert & Milberg, 1989), other studies have reported both normal priming and hyperpriming (greater than normal priming) with researchers concluding either that attentional abnormalities play a role in performance on semantic priming tasks (Hartman, 1991) or that degradation of the semantic network is responsible, where degraded concepts have more to gain from spread of activation than nondegraded concepts (e.g. Chertkow et al., 1989). In addition to semantic and episodic deficits, people with AD exhibit a higher incidence of memory distortions compared to their cognitively healthy older peers. These memory distortions can be severe, such as confabulation (Nedjam, Devouch, & Dalla Barba, 2004), though generally they are more mundane. For example, people with AD may have thought that they had turned off their stove when they simply misremembered that they turned off the stove. Organizational strategies, such as using pillboxes for medication, can help in the remembering of daily living activities. However, this type of strategy does not help when people with AD experience false memories—not looking in their pillbox, for example, because they falsely remember taking their medication. The Deese/Roediger-McDermott paradigm (DRM; Deese, 1959; Roediger & McDermott, 1995) is probably the most popular and commonly used paradigm to study false memories in a laboratory setting. Here, participants are presented with a list of words all of which are associatively related (e.g., table, sit, legs) and are also associated with a critical lure (CL) word that is never presented (i.e., chair). When memory is subsequently tested using recall or recognition tasks, healthy participants show a tendency to falsely recall and recognize the CLs as having been presented on the list (Balota et al., 1999; McDermott, 1999). Several studies have used the DRM paradigm with AD people and shown that they tend to produce fewer CLs than control participants (e.g., Balota et al., 1999; Budson et al., 2002; Gallo, Bell, Beier, & Schacter, 2006; Waldie & Kwong See, 2003), although the reverse has also been observed (see Watson, Balota, & Sergent-Marshall, 2001). Some studies have shown no reliable differences in CL production in people with AD and healthy controls (Roediger, Balota, & Watson, 2001). There are a number of theories that can explain the lower production of CLs in people with AD. The activation-monitoring theory (see Roediger, Watson, McDermott, & Gallo, 2001) is one of the more dominant explanations (see Gallo, 2010 for a review). According to this theory, false memories arise due to two distinct processes: an activation process and a source-monitoring process. For example, in the DRM task, because the presentation of each of the lists of words automatically activates the related but unpresented CL, the CL is activated multiple times via an automatic spread of activation within the associative network. The sum of this activation increases the feeling of familiarity for this item, while simultaneously reducing the ability to remember the source of its activation (source-monitoring process). Thus, the production of the CL may result from its erroneous attribution to an external source. In healthy aging, any increase in false memories is often explained by a source-monitoring failure (Schacter et al., 1997). The activation-monitoring theory can also be used as a framework to explain the low production of CLs in people with AD in the DRM task. Several studies have reported a failure of the source-monitoring process in AD people (Rosa, Deason, Budson, & Gutchess, 2015). Thus, it seems implausible to attribute their lower production of CLs, compared with healthy older adults, to a more efficient source monitoring. Budson, Daffner, Desikan, and Schacter (2000) and later Gallo (2010) proposed that the activation of target items in people with AD during the presentation of the DRM lists does not spread toward the CL, due either to a disturbance of the semantic network or an attentional overload. Therefore, if the CL has not been activated during list presentation, the CL could not later be falsely remembered. However, Evrard, Colombel, Gilet, and Corson (2016) suggested that the activation of the CL is preserved in people with AD, but that its mnemonic trace does not persist long enough in memory to enable its later production, due to a decline in episodic memory. By way of summary then, healthy older adults and those with AD both experience declines in episodic memory. However, semantic memory is thought to be better preserved in healthy older adults than it is in those with AD. Thus, on the one hand, differences between these groups in false memory production may be mediated more by differences in semantic than episodic memory (Madore, Addis, & Schacter, 2015; Madore, Jing, & Schacter, 2016). That is, perhaps people with AD experience greater difficulty than their healthy counterparts with associative activation of related information within semantic networks (Budson et al., 2000; Dewhurst, Thorley, Hammond, & Ormerod, 2011; Gallo et al., 2006). On the other hand, perhaps the CL does get activated, but the time course for the decline in this activation is faster for those with AD than those without. One way to test these proposals is to introduce a second task where performance improvements on that task are predicated on the earlier production of the CL from the DRM list presentation. By way of background, there are a number of studies that have found that false memories have positive consequences on subsequent task performance. For example, McKone and Murphy (2000) showed that false memories generated using the DRM paradigm could prime performance on related memory tasks (e.g., stem-cued recall). Studies like these have prompted researchers to examine the possible beneficial effects of false memories on cognitive tasks other than those related to memory (Howe, 2011; Schacter, Guerin, & St. Jacques, 2011). Howe, Garner, Dewhurst, and Ball (2010) were the first to carry out research investigating the role that false memories play in priming insight-based problem solving using compound remote associate tasks (CRATs) (see Mednick, 1962; Sio). CRAT problems, originally developed by Mednick (1962), involve the presentation of three words (e.g., apple, family, and house) and the task is to come up with a word (i.e., tree) which, when combined with each of the three original words, creates compound words or common phrases (i.e., apple tree, family tree, treehouse). Howe et al. (2010) presented adults with DRM lists whose CLs served as potential primes for half of the subsequent CRAT problems that participants had to solve. They found that when participants falsely recalled the CLs of the studied DRM lists, the corresponding CRATs were solved more frequently and significantly faster than CRATs that had not been primed or cases in which DRM lists had been presented but CLs were not falsely remembered. Howe, Garner, Charlesworth, and Knott (2011) extended this research to children and found exactly the same results. What this research shows that like true memories, false memories can successfully prime higher-order cognitive tasks (i.e., insight-based problem solving). In the current research, we wanted to see whether these priming effects also occur in healthy older adults and, more importantly, people with AD. If this finding can be extended to people with AD, this would extend our understanding of memory processes in this sub-group of memory-impaired people and importantly, could have positive implications for memory rehabilitation. If people with AD have greater problems with spreading activation than healthy older adults (Budson et al., 2000; Gallo et al., 2006), then they should be less likely to remember the CL than healthy older adults. In addition, if false memories have a shorter “life span” for people with AD, then even when CLs are produced on the memory test, priming effects on subsequently administered CRAT problems should be attenuated or absent. However, if as some previous research suggests (Roediger et al., 2001) both healthy older adults and people with AD do have intact semantic networks and CL longevity is not an issue, then it is expected that CLs will be falsely remembered and priming will occur on subsequent CRAT problems. In the present study, we used CRAT problems whose baseline solution rates were moderate (30%–80%) for older adults. Because the CRAT norms that are available were based on solutions provided by children and young adults, we created our own age-appropriate CRAT norms prior to conducting the priming experiment. Our rationale for this was that we wanted to eliminate differences of age due simply to knowledge base, a procedure consistent with previous studies (e.g., Howe et al., 2011), as these differences were not of interest in the current research. Experiment 1: Norming CRAT Problems for Use With Older Adults Before turning to the main experiment, we report a pilot study in which we collected norms for CRAT problems for use with older adults. In line with previous research, age-appropriate CRAT problems were developed in order to eliminate any potential age effects in problem-solving performance due to the use of extant norms which were developed using samples of children and young adults. Method Participants A total of 32 healthy older adults (13 males and 19 females) took part in this experiment; their mean age was 78.19 (SD = 5.67). The older adults had normal cognitive functioning (as assessed by the Mini-Mental State Examination [MMSE]; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) with a mean score of 27.31 (SD = 2.52), normal activities of daily living, and most importantly, did not meet diagnostic criteria for dementia. These older adults were volunteers who were community dwelling and were tested in their own home or local community center. Materials Older adults were presented with 20 CRAT problems taken from the Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003) norms. The items on the CRATs required a solution that was associated with all three words of the triad through the construction of a compound word or common phrase (e.g., cream, skate, and water combined with the solution word ice, creating the compounds ice cream, ice skate, and ice water). (Note that only problems with solution rates above 30% and solved within 30 s were selected for subsequent use with older adults with AD.) All the solution words had a familiarity rating of 500 or above (with a maximum entry of 645 and a mean of 566 [Coltheart, 1981]) and a word frequency of 10 or above (with a maximum entry of 686 and a mean of 126 [Kucera & Francis. 1967]). Procedure Participants were tested individually in a quiet room. Instructions similar to those used by Howe et al. (2011) and by Bowden and Jung-Beeman (2003) were given. Specifically, participants were told that they would see three words and that they should try and produce a fourth word that, when combined with each of the three items, would make up a common compound word or phrase. Participants were first given three demonstrations by the experimenter followed by two practice problems prior to the experiment itself. The three problem words were presented on a computer laptop screen simultaneously in a horizontal orientation, with one word above, below, and at the center fixation point. Participants were given 30 s to produce the solution (this was a verbal solution). If the solution was produced within the time limit, both the solution word and solution time were recorded and the next problem was presented. If participants did not produce the correct response within the time limit, the solution was provided by the experimenter and the program automatically moved to the next problem. Results Table 1 shows the average solution rates and times for the 20 problems separately. As can be seen, older adults were able to solve most of these CRAT problems. Importantly, for the next experiment, there was a good range of solution rates and times for these CRAT problems. What this means is that priming effects, should they exist, can be measured without constraints imposed by floor and ceiling effects. Table 1. CRAT Problems: Solution Rates and Times CRAT problem CRAT solution % Solved Solution time (s) Heart/potato/tooth Sweet 18.8 30.2 (12.24) Over/deep/Walk Sleep 21.9 23.7 (17.45) Skin/ball/tissue Soft 25 13.5 (10.02) Bike/top/goat Mountain 34.4 20.1 (17.68) Base/tank/territorial Army 40.6 21.3 (18.03) Cut/sore/war Cold 43.8 21.9 (18.86) Hold/print/stool Foot 43.8 18.6 (13.62) List/death/Bone Wish 46.9 19.2 (13.84) Polo/flannel/vest Shirt 46.9 24.5 (20.69) Bow/Haul/jump Long 53.2 18.6 (16.09) Bowl/Juice/Salad Fruit 53.2 11.6 (9.73) Measuring/cake/tea Cup 59.4 18.5 (13.97) Pal/tip/knife Pen 62.5 14.3 (12.50) Bite/vein/web Spider 68.8 17.1 (13.63) Jack/magic/board Black 75 13.5 (12.48) Crumbs/dough/Knife Bread 81.3 13.4 (12.54) Care/spa/mental Health 87 10.2 (7.73) Knitting/stick/pine Needle 87.5 16.2 (14.52) Sill/frame/cleaner Window 87.6 11.8 (14.09) School/Chair/Heels High 90.6 12.8 (13.63) Rocking/wheel/high Chair 93.7 9.8 (11.71) CRAT problem CRAT solution % Solved Solution time (s) Heart/potato/tooth Sweet 18.8 30.2 (12.24) Over/deep/Walk Sleep 21.9 23.7 (17.45) Skin/ball/tissue Soft 25 13.5 (10.02) Bike/top/goat Mountain 34.4 20.1 (17.68) Base/tank/territorial Army 40.6 21.3 (18.03) Cut/sore/war Cold 43.8 21.9 (18.86) Hold/print/stool Foot 43.8 18.6 (13.62) List/death/Bone Wish 46.9 19.2 (13.84) Polo/flannel/vest Shirt 46.9 24.5 (20.69) Bow/Haul/jump Long 53.2 18.6 (16.09) Bowl/Juice/Salad Fruit 53.2 11.6 (9.73) Measuring/cake/tea Cup 59.4 18.5 (13.97) Pal/tip/knife Pen 62.5 14.3 (12.50) Bite/vein/web Spider 68.8 17.1 (13.63) Jack/magic/board Black 75 13.5 (12.48) Crumbs/dough/Knife Bread 81.3 13.4 (12.54) Care/spa/mental Health 87 10.2 (7.73) Knitting/stick/pine Needle 87.5 16.2 (14.52) Sill/frame/cleaner Window 87.6 11.8 (14.09) School/Chair/Heels High 90.6 12.8 (13.63) Rocking/wheel/high Chair 93.7 9.8 (11.71) Note: Standard deviation is in parenthesis and solution times are presented. CRAT = Compound remote associate task. View Large Table 1. CRAT Problems: Solution Rates and Times CRAT problem CRAT solution % Solved Solution time (s) Heart/potato/tooth Sweet 18.8 30.2 (12.24) Over/deep/Walk Sleep 21.9 23.7 (17.45) Skin/ball/tissue Soft 25 13.5 (10.02) Bike/top/goat Mountain 34.4 20.1 (17.68) Base/tank/territorial Army 40.6 21.3 (18.03) Cut/sore/war Cold 43.8 21.9 (18.86) Hold/print/stool Foot 43.8 18.6 (13.62) List/death/Bone Wish 46.9 19.2 (13.84) Polo/flannel/vest Shirt 46.9 24.5 (20.69) Bow/Haul/jump Long 53.2 18.6 (16.09) Bowl/Juice/Salad Fruit 53.2 11.6 (9.73) Measuring/cake/tea Cup 59.4 18.5 (13.97) Pal/tip/knife Pen 62.5 14.3 (12.50) Bite/vein/web Spider 68.8 17.1 (13.63) Jack/magic/board Black 75 13.5 (12.48) Crumbs/dough/Knife Bread 81.3 13.4 (12.54) Care/spa/mental Health 87 10.2 (7.73) Knitting/stick/pine Needle 87.5 16.2 (14.52) Sill/frame/cleaner Window 87.6 11.8 (14.09) School/Chair/Heels High 90.6 12.8 (13.63) Rocking/wheel/high Chair 93.7 9.8 (11.71) CRAT problem CRAT solution % Solved Solution time (s) Heart/potato/tooth Sweet 18.8 30.2 (12.24) Over/deep/Walk Sleep 21.9 23.7 (17.45) Skin/ball/tissue Soft 25 13.5 (10.02) Bike/top/goat Mountain 34.4 20.1 (17.68) Base/tank/territorial Army 40.6 21.3 (18.03) Cut/sore/war Cold 43.8 21.9 (18.86) Hold/print/stool Foot 43.8 18.6 (13.62) List/death/Bone Wish 46.9 19.2 (13.84) Polo/flannel/vest Shirt 46.9 24.5 (20.69) Bow/Haul/jump Long 53.2 18.6 (16.09) Bowl/Juice/Salad Fruit 53.2 11.6 (9.73) Measuring/cake/tea Cup 59.4 18.5 (13.97) Pal/tip/knife Pen 62.5 14.3 (12.50) Bite/vein/web Spider 68.8 17.1 (13.63) Jack/magic/board Black 75 13.5 (12.48) Crumbs/dough/Knife Bread 81.3 13.4 (12.54) Care/spa/mental Health 87 10.2 (7.73) Knitting/stick/pine Needle 87.5 16.2 (14.52) Sill/frame/cleaner Window 87.6 11.8 (14.09) School/Chair/Heels High 90.6 12.8 (13.63) Rocking/wheel/high Chair 93.7 9.8 (11.71) Note: Standard deviation is in parenthesis and solution times are presented. CRAT = Compound remote associate task. View Large Experiment 2: Examining Priming Effects in Older Healthy Adults and Those With AD With these norms in hand, we now turn to the main question concerning the role of false memories in priming solutions to insight-based problem solving in healthy older adults and people with AD. Method Participants A new sample of 60 participants was recruited whose demographic and other characteristics are shown in Table 2. A statistical power analysis was performed for sample size estimation. The effect size in this study was considered to be medium using Cohen’s (1988) criteria. With an α = 0.05 and power = 0.80, our sample size of 60 (30 participants per group) is considered more than adequate. Thirty participants had a clinical diagnosis of probable or possible AD (McKhann et al., 1984). Patients were diagnosed as being demented with the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, third edition (DSM III-R; American Psychiatric Association, 1987) criteria as having AD by the National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorder and Stroke and the Alzheimer’s disease and Related Disorders Association (NINCDS-ADRDA) criteria (McKhann et al., 1984). AD was diagnosed by a clinician using neuropsychological examination, MMSE (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975), family interview, laboratory screening (i.e., hematology; B12 and folate levels; renal, liver, and thyroid function; calcium and syphilis serology), and medical examination. If there was a suggestion of a psychiatric disorder, patients were also assessed by a psychiatrist. Patients with a history of stroke or depression were excluded from this study. Patients with a Hachinski score (Hachinski et al., 1975) that indicated they might have vascular component to their dementia were also excluded. Thirty participants made up an older adult control (OAC) group. These people were community dwelling and were recruited from a panel of older adults who had expressed an interest in participating in research (n = 18), or were recruited from Age Concern London (n = 12). All OAC participants had recently been screened for dementia using the MMSE, all scoring above the standard cutoff of 26/30. Table 2. Means (and SE) Demographic Characteristics of Participants AD OAC MMSE 19.54 (0.55) 27.39 (0.58) NART 106.54 (1.79) 116.78 (0.95) Education 10.02 (0.27) 12.32 (0.42) Age 76.43 (1.13) 77.32 (0.84) AD OAC MMSE 19.54 (0.55) 27.39 (0.58) NART 106.54 (1.79) 116.78 (0.95) Education 10.02 (0.27) 12.32 (0.42) Age 76.43 (1.13) 77.32 (0.84) Note: AD = Alzheimer’s disease; OAC = Older adult control; MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination Score. Predicted IQ from the National Adult Reading Tests (NART). Education = Years of formal education. View Large Table 2. Means (and SE) Demographic Characteristics of Participants AD OAC MMSE 19.54 (0.55) 27.39 (0.58) NART 106.54 (1.79) 116.78 (0.95) Education 10.02 (0.27) 12.32 (0.42) Age 76.43 (1.13) 77.32 (0.84) AD OAC MMSE 19.54 (0.55) 27.39 (0.58) NART 106.54 (1.79) 116.78 (0.95) Education 10.02 (0.27) 12.32 (0.42) Age 76.43 (1.13) 77.32 (0.84) Note: AD = Alzheimer’s disease; OAC = Older adult control; MMSE = Mini-Mental State Examination Score. Predicted IQ from the National Adult Reading Tests (NART). Education = Years of formal education. View Large As noted, the demographic characteristics of the participants are shown in Table 2. One-way analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were used to analyze these demographic variables. These analyses showed that there were no significant differences between groups in mean age, F < 1. There was a significant difference in the National Adult Reading Test (NART) predicted IQ scores (Nelson, 1982) for the groups, F(1, 58) = 18.56, p < .001, with the AD group scoring reliably lower than the OACs. There was also a significant difference in the mean number of years of formal education between groups, F(1, 58) = 12.01, p < .001, with the OACs having reliably more years of formal education. For MMSE Score (Folstein et al., 1975), as expected, there was also a significant difference between groups, F(1,58) = 56.21, p < .001, with the people with AD performing reliably lower that the OACs. In addition, people with AD were also tested on the word list memory test from the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer’s Disease (CERAD) neuropsychology battery (Morris et al., 1993; Welsh, Butters, Hughes, Mohs, & Heyman, 1991). For delayed recall, they recalled an average of 0.8 (SD = 1.92) words. This level of performance on this particular task is in keeping with other reports for people with AD in the literature (e.g., Akhtar, Moulin, & Bowie, 2006; Moulin, James, Freeman, & Jones, 2004), where mean performance is typically less than one for people with AD. Mean normal performance is around six items (Moulin et al., 2004). Older healthy adults gave their written informed consent. For people with AD, written informed consent was given either by them or their primary caregivers. The OACs were community dwelling and people with AD were recruited from two day care centers in London. All participants were fluent in English. Design, materials, and procedure A 2 (Group: AD vs OAC) × 2 (Priming: primed vs unprimed) mixed design was used, where the first factor was between-participants and the second was a within-participant factor. For purposes of the analyses, primed items were further divided according to whether participants falsely remembered the CL (designated “primed/FM”) or did not falsely remember the CL (designated “primed/no-FM”). This resulted in a 2 (Group) × 3 (Priming) design. We followed the same procedure as Howe et al. (2010, 2011) such that each participant was primed on half the subsequent CRAT problems with preceding DRM lists whose CLs were also the solution to those CRAT problems. The order of both the DRM lists and CRAT problems was carefully counterbalanced to eliminate order effects. Ten CRAT problems were selected from the normative data in Experiment 1 (see Supplementary Appendix). Their nonprimed solution rates ranged between 30% and 80% in order to insure that they were neither too easy nor too hard. In addition, 10 DRM lists whose CLs were the corresponding solutions to these CRATs were selected. Each list contained 10 word associates of the CL and were taken from Roediger et al. (2001) norms. Lists were randomly divided into two groups of five with participants being primed using one of these sets. In order to prevent differences in false memory rates, the two sets of five DRM lists were equated on backward associative strength (BAS) (List Set 1 BAS = 0.777, List Set 2 BAS = 0.725). Participants were given one set of the five DRM lists in a randomized order. Each list was presented verbally, followed by a distractor task (counting backward by four from a 3-digit number for 30 s). This was followed by a recognition test similar to that in Howe, Wilkinson, Garner, and Ball (2016) whereby participants were verbally presented with the 5 CL words from the studied DRM lists, 5 unstudied and unrelated CLs, 32 true items from the studied DRM lists, 32 foils unrelated to studied DRM lists, and 8 filler items. A recognition test was implemented rather than a recall test to reduce effects of priming during retrieval (Olszewska & Ulatowska, 2013). For each word presented in the recognition task, participants had to select either [O], indicating that the word was Old and that they recognize the word from the previously presented lists, or [N] if they thought the current word presented was a new word that they did not hear in the previous word lists. Following completion of the DRM lists, all 10 CRAT problems were completed. The same procedure used for the CRAT pilot study was used for the main experiment and solution rates and solution times were collected for correctly solved CRAT problems. Results We controlled for the effects of educational level and NART statistically. In all of our analyses, we ran an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with these demographic variables as covariates. For all ANCOVAs, the pattern of findings for the main effect of group and interactions with group were unchanged from the analysis with ANOVAs. For simplicity, therefore, the ANOVAs are reported here. False memories were comparable to previous studies (Roediger, Balota, & Watson, 2001; Waldie & Kwong-See, 2003). The recognition task showed that both the OACs and people with AD created false memories for the CL words, with people with AD falsely recognizing the CL 61% (M = 3.05, SD = 1.31) of the time and the OAC group 60% (M = 3.01, SD = 0.91) of the time. There were no reliable differences. Overall recognition scores were analyzed using a 2 (Group: AD vs OAC) × 4 (list type: CLs, unstudied unrelated CLs, foils and list items) mixed model ANOVA. Analysis revealed a significant main effect of list type, F(3, 58) = 6.51, p < .001. Pairwise comparisons revealed greater recognition in list items (80%) compared to foil items (66.7%) (M = 25.63, SD = 2.51 vs M = 21.34, SD = 3.3) and greater recognition of CL words (60.6%) compared to unrelated CL words (40.4%) (M = 3.03, SD = 0.80 vs M = 2.02, SD = 1.08). There was no main effect for group (F < 1) and no interaction (F < 1). Because false alarm rates for recognition tests often require a correction for response bias, we analyzed discrimination and response bias scores using signal detection analysis. We used the Snodgrass and Corwin (1988) correction for signal detection theory (SDT) measures, whereby 0.5 was added to hit and false alarm rates and the corrected score was divided by N + 1. This was used in order to prevent values of 1.0 and 0. The Snodgrass and Corwin correction was conducted for SDT measures for all list items. For discriminability (d’), larger values indicate better memory performance, and for criterion value (C), values greater than 0 represent a conservative response bias and less than 0 represents a liberal response bias. The values of d’ and C are shown in Table 3. The calculation of d’ and C for used the false alarm rate for unrelated foils. Signal detection measures for hits and CLs were analyzed using separate independent t tests. The analysis of d’ for hits and CLs revealed no reliable differences (t < 1). Analysis of the criterion C, revealed no reliable differences for hits or CLs (t < 1). Table 3. Means and SD of Signal Detection Measures of Discriminability (d’) and Bias (C) for Hits and Critical Lures (CL) OAC AD d’ C d’ C Hits 1.42 (0.48) 0.23 (0.2) 1.13 (0.39) 0.18 (0.1) CL 0.93 (0.78) 0.22 (0.39) 0.84 (0.53) 0.18 (0.32) OAC AD d’ C d’ C Hits 1.42 (0.48) 0.23 (0.2) 1.13 (0.39) 0.18 (0.1) CL 0.93 (0.78) 0.22 (0.39) 0.84 (0.53) 0.18 (0.32) Note: AD = Alzheimer’s disease; CL = Critical lures; OAC = Older adult control. View Large Table 3. Means and SD of Signal Detection Measures of Discriminability (d’) and Bias (C) for Hits and Critical Lures (CL) OAC AD d’ C d’ C Hits 1.42 (0.48) 0.23 (0.2) 1.13 (0.39) 0.18 (0.1) CL 0.93 (0.78) 0.22 (0.39) 0.84 (0.53) 0.18 (0.32) OAC AD d’ C d’ C Hits 1.42 (0.48) 0.23 (0.2) 1.13 (0.39) 0.18 (0.1) CL 0.93 (0.78) 0.22 (0.39) 0.84 (0.53) 0.18 (0.32) Note: AD = Alzheimer’s disease; CL = Critical lures; OAC = Older adult control. View Large The mean CRAT solution rates (proportions) and the mean CRAT solution times (in seconds) were calculated for each participant and analyzed separately in a series of 2 (Group: AD vs OAC) × 3 (Priming: primed/FM vs primed/no-FM vs unprimed) ANOVAs. For primed CRAT problems, solution rates and solution times were conditionalized on whether participants had produced the CL during recall (i.e., primed/FM = CL produced and primed/no-FM = no CL produced). Thus, both solution rates and solution times were subjected to separate ANOVAs where the factors were solution type (unprimed, primed/no-FM, or primed/FM) and group. The data are shown in Table 4. Table 4. Mean CRAT Problem Solution Rates and Solution Times for Older Adults and Alzheimer’s Patients for FM Priming Priming Participant Unprimed Priming/FM Priming/NO-FM Solution times (seconds) Older Adults 36.97 (1.92) 21.20 (1.27) 37.24 (1.61) Alzheimer’s patients 44.06 (2.58) 21.98 (1.72) 39.70 (2.16) Solution rates (proportion) Older Adults 0.38 (0.24) 0.59 (0.39) 0.23 (0.15) Alzheimer’s patients 0.32 (0.31) 0.57 (0.51) 0.24 (0.31) Priming Participant Unprimed Priming/FM Priming/NO-FM Solution times (seconds) Older Adults 36.97 (1.92) 21.20 (1.27) 37.24 (1.61) Alzheimer’s patients 44.06 (2.58) 21.98 (1.72) 39.70 (2.16) Solution rates (proportion) Older Adults 0.38 (0.24) 0.59 (0.39) 0.23 (0.15) Alzheimer’s patients 0.32 (0.31) 0.57 (0.51) 0.24 (0.31) Note: Standard errors are in parenthesis. CRAT = Compound remote associate task; FM = False memory. View Large Table 4. Mean CRAT Problem Solution Rates and Solution Times for Older Adults and Alzheimer’s Patients for FM Priming Priming Participant Unprimed Priming/FM Priming/NO-FM Solution times (seconds) Older Adults 36.97 (1.92) 21.20 (1.27) 37.24 (1.61) Alzheimer’s patients 44.06 (2.58) 21.98 (1.72) 39.70 (2.16) Solution rates (proportion) Older Adults 0.38 (0.24) 0.59 (0.39) 0.23 (0.15) Alzheimer’s patients 0.32 (0.31) 0.57 (0.51) 0.24 (0.31) Priming Participant Unprimed Priming/FM Priming/NO-FM Solution times (seconds) Older Adults 36.97 (1.92) 21.20 (1.27) 37.24 (1.61) Alzheimer’s patients 44.06 (2.58) 21.98 (1.72) 39.70 (2.16) Solution rates (proportion) Older Adults 0.38 (0.24) 0.59 (0.39) 0.23 (0.15) Alzheimer’s patients 0.32 (0.31) 0.57 (0.51) 0.24 (0.31) Note: Standard errors are in parenthesis. CRAT = Compound remote associate task; FM = False memory. View Large Concerning solution times, there was a main effect for priming F(2, 58) =15.26, p < .001, η2p = .244, where post hoc tests (Tukey’s) showed that solution times were faster for primed/FM problems (M = 21.49) compared to primed/No-FM problems (M = 38.12, p < .01) and unprimed CRAT problems (M = 38.50, p = < .01), and the latter two conditions did not differ. Furthermore, solution time results showed no significant difference across participant groups, with OAC’s average problem-solving time being 31.39 s (SE = 1.132) and AD’s being 34.09 s (SE = 1.519). There was no interaction. Concerning solution rates, there was a main effect for priming F(2, 58) = 15.26, p < .001, η2p = .248, where post hoc tests (Tukey’s LSD) showed that solution rates were higher for primed/FM CRAT problems (M = 0.52) than for primed/no-FM (M = 0.23) and when participants were unprimed (M = 0.26), and the latter two did not differ. There was no main effect for group, where OACs (M = 0.42) and AD (M = 0.39) solved similar numbers of CRAT problems and no interaction. Discussion The present study set out to investigate whether false memories can have a positive consequence on human cognition with older healthy adults and those with AD, as has been shown in children and young adults (Howe et al., 2010, 2011). To investigate this, participants were asked to solve CRAT problems, half of which had been preceded by the presentation of DRM lists whose CLs were also the solutions to those problems. Consistent with previous research, our study showed no reliable differences in the number of false memories produced in the recognition task (Roediger, Balota, & Watson, 2001; Waldie & Kwong-See, 2003). This finding can be explained by the fact that both older healthy adults and those with AD have intact semantic networks that automatically activate CLs upon DRM list presentation. Our findings support existing evidence regarding the underlying mechanisms in the production of false memories (Roediger et al., 2001). Previous research has shown the generation of false memories from the automatic spread of activation within the semantic networks and the corresponding activations of word associations. The findings from the present study further extend this notion, providing evidence that not only are false memories associated with the spreading activation among semantic associates but essentially act similarly to true memories when it comes to priming subsequent task performance (McDermott, 1999). Furthermore, when a recognition test is administered in this priming paradigm, endorsement of the false memory item versus no endorsement is an index of the strength of activation of the CL in memory. That is, no recognition = below threshold activation and recognition = above threshold activation. Although false memories arise at encoding, test performance reveals the strength of that activation. It also turns out that presenting the CL at test has little to no effect on memory strength of the CL because, as already mentioned, false memories arise during the encoding not retrieval process (see Howe et al., 2016). Our findings are the first to show that false memories can successfully prime insight-based problem solving in both AD and OACs. Just like in Howe et al. (2011), we propose when problem solutions were primed by the prior presentation of DRM lists whose CLs were falsely remembered and were solutions to those problems, critically both the probability of such problems being solved and the speed with which they were solved improved significantly. This was true regardless of whether the problem solvers were people with AD or OACs. These findings strongly suggest that false memories do not “fade” more rapidly for people with AD than for OACs and they are capable of priming and facilitating performance on a subsequent problem-solving task. What is important to consider here, is the DRM lists can prime and facilitate performance on problem-solving tasks both in terms of the rate and the speed which they are solved. However, one can only make this conclusion when the CL is falsely recognized. Such facilitation is not found when the false CL has not been remembered. Interestingly, priming with no recognition of the CL resulted in problem-solving rates and times identical to conditions in which there was no priming. This adds to the growing view that false memories, like true memories, can successfully prime higher cognitive processes, at least in terms of problems involving insight-based solutions (Diliberto-Macaluso, 2005; Howe et al., 2010, 2011, 2016). Our research is the first to demonstrate that false memory priming effects occur regardless of cognitive abilities. In the present study, priming effects were equally robust in OACs and people with AD. This cognitive invariance has important theoretical implications. We suggest intact semantic networks exist in both these groups of older adults. To add strength to this argument, we compared our findings to previous research with younger adults (e.g., Howe et al., 2011, 2016). What this comparison shows is that rates of priming for younger adults in those studies is similar to those same rates for the older adults in the present study. Specifically, regardless of differences in materials and overall false memory rates, when young adults remembered the CL, their priming power for solving subsequent CRAT problems was similar to the rate for when older adults got the CL in the present research. That is, average solution times for young adults (19.22) was similar to that for older adults (21.20) as were the average solution rates for younger adults (0.76) and older adults (0.6). Thus, what our study shows is that semantic networks are relatively well preserved in people with AD and OACs, at least when compared to those same rates for younger adults in earlier research. From all of this research, it is clear that false memories generated from the DRM word lists and CRAT problem solutions arise from the nonconscious and automatic spreading of activation among semantic concepts (Balota et al., 1999; Roediger et al., 2001). Therefore, as a result of priming occurring in both of the populations studied here, spreading of activation between nodes in the semantic networks must be intact. In the extant literature, decline in performance on tasks such as verbal fluency have been attributed to the breakdown in semantic networks, particularly for people with AD (Balota et al., 1999; Watson et al., 2001). What our findings suggest is that these breakdowns are not due to the deterioration of semantic networks but rather, due to possible failures in source monitoring. Although further research is needed to confirm this hypothesis, what our study shows is that there was no decline in spreading activation within semantic networks; false memories were as frequent in people with AD as in those without AD (our OACs) and they served as equally powerful primes for both groups when solving CRAT problems. Another hypothesis worth considering could be that CRAT problems were solved via insight-like (perhaps automatic, nonconscious) strategy or via a more analytic (perhaps deliberate) strategy (e.g., Kounios et al., 2006; Kounios & Beeman 2009). These studies show that distinct brain mechanisms are involved for the two types of solutions. Although in the present study there were no differences between OACs and people with AD in using primes for solving CRAT problems, the mechanisms through which the two groups of participants reached the solution could have differed. Of course, this hypothesis would require additional research. Previous research has shown the positive consequences that false memories have problem-solving tasks in both children and adults, yet this has not been fully examined in individuals with associated cognitive decline (Howe et al., 2010). The results from the current study demonstrate for the first time the priming effects false memories have on complex insight-based problem-solving tasks such as CRATs on OACs and people with AD. Additionally, our findings add to the recent literature on the positive consequences that false memories have on human cognition, particularly in the way they facilitate performance on higher-order cognitive tasks such as the CRAT. Given that these significant results were found in both older adults and people with AD, our findings strongly suggest that significant differences that may arise in memory functioning are not the result of deterioration in spreading activating in semantic networks, at least not in the DRM/CRAT tasks. Finally, our findings have a number of important theoretical and practical implications. First, we propose that OACs and people with AD have intact semantic networks. Second, although there are clear differences between true and false memories (Roediger & McDermott, 1995) our findings add to the growing literature suggesting that false memories can work in a very similar way to those observed for true memories (Diliberto-Macaluso, 2005). Third, our findings add to an emerging consensus that false memories, just like false beliefs (Howe & Derbish, 2010), can have beneficial effects in human cognition and not simply the negative consequences we are all familiar with (see Howe & Knott, 2015). We are aware that some may interpret false memories as negative regardless of their benefits as outlined in this paper, we believe that this by-product of a powerful reconstructive memory system is positive (see Howe et al., 2010). Our findings have taken us a step closer to realizing at least one beneficial aspect of false recollection in that it helps to establish that false memories, like true memories, can and do provide significant advantages when it comes to more complex cognitive processes, specifically insight-based problem solving for both OACs, people with AD, children, and adults (Howe et al., 2011). Supplementary Material Supplementary data is available at The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences online. Funding This research was supported by a grant to MLH from the Economic and Social Research Council of Great Britain (RES-062-23-3327). Conflict of Interest None reported. References Akhtar , S. , Moulin , C. J. , & Bowie , P. C . ( 2006 ). Are people with mild cognitive impairment aware of the benefits of errorless learning ? Neuropsychological Rehabilitation , 16 , 329 – 346 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/09602010500176674 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Albert , M. , & Milberg , W . ( 1989 ). Semantic processing in patients with Alzheimer’s disease . Brain and Language , 37 , 163 – 171 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(89)90106-5 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed American Psychiatric Association . ( 1987 ). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Health Disorders (DSM-III-R) . Washington DG . Anderson , J. R . ( 1983 ). A spreading activation theory of memory . Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior , 22 , 261 – 295 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(83)90201–3 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Balota , D. A. , Cortese , M. J. , Duchek , J. M. , Adams , D. , Roediger III, H. L. , McDermott , K. B. , & Yerys , B. E . ( 1999 ). Veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type . Cognitive Neuropsychology , 16 , 361 – 384 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/026432999380834 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Balota , D. A. , & Duchek , J. M . ( 1991 ). Semantic priming effects, lexical repetition effects, and contextual disambiguation effects in healthy aged individuals and individuals with senile dementia of the Alzheimer type . Brain and Language , 40 , 181 – 201 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(91)90124-J Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Baudic , S. , Tzortzis , C. , Barba , G. D. , & Traykov , L . ( 2004 ). Executive deficits in elderly patients with major unipolar depression . Journal of geriatric psychiatry and neurology , 17 , 195 – 201 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0891988704269823 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Bowden , E. M. , & Jung-Beeman , M . ( 2003 ). Normative data for 144 compound remote associate problems . Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers , 35 , 634 – 639 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Budson , A. E. , Daffner , K. R. , Desikan , R. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 2000 ). When false recognition is unopposed by true recognition: Gist-based memory distortion in Alzheimer’s disease . Neuropsychology , 14 , 277 – 287 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Budson , A. E. , & Solomon , P. R . ( 2012 ). New diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment for the practical neurologist . Practical Neurology , 12 , 88 – 96 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/practneurol-2011-000145 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Budson , A. E. , Sullivan , A. L. , Mayer , E. , Daffner , K. R. , Black , P. M. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 2002 ). Suppression of false recognition in Alzheimer’s disease and in patients with frontal lobe lesions . Brain , 125 , 2750 – 2765 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Chertkow , H. , Bub , D. , & Seidenberg , M . ( 1989 ). Priming and semantic memory loss in Alzheimer’s disease . Brain and Language , 36 , 420 – 446 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(89)90078-3 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Collins , A. M. , & Loftus , E. F . ( 1975 ). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing . Psychological Review , 82 , 407 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Coltheart , M . ( 1981 ). The MRC psycholinguistic database . The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 33 , 497 – 505 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Deese , J . ( 1959 ). On the prediction of occurrence of particular verbal intrusions in immediate recall . Journal of Experimental Psychology , 58 , 17 – 22 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046671 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Dewhurst , S. A. , Thorley , C. , Hammond , E. R. , & Ormerod , T. C . ( 2011 ). Convergent, but not divergent, thinking predicts susceptibility to associative memory illusions . Personality and Individual Differences , 51 , 73 – 76 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.018 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Diliberto-Macaluso , K. A . ( 2005 ). Priming and false memories from Deese-Roediger-McDermott lists on a fragment completion test with children . The American Journal of Psychology , 118 , 13 – 28 . Google Scholar PubMed Evrard , C. , Colombel , F. , Gilet , A. L. , & Corson , Y . ( 2016 ). Intact semantic priming of critical lures in alzheimer’s disease: Implications for false memory . The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences , 71 , 671 – 674 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbv006 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Folstein , M. F. , Folstein , S. E. , & McHugh , P. R . ( 1975 ). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician . Journal of Psychiatric Research , 12 , 189 – 198 . doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Gallo , D. A . ( 2010 ). False memories and fantastic beliefs: 15 years of the DRM illusion . Memory & Cognition , 38 , 833 – 848 . doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.7.833 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Gallo , D. A. , Bell , D. M. , Beier , J. S. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 2006 ). Two types of recollection-based monitoring in younger and older adults: Recall-to-reject and the distinctiveness heuristic . Memory (Hove, England) , 14 , 730 – 741 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210600648506 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Hachinski , V. C. , Iliff , L. D. , Zilhka , E. , Du Boulay , G. H. , McAllister , V. L. , Marshall , J. ,… Symon , L . ( 1975 ). Cerebral blood flow in dementia . Archives of Neurology , 32 , 632 – 637 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1975.00490510088009 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Hartman , M. , & Hasher , L. ( 1991 ). Aging and suppression: Memory for previously relevant information . Psychology and aging , 6 , 587 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Howe , M. L . ( 2011 ). The adaptive nature of memory and its illusions . Current Directions in Psychological Science , 20 , 312 – 315 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411416571 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Howe , M. L. , & Derbish , M. H . ( 2010 ). On the susceptibility of adaptive memory to false memory illusions . Cognition , 115 , 252 – 267 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.016 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Howe , M. L. , Garner , S. R. , Charlesworth , M. , & Knott , L . ( 2011 ). A brighter side to memory illusions: False memories prime children’s and adults’ insight-based problem solving . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 108 , 383 – 393 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.012 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Howe , M. L. , Garner , S. R. , Dewhurst , S. A. , & Ball , L. J . ( 2010 ). Can false memories prime problem solutions ? Cognition , 117 , 176 – 181 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.08.009 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Howe , M. L. , & Knott , L. M . ( 2015 ). The fallibility of memory in judicial processes: Lessons from the past and their modern consequences . Memory (Hove, England) , 23 , 633 – 656 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2015.1010709 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Howe , M. L. , Wilkinson , S. , Garner , S. R. , & Ball , L. J . ( 2016 ). On the adaptive function of children’s and adults’ false memories . Memory , 24 , 1062 – 1077 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2015.1068335 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Kirshner , H. S. , Webb , W. G. , & Kelly , M. P . ( 1984 ). The naming disorder of dementia . Neuropsychologia , 22 , 23 – 30 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(84)90004–6 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Kounios , J. , & Beeman , M . ( 2009 ). The Aha! moment: The cognitive neuroscience of insight . Current Directions in Psychological Science , 18 , 210 – 216 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01638.x Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Kounios , J. , Frymiare , J. L. , Bowden , E. M. , Fleck , J. I. , Subramaniam , K. , Parrish , T. B. , & Jung-Beeman , M . ( 2006 ). The prepared mind: Neural activity prior to problem presentation predicts subsequent solution by sudden insight . Psychological Science , 17 , 882 – 890 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01798.x Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Kučera , H. , & Francis , W. N . ( 1967 ). Computational analysis of present-day American English . Brown University Press . Madore , K. P. , Addis , D. R. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 2015 ). Creativity and memory: Effects of an episodic-specificity induction on divergent thinking . Psychological Science , 26 , 1461 – 1468 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615591863 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Madore , K. P. , Jing , H. G. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 2016 ). Divergent creative thinking in young and older adults: Extending the effects of an episodic specificity induction . Memory & Cognition , 44 , 974 – 988 . doi: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-016-0605-z Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed McDermott , R . ( 1999 ). Why information technology inspired but cannot deliver knowledge management . California Management Review , 41 , 103 – 117 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Meade , M. L. , Watson , J. M. , Balota , D. A. , & Roediger , H. L . ( 2007 ). The roles of spreading activation and retrieval mode in producing false recognition in the DRM paradigm . Journal of Memory and Language , 56 , 305 – 320 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.07.007 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Mednick , S. A . ( 1962 ). The associative basis of the creative process . Psychological Review , 69 , 220 – 232 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed McKhann , G. , Drachman , D. , Folstein , M. , Katzman , R. , Price , D. , & Stadlan , E. M . ( 1984 ). Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: Report of the NINCDS-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease . Neurology , 34 , 939 – 944 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed McKone , E. , & Murphy , B . ( 2000 ). Implicit false memory: Effects of modality and multiple study presentations on long-lived semantic priming . Journal of Memory and Language , 43 , 89 – 109 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2702 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Morris , J. C. , Edland , S. , Clark , C. , Galasko , D. , Koss , E. , Mohs , R. ,… Heyman , A . ( 1993 ). The consortium to establish a registry for Alzheimer’s disease (CERAD). Part IV. Rates of cognitive change in the longitudinal assessment of probable Alzheimer’s disease . Neurology , 43 , 2457 – 2465 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Moulin , C. J. , James , N. , Freeman , J. E. , & Jones , R. W . ( 2004 ). Deficient acquisition and consolidation: Intertrial free recall performance in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment . Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology , 26 , 1 – 10 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.26.1.1.23940 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Nebes , R. D . ( 1989 ). Semantic memory in Alzheimer’s disease . Psychological Bulletin , 106 , 377 – 394 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Nedjam , Z. , Devouche , E. , & Dalla Barba , G . ( 2004 ). Confabulation, but not executive dysfunction discriminate AD from frontotemporal dementia . European Journal of Neurology , 11 , 728 – 733 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-1331.2004.00981.x Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Nelson , H. E . ( 1982 ). National Adult Reading Test (NART): For the assessment of premorbid intelligence in patients with dementia: Test manual . NFER-Nelson . Nichols , R. M. , Bogart , D. , & Loftus , E. F . ( 2015 ). False Memories. International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences ( 2nd ed ., pp. 709 – 714 ). Oxford : Elsevier . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Norman , K. A. , & Schacter , D. L . ( 1997 ). False recognition in younger and older adults: Exploring the characteristics of illusory memories . Memory & Cognition , 25 , 838 – 848 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Ober , B. A. , Dronkers , N. F. , Koss , E. , Delis , D. C. , & Friedland , R. P . ( 1986 ). Retrieval from semantic memory in Alzheimer-type dementia . Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology , 8 , 75 – 92 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638608401298 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Ober , B. A. , & Shenaut , G. K . ( 1988 ). Lexical decision and priming in Alzheimer’s disease . Neuropsychologia , 26 , 273 – 286 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(88)90080–2 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Olszewska , J. , & Ulatowska , J . ( 2013 ). Encoding strategy affects false recall and recognition: Evidence from categorical study material . Advances in Cognitive Psychology , 9 , 44 – 52 . doi: https://doi.org/10.2478/v10053-008-0130-0 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Roediger III, H. L. , Balota , D. A. , & Watson , J. M . ( 2001 ). Spreading activation and arousal of false memories . In H. L. Roediger , J. S. Nairne , I. Neath , & A. M. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 95 – 115 ). Washington, DC : American Psychological Association . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Roediger , H. L. , & McDermott , K. B . ( 1995 ). Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 21 , 803 – 814 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Roediger , H. L. 3rd , Watson , J. M. , McDermott , K. B. , & Gallo , D. A . ( 2001 ). Factors that determine false recall: A multiple regression analysis . Psychonomic Bulletin & Review , 8 , 385 – 407 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Rosa , N. M. , Deason , R. G. , Budson , A. E. , & Gutchess , A. H . ( 2015 ). Self-referencing and false memory in mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease . Neuropsychology , 29 , 799 – 805 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000186 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Schacter , D. L. , Guerin , S. A. , & St Jacques , P. L . ( 2011 ). Memory distortion: An adaptive perspective . Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 15 , 467 – 474 .doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612457377 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Schacter , D. L. , Koutstaal , W. , Johnson , M. K. , Gross , M. S. , & Angell , K. E . ( 1997 ). False recollection induced by photographs: A comparison of older and younger adults . Psychology and Aging , 12 , 203 – 215 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Snodgrass , J. G. , & Corwin , J . ( 1988 ). Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia . Journal of Experimental Psychology. General , 117 , 34 – 50 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Spieler , D. H. , Balota , D. A. , & Faust , M. E . ( 1996 ). Stroop performance in healthy younger and older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer’s type . Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance , 22 , 461 – 479 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Tun , P. A. , Wingfield , A. , Rosen , M. J. , & Blanchard , L . ( 1998 ). Response latencies for false memories: Gist-based processes in normal aging . Psychology and Aging , 13 , 230 – 241 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Waldie , B. D. , & Kwong See , S. T . ( 2003 ). Remembering words never presented: False memory effects in dementia of the Alzheimer type . Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition , 10 , 281 – 297 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1076/anec.10.4.281.28969 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Watson , J. M. , Balota , D. A. , & Sergent-Marshall , S. D . ( 2001 ). Semantic, phonological, and hybrid veridical and false memories in healthy older adults and in individuals with dementia of the Alzheimer type . Neuropsychology , 15 , 254 – 267 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1037//0894-4105.15.2.254 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Welsh , K. , Butters , N. , Hughes , J. , Mohs , R. , & Heyman , A . ( 1991 ). Detection of abnormal memory decline in mild cases of Alzheimer’s disease using CERAD neuropsychological measures . Archives of Neurology , 48 , 278 – 281 . doi: https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1991.00530150046016 Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed © The Author(s) 2018. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com. This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/about_us/legal/notices)

Journal

The Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social SciencesOxford University Press

Published: Jun 5, 2018

References