Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 14-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

Altered Neurophysiological Processing of Auditory Attention in Preschool Children With Sickle Cell Disease

Altered Neurophysiological Processing of Auditory Attention in Preschool Children With Sickle... Abstract Objective Sickle cell disease (SCD) is a genetic red blood cell disorder that often leads to stroke and executive dysfunction in school-age children and adults. This study aimed to characterize the development of the neural correlates of selective attention, an early component of executive function, in preschool children with SCD. Methods Auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded while children attended to a story stream in one ear and ignored a second story in the other ear interchangeably. In total, 12 patients (mean age = 5.5, 7 males) and 22 typically developing children (mean age = 4.4, 10 males) were included in the final analyses. Results By 100 ms, more positive ERP amplitudes were observed for attended relative to unattended stimuli in typically developing children but not those with SCD, suggesting deficits in the ability to focus attention. Reduced attention effects were associated with lower performance intellectual quotient Conclusion There are deficits in early attention modulation in young children with SCD. academic functioning, attention, developmental disabilities, neuropsychology, sickle cell disease Sickle cell disease (SCD) is a blood disorder that can result in a global pattern of diffuse brain injury, thought in part to be secondary to chronic anemia and hypoxia (Baldeweg et al., 2006; Steen et al., 2005). Clinical stroke, a focal neurological event lasting >24 hr, and silent stroke, which results in small lesions that can only be observed through structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), are commonly reported in patients with SCD, but volumetric differences can also be observed in quantitative imaging studies even when structural MRI appears normal (Jordan & DeBaun, 2017; van der Land et al., 2015). The frontal lobes have a protracted period of development and the frontoparietal regions, supplied by the internal carotid artery and the middle and anterior cerebral arteries, are most commonly affected by stroke in SCD (Ohene-Frempong et al., 1998). There is also evidence for abnormal cerebral blood flow, despite normal MRI (Prohovnik, Hurlet-Jensen, Adams, De Vivo, & Pavlakis, 2009). As a result, the frontal lobe and its connections are thought to be the most susceptible areas to SCD-related pathology. The frontal cortex plays a prominent role in the brain network underlying executive skills, the cognitive domain most affected in children with SCD with or without visible tissue injury (Hogan, Telfer, Kirkham, & de Haan, 2013; Schatz, Finke, Kellett, & Kramer, 2002; Wang et al., 2001; Watkins et al., 1998). Neurocognitive deficits appear early in development impacting intellectual quotient (IQ) and school readiness; however, the developmental trajectory of executive skills in children with SCD compared with their peers remains unclear (Hogan et al., 2013; Noll et al., 2001; Steen et al., 2002; Tarazi, Grant, Ely, & Barakat, 2007; Thompson, Gustafson, Bonner, & Ware, 2002). Selective attention is the ability to enhance the processing of relevant stimuli while suppressing the processing of irrelevant or distracting stimuli (Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Hillyard, Hink, Schwent, & Picton, 1973). The ability to selectively attend to target stimuli and ignore irrelevant environmental information is present from early in life, but the speed and efficiency of these processes increase with age (Ridderinkhof & van der Stelt, 2000). Selective attention and the neural systems that underlie this process undergo significant development in the preschool years (Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2004; Rueda, Rothbart, McCandliss, Saccomanno, & Posner, 2005). As well as laying down a foundation for the development of later emerging executive skills, selective attention also plays a role in other cognitive domains such as language, writing, and mathematics (Engle & Kane, 2004; Stevens & Bavelier, 2012), and is an important predictor of school readiness and academic achievement (Duncan et al., 2007; Rueda, Checa, & Rothbart, 2010). Few studies have attempted to study selective attention skills in detail in SCD. A recent study found specific selective attention deficits on the Test of Everyday Attention in a sample of adults with SCD as compared with matched controls, despite no evidence of MRI pathology (Vichinsky et al., 2010). Many developmental models of executive function depict it as a process of simpler skills, such as selective attention, emerging first which are then bootstrapped into more complex executive skills (Anderson, 2002). Understanding when deficits emerge may be a first step to preventing potential snowballing effects of early skill deficits impacting the emergence of more complex skills. The use of event-related potential (ERP) and electroencephalography (EEG) techniques offers a window into potential differences in the brain functions underlying executive skills such as selective attention. EEG is the measurement of continuous electrical activity in the brain, while the ERP is a voltage change that is time-locked to a specific stimulus. An ERP is acquired by averaging multiple trials, so that the EEG signal of interest can be isolated from other brain activity (Taylor & Baldeweg, 2002). ERP components are averaged waveforms that have been attributed to a certain cognitive or sensory process (Downes, Bathelt, & de Haan, 2017). ERPs have high temporal resolution, are not prone to examiner bias, and offer the opportunity to conduct robust investigations of executive functions using a multi-level methodology (Astle & Scerif, 2009). Hillyard and colleagues first identified the ERP index of selective auditory attention in adults using a dichotic listening task where the adults were instructed to pay attention to a series of tones presented in one ear and to ignore tones in the other ear (Hillyard et al., 1973). In response to these tones, adults typically show an early positivity (P1) followed by a negative component (N1) at about 100 ms and a second positivity (P2) at ∼200 ms (Nager, Estorf, & Münte, 2006). Selective attention is observed in adults as a larger N1 for the attended stimuli relative to the unattended stimuli. Instead of the P1-N1-P2 response observed in adults, children tend to show an elongated broad positivity that starts at ∼100 ms in the same conditions (Sharma, Kraus, McGee, & Nicol, 1997). ERP research using dichotic listening paradigms has shown that children as young as 3 years can selectively attend to one auditory source while ignoring another (Sanders et al., 2006; Sanders & Zobel, 2012). In response to probe stimuli, preschool children from 3-year-olds have been found to produce a broad positivity that extends from 100 to ∼300 ms, while 6–8-year-olds show a shorter positivity from 100 to 200 ms (Sanders et al., 2006). This positivity is observed to have larger amplitudes in response to attended stimuli. The extended effect in younger children could reflect a prolonged influence of attention on neural processing and variability between and within children (Stevens & Bavelier, 2012). In a recent study, the transition from the neurophysiological response observed in young children to the adult P1-N1-P2 response was investigated and it was found that the P1-N1-P2 complex emerges in early adolescence (Karns, Isbell, Giuliano, & Neville, 2015). Initial evidence of the earlier P1 can be observed from 10 years of age but is not present in younger children (Karns et al., 2015). The P1-N1-P2 complex has a protracted developmental course and changes in functional development likely reflect the slow development of the frontoparietal network (Ponton et al., 2000; Yurgelun-Todd, 2007). The aim of this study is to explore potential neurophysiological differences in selective auditory attention in preschool children with SCD using an ecologically valid dichotic listening paradigm that was developed based on previous studies with preschool children (Isbell, Stevens, Hampton Wray, Bell, & Neville, 2016; Sanders et al., 2006). It is hypothesized that children with SCD will show less pronounced amplitudes as compared with the typically developing children in the attended condition and thereby demonstrate less of an attention effect. The attention effect, or the mean amplitude difference between the attended and the unattended condition, has been previously explored in this age range as an index of attention control (Isbell, Hampton Wray, & Neville, 2016). Given that the ERP response elicited in this task is an internal and measurable aspect of cognitive control (Van Beijsterveldt & Van Baal, 2002), a secondary aim is to look at associations between neural modulation and performance on behavioral tasks of nonverbal IQ and executive attention. It is hypothesized that the children with the poorest executive scores on the neuropsychological assessments will show the smallest attention effect in the ERP response. Methods Participants Patients were informed of the study by their consultant hematologist at Barts NHS Trust during their routine clinical visit if they met the inclusionary criteria. Inclusionary criteria for the patient group and the control group included being aged between 36 and 72 months, fluent in English, and no history of stroke, brain MRI investigations, known neurological issues, other disorders, or preterm delivery. A diagnosis of SCD based on genotype was required. The participation rate for patients was ∼85%. Reasons for nonparticipation included unavailability or a lack of response after referral. Three families cancelled the scheduled session. Control children were recruited through the same clinics as the patients and through local advertisement. Twenty-four patients (23 HbSS genotype and 1 HbSC genotype; full-scale IQ [FSIQ] = 98.8) and 38 typically developing control children (FSIQ = 108.7) were recruited. All children spoke English as their first language. Mean averaged maximum velocity in the middle cerebral artery for patients at the most recent clinical visit was 155.3 (21.3) cm/s; no patients had abnormal transcranial Doppler recordings (velocity >200 cm/s). Five patients were currently on blood transfusion, four patients were being treated with hydroxyurea, and no patients reported experiencing current pain. Thirty-four EEG data sets were available for analysis after preprocessing (12 patients [1 HbSC] and 22 comparison children; Table I). Two patients and five controls were excluded because of EEG system error. Additionally, two patients and five controls were excluded because of removal of sensor net and/or headphones during the session. One control child had thick braids that prevented successful application of the apparatus. It was disclosed during assessment that a control child was born extremely preterm and so was excluded from analysis. Additionally, one patient was diagnosed with specific language impairment (SLI) between recruitment and assessment, and one patient did not pass the training phase because of poor comprehension of instruction. Six patients and four controls were removed for not meeting the minimum criteria of 25 stimuli per condition after preprocessing. Final groups, including 12 patients and 22 control children, were matched for age, full-scale IQ, socioeconomic status, and gender (Table I), although only 36% of the control group was matched for ethnicity (Black British). Table I. Group Descriptives Variable Patient group (n = 12) Control group (n = 22) p-value Age (mean; SD) 5.0 (0.96) 4.4 (0.92) .1 FSIQ (M; SD) 101.2 (10.5) 109.0 (14.3) .1 VIQ 101.1 (10.9) 112.4 (11.5) .01 PIQ 99.3 (12.3) 105.6 (15.5) .2 NIHTB attentional control 94.7 (24.4) 102.3 (17.5) (n = 17) .3 DDTP omissions 23.4 (19.6) (n = 10) 23.4 (15.7) (n = 13) .9 DDTP commissions 72.8 (121.6) (n = 10) 12.03 (21.7) (n = 13) .1 Male 7 10 .4 Ethnicity Black British 12 8 White British 10 Asian British/Other 4 Socioeconomic status (by income) .7 Lowest 3 4 Middle 6 10 Highest 3 6 No information 0 2 Maternal education .5 Secondary level 5 7 Third level 6 10 No information 1 5 Variable Patient group (n = 12) Control group (n = 22) p-value Age (mean; SD) 5.0 (0.96) 4.4 (0.92) .1 FSIQ (M; SD) 101.2 (10.5) 109.0 (14.3) .1 VIQ 101.1 (10.9) 112.4 (11.5) .01 PIQ 99.3 (12.3) 105.6 (15.5) .2 NIHTB attentional control 94.7 (24.4) 102.3 (17.5) (n = 17) .3 DDTP omissions 23.4 (19.6) (n = 10) 23.4 (15.7) (n = 13) .9 DDTP commissions 72.8 (121.6) (n = 10) 12.03 (21.7) (n = 13) .1 Male 7 10 .4 Ethnicity Black British 12 8 White British 10 Asian British/Other 4 Socioeconomic status (by income) .7 Lowest 3 4 Middle 6 10 Highest 3 6 No information 0 2 Maternal education .5 Secondary level 5 7 Third level 6 10 No information 1 5 Note. Pearsons’s chi-square used for category comparison and independent t-tests for comparison of continuous variables. DDTP = Doggie Deletion Task for Preschoolers; FSIQ = full-scale IQ; NIHTB = NIH toolbox; PIQ= performance IQ; VIQ = verbal IQ. Some of the participants did not complete the paper-based DDTP task because of task modification in the pilot phase (seven control children), timing issues (two patients/one control child), and experimenter error (one control child). Five of the control children did not complete the NIHTB task because of equipment issues. Table I. Group Descriptives Variable Patient group (n = 12) Control group (n = 22) p-value Age (mean; SD) 5.0 (0.96) 4.4 (0.92) .1 FSIQ (M; SD) 101.2 (10.5) 109.0 (14.3) .1 VIQ 101.1 (10.9) 112.4 (11.5) .01 PIQ 99.3 (12.3) 105.6 (15.5) .2 NIHTB attentional control 94.7 (24.4) 102.3 (17.5) (n = 17) .3 DDTP omissions 23.4 (19.6) (n = 10) 23.4 (15.7) (n = 13) .9 DDTP commissions 72.8 (121.6) (n = 10) 12.03 (21.7) (n = 13) .1 Male 7 10 .4 Ethnicity Black British 12 8 White British 10 Asian British/Other 4 Socioeconomic status (by income) .7 Lowest 3 4 Middle 6 10 Highest 3 6 No information 0 2 Maternal education .5 Secondary level 5 7 Third level 6 10 No information 1 5 Variable Patient group (n = 12) Control group (n = 22) p-value Age (mean; SD) 5.0 (0.96) 4.4 (0.92) .1 FSIQ (M; SD) 101.2 (10.5) 109.0 (14.3) .1 VIQ 101.1 (10.9) 112.4 (11.5) .01 PIQ 99.3 (12.3) 105.6 (15.5) .2 NIHTB attentional control 94.7 (24.4) 102.3 (17.5) (n = 17) .3 DDTP omissions 23.4 (19.6) (n = 10) 23.4 (15.7) (n = 13) .9 DDTP commissions 72.8 (121.6) (n = 10) 12.03 (21.7) (n = 13) .1 Male 7 10 .4 Ethnicity Black British 12 8 White British 10 Asian British/Other 4 Socioeconomic status (by income) .7 Lowest 3 4 Middle 6 10 Highest 3 6 No information 0 2 Maternal education .5 Secondary level 5 7 Third level 6 10 No information 1 5 Note. Pearsons’s chi-square used for category comparison and independent t-tests for comparison of continuous variables. DDTP = Doggie Deletion Task for Preschoolers; FSIQ = full-scale IQ; NIHTB = NIH toolbox; PIQ= performance IQ; VIQ = verbal IQ. Some of the participants did not complete the paper-based DDTP task because of task modification in the pilot phase (seven control children), timing issues (two patients/one control child), and experimenter error (one control child). Five of the control children did not complete the NIHTB task because of equipment issues. Selective Auditory Attention Task The ERP paradigm and processing pipeline were piloted with adults and two preschool-age children. Several measures were taken to make this task more age-appropriate including the development of training and practice phases. Custom static low-detail visual cues for on-screen presentation were developed to match the story to be attended. Arrows were created for on-screen presentation to reduce memory load. Fifteen short stories from Aesop’s classical fables were adapted to be age-appropriate. All stories were recorded in a male and a female voice, so that the child was always attending to a male and a female voice concurrently, helping them to differentiate between the two auditory streams. Sound recordings were edited using Audacity software to remove gaps >100 ms and to make the recordings comparable for loudness. Story narrators were instructed to keep their tone and pitch at a consistent level. Auditory and visual stimuli were presented through age-appropriate adjustable headphones and on a Dell Optiplex (Dell Inc., TX) computer screen running Windows XP using a script programmed via Matlab 2012 R2012b (The MathWorks, MA) and Psychtoolbox V3 (Kleiner et al., 2007). The child watched a cartoon while the net was positioned. This was followed by a training session. The child was instructed to touch the ear that corresponded with the side that the arrow on the screen was pointing toward. After the training phase, the child undertook a practice session before EEG recording began. Participants were cued to selectively attend to one of two simultaneously presented stories that differed in location (left/right), voice (male/female), and content. The stimuli of interest, pure tone bursts of white noise with a length of 500 ms, were randomly inserted into the attended and unattended streams at the same loudness level as the stories. At the beginning of each story the child heard “Are you ready?” and the researcher pressed a key to proceed. After each story, the child was asked questions relating to the attended story. The offset latency for the pure tone bursts of white noise was measured as 23 ms using the Electrical Geodesics, Inc. latency-testing device. There was a maximum of 14 story trials that were each up to 1 min in duration. The interstimulus interval was 1.5 s. If the child expressed a request to end the testing session, the researcher asked if they would like to attempt one more story trial and terminated the session if the child did not agree. Table II shows the average number of story trials completed for each group. Table II. Group Comparison on Behavioral Task Performance and Number of Usable Events Variable Patient group Control group p-value Total number of story trials completed M (SD) 10.7 (3.7) 10.8 (3.2) .92 Percentage of correct/attended story trials M (SD) 82.5 (18) 89.7 (17) .27 Number of events before processing M (SD) 107.5 (35) 123.4 (38) .17 Number of events after processing M (SD; N) 81 (35; 12) 86 (23; 22) .63 Variable Patient group Control group p-value Total number of story trials completed M (SD) 10.7 (3.7) 10.8 (3.2) .92 Percentage of correct/attended story trials M (SD) 82.5 (18) 89.7 (17) .27 Number of events before processing M (SD) 107.5 (35) 123.4 (38) .17 Number of events after processing M (SD; N) 81 (35; 12) 86 (23; 22) .63 Table II. Group Comparison on Behavioral Task Performance and Number of Usable Events Variable Patient group Control group p-value Total number of story trials completed M (SD) 10.7 (3.7) 10.8 (3.2) .92 Percentage of correct/attended story trials M (SD) 82.5 (18) 89.7 (17) .27 Number of events before processing M (SD) 107.5 (35) 123.4 (38) .17 Number of events after processing M (SD; N) 81 (35; 12) 86 (23; 22) .63 Variable Patient group Control group p-value Total number of story trials completed M (SD) 10.7 (3.7) 10.8 (3.2) .92 Percentage of correct/attended story trials M (SD) 82.5 (18) 89.7 (17) .27 Number of events before processing M (SD) 107.5 (35) 123.4 (38) .17 Number of events after processing M (SD; N) 81 (35; 12) 86 (23; 22) .63 ERP Recording and Analysis EEG data were obtained and recorded using NetStation V4.1.2 (Electrical Geodesics Inc., OR) on Mac OS 10.3.9 software. A NetAmps 200 amplifier and HydroCel Geodesic EEG were recorded using the Electrical Geodesics sensor net system from 128 electrodes and digitized at 250 Hz with a bandwidth of 0.1–100 Hz. A ground electrode was in place and the vertex electrode (Cz) was used as an online reference. Channel impedances were adjusted where necessary and appropriate to levels <50 kΩ. An electroculogram was recorded for the detection of eye-related artifacts. Electrodes were positioned above and below and to the side of both eyes. Offline, data were filtered with finite impulse response filters at a high-pass frequency of 0.1 Hz and a low-pass frequency of 30 Hz in EEGLAB 11.0.3 (Delorme et al., 2011). The EEG signal was epoched at 200 ms before the stimulus event (the pure tone bursts of white noise) to 600 ms after stimulus presentation. Automatic epoch rejection of bad epochs occurred at a threshold of plus or minus 100 microvolts. The average voltage of the 200 ms segment before stimulus onset was set as the baseline. Visual inspection was used to remove artifacts such as eye blinks, saccades, muscle activity, and skin potentials (Luck, 2005). The data were rereferenced from the vertex reference to an average montage. The time window of interest (100–300 ms) was chosen based on a review of the relevant literature for this age (Coch et al., 2005; Karns et al., 2015; Sanders, Stevens, Coch, & Neville, 2006; Sanders & Zobel, 2012; Stevens, Sanders, & Neville, 2006; Stevens et al., 2009). The mean peak amplitude for this time window was investigated. Trials were averaged together to acquire a single averaged segment for the ignore and attend condition for each participant. A criterion of a minimum of 25 artifact-free trials in each condition after all preprocessing steps was imposed. This figure is within the range of minimum number of trials required in similar ERP studies with young children (Coch, Sanders, & Neville, 2005; de Haan, Pascalis, & Johnson, 2002; Sanders et al., 2006). It has been recommended that studies with young children should include at least 10–20 trials per condition to obtain a reliable estimation of the ERP component under investigation (Cuevas, Cannon, Yoo, & Fox, 2014; DeBoer, Scott, Nelson, & de Haan, 2007). Each child completed up to 14 story sessions, so participants had up to 160 events (12–15 per story session) before processing. Children were reminded to fixate on the screen and to sit still between each story to maximize the number of artifact-free trials and were offered a teddy bear to hold if they struggled to stay still. To further improve signal-to-noise ratio, several channels were combined for channel-level analyses (Coch et al., 2005; Sanders & Zobel, 2012; Strait, Slater, Abecassis, & Kraus, 2014). Four electrode groupings (Figure 1) are defined based on previous studies with similar age groups and paradigms and a topographical investigation of the current population (Coch et al., 2005; Isbell, Stevens et al., 2016; Sanders et al., 2006). Figure 1. View largeDownload slide Electrode sites. The Geodesic Sensor Net 128 channel layout in accordance to the 10–20 system of electrode placement. Data were averaged across four channels at each site to increase the signal-to-noise ratio. The four channel cluster sites are located over the medial frontal, left frontal, right frontal, and frontocentral sites and are illustrated through the shaded dots. Figure 1. View largeDownload slide Electrode sites. The Geodesic Sensor Net 128 channel layout in accordance to the 10–20 system of electrode placement. Data were averaged across four channels at each site to increase the signal-to-noise ratio. The four channel cluster sites are located over the medial frontal, left frontal, right frontal, and frontocentral sites and are illustrated through the shaded dots. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI-III-UK) (population mean = 100, SD = 15) was administered to obtain IQ (Wechsler, 2002). The IQ scores for the final groups were slightly higher than the original group means for both groups, although executive scores were similar in the final group (Table I) to that observed for the original groups. Children were required to have a verbal IQ >75 to proceed to the ERP task. This threshold was implemented to ensure that children could comprehend task instruction. The chosen cutoff was based on language cutoffs used in similar studies that used ERP paradigms to investigate attention in preschool-age children (Stevens, Lauinger, & Neville, 2009). NIH Toolbox Test of Attention Control The standardized NIH toolbox (NIHTB) test of attention control (population mean = 100, SD = 15) was administered. Poorer scores in the NIHTB task reflect longer reaction time and incorrect responses. The NIHTB task has been validated from 3 years of age (Zelazo et al., 2013). Five control participants had missing NIHTB data because of technical difficulties during data collection. Doggie Deletion Task for Preschoolers The Doggie Deletion Task for Preschoolers (DDTP), a revised cancellation task, was also used to measure attention control (Byrne, Bawden, DeWolfe, & Beattie, 1998). More omissions and commissions on the DDTP reflect poorer attention control. The DDTP task was previously developed with typically developing children at the London Babylab (Downes et al., 2016). Procedure Ethical approval was obtained from the local NHS committee (Ref: 13/LO/0962), and site-specific approval was obtained from UCL Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health and Barts NHS Trust. Written informed consent was obtained from each parent and verbal assent was obtained from each child. The testing session took place in the London Babylab, UCL Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health. All consent procedures and data collection were conducted in English by the same researcher (M.D.). All children first completed the WPPSI-III-UK, followed by the DDTP and the NIHTB test of attention control, in that order. After a scheduled break, the child then completed the EEG session, which lasted ∼30 min. Results ERP Behavioral Results There were no group differences between the total number of completed story trials, number of correctly attended story trials, number of events before preprocessing, or final number of events (Table II). Group Differences As expected, the control group showed a broad positivity peaking at ∼150–200 ms poststimulus onset. However, this was not observed for the patient group who showed a less pronounced and inconsistent amplitude (Figure 2). Figure 2. View largeDownload slide ERP waveforms for both groups. Grand average ERP plots. Plots show attended (broken lines) and unattended (solid lines) waveforms for the SCD group over the right frontal (A), left frontal (B), centrofrontal (C), and frontomedial (D) sites and for the typically developing children over the right frontal (E), left frontal (F), centrofrontal (G), and frontomedial (H) sites, showing differences in the area of interest (shaded gray area). Note. ERP = event-related potential; SCD = sickle cell disease. Figure 2. View largeDownload slide ERP waveforms for both groups. Grand average ERP plots. Plots show attended (broken lines) and unattended (solid lines) waveforms for the SCD group over the right frontal (A), left frontal (B), centrofrontal (C), and frontomedial (D) sites and for the typically developing children over the right frontal (E), left frontal (F), centrofrontal (G), and frontomedial (H) sites, showing differences in the area of interest (shaded gray area). Note. ERP = event-related potential; SCD = sickle cell disease. Overall group differences for attended and unattended conditions were further explored across the four frontal clusters of interest using a multivariate analysis of variance (ANOVA). Consistent with previous studies, the control group showed the attention effect (a larger response to the attended signal) to be greatest at the frontomedial site in comparison with the other three sites of interest; however, this did not reach significance (F(2.2, 46.19) = 2.164, p = .10). In contrast to the control group, the patients did not show as strong an effect for the attended condition at any of the sites (see Figure 3). The control group showed a significant difference in the mean peak between the attended and unattended condition (t = 2.2, p = .04), but this was not observed for the patients (t = .60, p = .56). The frontomedial electrode grouping was chosen from the four clusters of interest for further analysis. Mean amplitudes were analyzed using a two-way univariate ANOVA with group (patient vs. control) and condition (attended vs. unattended) as the between-subject factors and the mean amplitudes at the frontomedial site as the within-subject factor. The two mechanisms underlying attention modulation in this task, enhancement of the amplitude in the attended condition and suppression of the amplitude in the unattended condition, were analyzed. Significant group differences were observed between groups for amplitude in the attended condition at the frontomedial site with the control group showing larger mean amplitudes (t = 2.2, p = .03), but no group differences were observed for the unattended condition (t = −.12, p = .91). Table III shows that, although there was greater mean amplitudes for the control group in the attended condition on the other sites, this did not reach significance (F(4, 29) = 1.72, p = .16), and there was no trend observed at any site for the unattended condition. Table III. Mean Amplitude of the Early Frontal Positivity (100–300 ms) in the Medial Frontal, Left Frontal, Right Frontal, and Central Sites for Both Conditions Group Condition Medial frontal Left frontal Right frontal Frontocentral M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) Patient group Attended −0.229 (0.57) 0.381 (0.47) −0.071 (0.59) 0.386 (0.41) Unattended 0.249 (.49) 0.462 (0.43) 0.282 (0.38) 0.649 (0.43) Control group Attended 1.637 (0.58) 1.389 (0.48) 0.662 (0.29) 1.234 (0.49) Unattended 0.167 (.46) 0.369 (0.57) 0.282 (0.37) 0.555 (0.41) Group Condition Medial frontal Left frontal Right frontal Frontocentral M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) Patient group Attended −0.229 (0.57) 0.381 (0.47) −0.071 (0.59) 0.386 (0.41) Unattended 0.249 (.49) 0.462 (0.43) 0.282 (0.38) 0.649 (0.43) Control group Attended 1.637 (0.58) 1.389 (0.48) 0.662 (0.29) 1.234 (0.49) Unattended 0.167 (.46) 0.369 (0.57) 0.282 (0.37) 0.555 (0.41) Note. Significant group differences (p < .05) are shown in bold, and trends for group differences (p < .1) are shown in italics. Table III. Mean Amplitude of the Early Frontal Positivity (100–300 ms) in the Medial Frontal, Left Frontal, Right Frontal, and Central Sites for Both Conditions Group Condition Medial frontal Left frontal Right frontal Frontocentral M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) Patient group Attended −0.229 (0.57) 0.381 (0.47) −0.071 (0.59) 0.386 (0.41) Unattended 0.249 (.49) 0.462 (0.43) 0.282 (0.38) 0.649 (0.43) Control group Attended 1.637 (0.58) 1.389 (0.48) 0.662 (0.29) 1.234 (0.49) Unattended 0.167 (.46) 0.369 (0.57) 0.282 (0.37) 0.555 (0.41) Group Condition Medial frontal Left frontal Right frontal Frontocentral M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) Patient group Attended −0.229 (0.57) 0.381 (0.47) −0.071 (0.59) 0.386 (0.41) Unattended 0.249 (.49) 0.462 (0.43) 0.282 (0.38) 0.649 (0.43) Control group Attended 1.637 (0.58) 1.389 (0.48) 0.662 (0.29) 1.234 (0.49) Unattended 0.167 (.46) 0.369 (0.57) 0.282 (0.37) 0.555 (0.41) Note. Significant group differences (p < .05) are shown in bold, and trends for group differences (p < .1) are shown in italics. Figure 3. View largeDownload slide Topographical maps for both groups. Topographic two-dimensional voltage maps show scalp-potential distributions averaged over a 200 ms time-window. Figures indicate magnitude and ranges of ERPs elicited for (A) attended condition, (B) ignored condition and (C) the attention effect (difference wave = attended–unattended) for SCD patients and (D) attended condition, (E) ignored condition, and (F) attention effect for typically developing control children at 100–300 ms. Maxima corresponding to frontal effects are evident for the typically developing children in the attended condition (D) but are not as perceptible for the control children (A). A larger attention effect or difference wave in the frontal region can be observed for the controls (F) than for the patients (C). Note. ERP = event-related potential; SCD = sickle cell disease. Figure 3. View largeDownload slide Topographical maps for both groups. Topographic two-dimensional voltage maps show scalp-potential distributions averaged over a 200 ms time-window. Figures indicate magnitude and ranges of ERPs elicited for (A) attended condition, (B) ignored condition and (C) the attention effect (difference wave = attended–unattended) for SCD patients and (D) attended condition, (E) ignored condition, and (F) attention effect for typically developing control children at 100–300 ms. Maxima corresponding to frontal effects are evident for the typically developing children in the attended condition (D) but are not as perceptible for the control children (A). A larger attention effect or difference wave in the frontal region can be observed for the controls (F) than for the patients (C). Note. ERP = event-related potential; SCD = sickle cell disease. The Attention Effect and Removal of Outliers Near-significant group differences were observed for the magnitude of the attention effect (the difference between the attended and unattended conditions) at the frontomedial site (t = 1.8, p = .07). Group analyses were repeated to ensure that the three outliers in the negative range for the attention effect in the control group (all 3-year-olds suggesting more variability in the youngest children) and the main outlier in the patient group were not having effect on group differences. The group difference for the attention effect (attended–unattended) reached significance (p = .006) after removal of outliers. The post hoc group difference observed for the attended condition also increased in significance (p = .013), although the lack of a group difference observed for the unattended condition remained the same. Associations With Cognitive Measures There was no relation observed between performance IQ (PIQ) and the attention effect using Pearson’s correlations, although a greater positivity in the attended condition was significantly associated with PIQ (r = .483, p = .004; Figure 4A). When the groups were separated, this association remained significant for the patients (r = .619, p = .040) but became a near-significant trend for the control children (r = .415, p = .055). Associations with omissions and commissions on the DDTP were explored using Spearman’s correlations (as scores were not normally distributed), and it was found that children with larger peaks in the unattended condition made more omissions across both groups (rho = .465, p = .02, n = 23; Figure 4B) and for the control group (rho = .583, p = .036) and patient group separately (rho = .717, p = .045). No associations with commissions were observed. The significance of the DDTP findings did not change when outliers were removed. No significant correlations emerged for the NIH attention control task (Table IV). Table IV. Associations Between the Behavioral Variables and the Neurophysiological Correlates of Attention Control Measure Patient group Control group Total Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition DDTP omissions* .150 .517 .717* .149 .501 .583* .128 .449 .465* DDTP commissions* .192 −.268 −.603 .186 .081 −.025 −.047 −.258 −.235 NIH inhibitory control .242 .485 .174 .023 −.309 −.364 .165 .097 −.144 Measure Patient group Control group Total Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition DDTP omissions* .150 .517 .717* .149 .501 .583* .128 .449 .465* DDTP commissions* .192 −.268 −.603 .186 .081 −.025 −.047 −.258 −.235 NIH inhibitory control .242 .485 .174 .023 −.309 −.364 .165 .097 −.144 * Spearman’s correlations used because of nonnormal distribution of the DDTP variables; *p < .05. Table IV. Associations Between the Behavioral Variables and the Neurophysiological Correlates of Attention Control Measure Patient group Control group Total Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition DDTP omissions* .150 .517 .717* .149 .501 .583* .128 .449 .465* DDTP commissions* .192 −.268 −.603 .186 .081 −.025 −.047 −.258 −.235 NIH inhibitory control .242 .485 .174 .023 −.309 −.364 .165 .097 −.144 Measure Patient group Control group Total Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition DDTP omissions* .150 .517 .717* .149 .501 .583* .128 .449 .465* DDTP commissions* .192 −.268 −.603 .186 .081 −.025 −.047 −.258 −.235 NIH inhibitory control .242 .485 .174 .023 −.309 −.364 .165 .097 −.144 * Spearman’s correlations used because of nonnormal distribution of the DDTP variables; *p < .05. Figure 4. View largeDownload slide ERP and behavior. The relations between ERP and cognitive tasks. (A) Association between the ERP for the attended condition and PIQ and (B) association between the ERP for the unattended condition and number of omissions. Note. ERP = event-related potential; PIQ = performance IQ. Figure 4. View largeDownload slide ERP and behavior. The relations between ERP and cognitive tasks. (A) Association between the ERP for the attended condition and PIQ and (B) association between the ERP for the unattended condition and number of omissions. Note. ERP = event-related potential; PIQ = performance IQ. Discussion Previous research with preschool children in special populations who have known executive deficits has found evidence for altered neural processing in auditory attention modulation (Stevens et al., 2006; 2009). Here, we extend this research to children with SCD. The main finding is that children with SCD show a less pronounced positive amplitude to stimuli in the attended stream and have a poorer “difference score” or attention effect. This study provides novel evidence for specific deficits in attention modulation in the neurophysiological response of young children with SCD. These results align with the behavioral findings of poorer executive function, particularly attention control, in older children with SCD (Daly, Kral, & Tarazi, 2011). The current findings are also in line with two previous ERP studies in SCD that found more diminished and variable ERP responses on tasks of executive skills (Colombatti et al., 2015; Hogan et al., 2006). Hogan and colleagues measured ERP components related to performance monitoring in children with SCD between 11- and 23-year-olds, with and without evidence of silent lesions, and found that children with SCD showed some evidence for executive deficits in comparison with typically developing controls, even in the absence of stroke, as demonstrated by a reduced error-related negativity amplitude, which reflects reduced unconscious processing of errors (Downes, Bathelt, & de Haan, 2017). They also administered a battery of behavioral executive tasks where both sickle groups showed significantly poorer scores across several domains, including selective attention. Colombatti and colleagues investigated the P3 using an auditory oddball paradigm in children with SCD between 6- and 15-year-olds who had no history of stroke. The P3, a positive ERP component that reflects information processing during tasks that involve attending to and discriminating between target stimuli and distracting stimuli, was found to be more protracted and variable in the children with SCD. In the present study, there was no difference between groups in the amount of trials completed or the amount of correctly attended stories, despite the difference in the underlying neural processes, highlighting the sensitivity of ERPs to processing differences. Stevens, Sanders, and Neville (2006) reported similar findings for their cohort of children with SLI, who also showed equivalent behavioral performance. Hogan et al. (2006) also reported no group differences in task performance on their performance-monitoring task in children with SCD, despite neurophysiological differences. The pattern for larger amplitudes in the attended condition for the control group, and their absence in the SCD patient group, suggests that the SCD group had difficulties with signal enhancement rather than distractor suppression in attention modulation (Stevens et al., 2009; Stevens, Sanders, & Neville, 2006). Although attenuated ERPs were anticipated for patients in the attended condition, the lack of any significant response was not expected. Previous research on auditory attention allocation in children with autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has also found it to be absent, attenuated, or inconsistent in multiple studies, even when compared with typically developing children (Donkers et al., 2015; Gomes et al., 2012; Loiselle, Stamm, Maitinsky, & Whipple, 1980). The lack of a response in the attended condition may suggest that patients are allocating limited attention resources, are less automatic in the allocation of these resources, or alternatively, that there is a dampening of information, more limited activation, or immature neural synchronization (Gilley, Sharma, Dorman, & Martin, 2005). Thus, children with SCD may have less established neural sources of attentional modulation, and the lack of neurophysiological modulation reflects poorly attuned attentional control. In the current study, there was an association observed between nonverbal IQ and the ERP amplitude in the attended condition, but not for the attention effect. A relation between the attention effect and nonverbal IQ has been reported in preschool children from low SES backgrounds, while additional studies have shown a relationship between the ERP amplitude in the unattended condition and parent reports of executive function (Isbell, Stevens et al., 2016; Lackner et al., 2013). Associations with the ERP were only observed for one of the two attention control tasks in our battery, where children with larger peaks in the unattended condition made more omission errors. Missing data or differences in task demands could have contributed to the lack of relation with behavior performance on the NIHTB task. Behavioral tasks measure multiple steps of cognitive processing, while ERP measures have the advantage of breaking down these steps into a series of components, which makes it more difficult to draw associations between the two. The behavioral attention tasks were also limited as they were both visual; an auditory task may have been more informative for direct comparison. Limitations One limitation of this study is the lack of neuroimaging, which means that although patients with a known history of stroke were excluded, some children may have had undetected silent stroke. Another limitation of this study is the small population. Nevertheless, the current patient population size was larger than the only previous published ERP studies in SCD (Colombatti et al., 2015; Hogan et al., 2006). A further limitation of this study was the high attrition rate, which resulted in only 34 data sets in the final analysis, representing only 55% of the children recruited for the study. High attrition rates of up to 45% in EEG and ERP studies are typical for this age range and are related to refusal to wear the EEG cap and excessive movement during the session (Cuevas et al., 2012; Morasch & Bell, 2011; Wolfe & Bell, 2007). Finally, as observed in previous ERP attention studies with this young age range, there was variability within each group (Isbell, Stevens et al., 2016; Stevens, Sanders, & Neville, 2006). Five patients (42%) and three children from the control group (14%) had attention effects >1 SD below the control group mean. Noisy ERP data in younger participants could have precluded the detection of stronger relations with behavioral measures. Future Investigations An advantage of the current paradigm is that it can be applied across a wide age range allowing for the developmental tracking of neural markers of attention (Strait et al., 2014). Future studies should apply this paradigm in older school-age children and adolescents to ascertain whether the lack of response in the current study is a developmental delay that eventually catches up over time or whether it is an early indicator of an altered course of functional neural development. Additionally, there is currently no published evidence of classical attention ERP paradigms, such as the auditory oddball paradigm, in infants and young children with SCD. Future research applying this paradigm with young children will help elucidate whether the early attention deficit in the current study is also evident in more basic attention experiments, or unique to the complex stimuli in the current study that require the exertion of more executive control. It is important to determine the role of early attention deficits in the development of well-established later executive deficits in older children with SCD (Schatz & Roberts, 2007). Similar to Barkley’s developmental model of executive function for children with ADHD, where primary deficits in inhibition are proposed to have knock-on effects for later emerging executive domains, it may be that poor attention control in young children with SCD impairs the emergence of higher-order executive functions (Barkley, 1997). Recent research suggests that ERPs can be used to index improvements in cognitive interventions with preschool children, potentially indicating better recruitment of neural systems important for selective attention (Espinet, Anderson, & Zelazo, 2013; Isbell, Stevens et al., 2016; Neville et al., 2013; Rueda, Checa, & Cómbita, 2012). Future research should consider using ERP responses in dichotic listening tasks as an end point in treatment trials of young children with SCD. Conclusion Taken together, these findings contribute to the elucidation of differences in the development of the neural underpinnings of selective attention in preschool children with SCD. Children with SCD specifically show poorer signal enhancement with attention. Further research is warranted to investigate potential differences in source localization and to determine whether group differences would be evident on other attention paradigms, such as the oddball paradigm. Future applications of the current paradigm in older children with SCD are also required to determine whether this group difference can still be observed. Nevertheless, the current study provides initial evidence for an altered neurophysiological response to selective attention in children with SCD, pinpointing a lack of signal enhancement, and also contributes further evidence for the relationship between behavioral performance and neural markers of attention. Acknowledgments The authors would like to thank the families who participated in this study. Funding This research was funded by the Child Health Research Charitable Incorporated Organisation and supported by the National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre at Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children NHS Foundation Trust and University College London and the National Institute for Health Research: Research for Patient Benefit Funding stream (PB-PG-1112-29099). Conflicts of interest: None declared. References Anderson P. ( 2002 ). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood . Child Neuropsychology , 8 , 71 – 82 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Astle D. E. , Scerif G. ( 2009 ). Using developmental cognitive neuroscience to study behavioral and attentional control . Developmental Psychobiology , 51 , 107 – 118 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Baldeweg T. , Hogan A. M. , Saunders D. E. , Telfer P. , Gadian D. G. , Vargha-Khadem F. , Kirkham F. J. ( 2006 ). Detecting white matter injury in sickle cell disease using voxel-based morphometry . Annals of Neurology , 59 , 662 – 672 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Barkley R. A. ( 1997 ). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, self-regulation, and time: Toward a more comprehensive theory . Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics , 18 , 271 – 279 . Google Scholar PubMed Byrne J. M. , Bawden H. N. , DeWolfe N. A. , Beattie T. L. ( 1998 ). Clinical assessment of psychopharmacological treatment of preschoolers with ADHD . Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology , 20 , 613 – 627 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Coch D. , Sanders L. D. , Neville H. J. ( 2005 ). An event-related potential study of selective auditory attention in children and adults . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 17 , 605 – 622 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Colombatti R. , Ermani M. , Rampazzo P. , Manara R. , Montanaro M. , Basso G. , Meneghetti G. , Sainati L. ( 2015 ). Cognitive evoked potentials and neural networks are abnormal in children with sickle cell disease and not related to the degree of anaemia, pain and silent infarcts . British Journal of Haematology , 169 , 597 – 600 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Cuevas K. , Cannon E. N. , Yoo K. , Fox N. A. ( 2014 ). The infant EEG mu rhythm: Methodological considerations and best practices . Developmental Review , 34 , 26 – 43 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Cuevas K. , Hubble M. , Bell M. A. ( 2012 ). Early childhood predictors of post-kindergarten executive function: Behavior, parent report, and psychophysiology . Early Education and Development , 23 , 59 – 73 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Daly B. , Kral M. C. , Tarazi R. A. ( 2011 ). The role of neuropsychological evaluation in pediatric sickle cell disease . The Clinical Neuropsychologist , 25 , 903 – 925 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed DeBoer T. , Scott L. S. , Nelson C. A. , de Haan M. ( 2007 ). Methods for acquiring and analyzing infant eventrelated potentials . In M. de Haan (Ed.), Infant EEG and event-related potentials ( 5 – 37 ). East Sussex: Psychology Press. de Haan M. , Pascalis O. , Johnson M. ( 2002 ). Specialization of neural mechanisms underlying face recognition in human infants . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 14 , 199 – 209 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Delorme A. , Mullen T. , Kothe C. , Acar Z. A. , Bigdely-Shamlo N. , Vankov A. , Makeig S. ( 2011 ). EEGLAB, SIFT, NFT, BCILAB, and ERICA: New tools for advanced EEG processing . Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience , 2011 , 130714 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Desimone R. , Duncan J. ( 1995 ). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention . Annual Review of Neuroscience , 18 , 193 – 222 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Donkers F. C. , Schipul S. E. , Baranek G. T. , Cleary K. M. , Willoughby M. T. , Evans A. M. , Bulluck J. C. , Lovmo J. E. , Belger A. ( 2015 ). Attenuated auditory event-related potentials and associations with atypical sensory response patterns in children with autism . Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders , 45 , 506 – 523 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Downes M. ( 2016 ). The development and assessment of executive functioning in preschool children with and without sickle cell anaemia . Doctoral thesis, UCL (University College London). Downes M. , Bathelt J. , de Haan M. ( 2017 ). Event-related potential measures of executive functioning from preschool to adolescence . Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology , 59 , 581 – 590 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Duncan G. J. , Dowsett C. J. , Claessens A. , Magnuson K. , Huston A. C. , Klebanov P. , Pagani L. S. , Feinstein L. , Engel M. , Brooks-Gunn J. , Brooks-Gunn J. , Sexton H. , Duckworth K. , Japel C. ( 2007 ). School readiness and later achievement . Developmental Psychology , 43 , 1428 – 46 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Engle R. W. , Kane M. J. ( 2004 ). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control . Psychology of Learning and Motivation , 44 , 145 – 200 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Espinet S. D. , Anderson J. E. , Zelazo P. D. ( 2013 ). Reflection training improves executive function in preschool-age children: Behavioral and neural effects . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 4 , 3 – 15 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Gilley P. M. , Sharma A. , Dorman M. , Martin K. ( 2005 ). Developmental changes in refractoriness of the cortical auditory evoked potential . Clinical Neurophysiology , 116 , 648 – 657 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Gomes H. , Duff M. , Ramos M. , Molholm S. , Foxe J. J. , Halperin J. ( 2012 ). Auditory selective attention and processing in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder . Clinical Neurophysiology , 123 , 293 – 302 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Hillyard S. A. , Hink R. F. , Schwent V. L. , Picton T. W. ( 1973 ). Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain . Science , 182 , 177 – 180 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Hogan A. M. , Telfer P. T. , Kirkham F. J. , de Haan M. ( 2013 ). Precursors of executive function in infants with sickle cell anemia . Journal of Child Neurology , 28 , 1197 – 1202 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Hogan A. M. , Vargha-Khadem F. , Saunders D. E. , Kirkham F. J. , Baldeweg T. ( 2006 ). Impact of frontal white matter lesions on performance monitoring: ERP evidence for cortical disconnection . Brain , 129 , 2177 – 2188 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Isbell E. , Hampton Wray A. , Neville H. J. ( 2016 ). Individual differences in neural mechanisms of selective auditory attention in preschoolers from lower socioeconomic status backgrounds: An event-related potentials study . Developmental Science , 19 , 865 – 880 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Isbell E. , Stevens C. , Hampton Wray A. , Bell T. , Neville H. J. ( 2016 ). 5-HTTLPR polymorphism is linked to neural mechanisms of selective attention in preschoolers from lower socioeconomic status backgrounds . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 22 , 36 – 47 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Jordan C. L. , DeBaun M. R. ( 2017 ). Cerebral hemodynamic assessment and neuroimaging across the lifespan in sickle cell disease . Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0271678X17701763?url_ver=Z39.88-2003&rfr_id=ori:rid:crossref.org&rfr_dat=cr_pub%3dpubmed. Karns C. M. , Isbell E. , Giuliano R. J. , Neville H. J. ( 2015 ). Auditory attention in childhood and adolescence: An event-related potential study of spatial selective attention to one of two simultaneous stories . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 13 , 53 – 67 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Kleiner M. , Brainard D. , Pelli D. , Ingling A. , Murray R. , Broussard C. ( 2007 ). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3 . Perception , 36 , 1 – 16 . Lackner C. L. , Santesso D. L. , Dywan J. , Wade T. J. , Segalowitz S. J. ( 2013 ). Electrocortical indices of selective attention predict adolescent executive functioning . Biological Psychology , 93 , 325 – 333 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Loiselle D. L. , Stamm J. S. , Maitinsky S. , Whipple S. C. ( 1980 ). Evoked potential and behavioral signs of attentive dysfunctions in hyperactive boys . Psychophysiology , 17 , 193 – 201 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Luck S. J. ( 2005 ). Ten simple rules for designing ERP experiments. In Handy T. C. (Ed.), Event-related potentials: A methods handbook . Cambridge : MIT Press . Morasch K. C. , Bell M. A. ( 2011 ). The role of inhibitory control in behavioral and physiological expressions of toddler executive function . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 108 , 593 – 606 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Nager W. , Estorf K. , Münte T. F. ( 2006 ). Crossmodal attention effects on brain responses to different stimulus classes . BMC Neuroscience , 7 , 31 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Neville H. J. , Stevens C. , Pakulak E. , Bell T. A. , Fanning J. , Klein S. , Isbell E. ( 2013 ). Family-based training program improves brain function, cognition, and behavior in lower socioeconomic status pre-schoolers . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America , 110 , 12138 – 12143 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Noll R. B. , Stith L. , Gartstein M. A. , Ris M. D. , Grueneich R. , Vannatta K. , Kalinyak K. ( 2001 ). Neuropsychological functioning of youths with sickle cell disease: Comparison with non-chronically ill peers . Journal of Pediatric Psychology , 26 , 69 – 78 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Ohene-Frempong K. , Weiner S. J. , Sleeper L. A. , Miller S. T. , Embury S. , Moohr J. W. , Wethers D. L. , Pegelow C. H. , Gill F. M. ( 1998 ). Cerebrovascular accidents in sickle cell disease: Rates and risk factors . Blood , 91 , 288 – 294 . Google Scholar PubMed Ponton C. W. , Eggermont J. J. , Kwong B. , Don M. ( 2000 ). Maturation of human central auditory system activity: Evidence from multi-channel evoked potentials . Clinical Neurophysiology , 111 , 220 – 236 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Prohovnik I. , Hurlet-Jensen A. , Adams R. , De Vivo D. , Pavlakis S. G. ( 2009 ). Hemodynamic etiology of elevated flow velocity and stroke in sickle-cell disease . Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism , 29 , 803 – 810 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Ridderinkhof K. R. , van der Stelt O. ( 2000 ). Attention and selection in the growing child: Views derived from developmental psychophysiology . Biological Psychology , 54 , 55 – 106 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Rueda M. R. , Checa P. , Cómbita L. M. ( 2012 ). Enhanced efficiency of the executive attention network after training in preschool children: Immediate changes and effects after two months . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 2 , S192 – S204 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Rueda M. R. , Checa P. , Rothbart M. K. ( 2010 ). Contributions of attentional control to socioemotional and academic development . Early Education and Development , 21 , 744 – 764 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Rueda M. R. , Posner M. I. , Rothbart M. K. ( 2004 ). Attentional control and self-regulation. In K.D .Vohs & R.F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (284–299). New York: The Guilford Press. Rueda M. R. , Rothbart M. K. , McCandliss B. D. , Saccomanno L. , Posner M. I. ( 2005 ). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America , 102 , 14931 – 14936 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Sanders L. D. , Stevens C. , Coch D. , Neville H. J. ( 2006 ). Selective auditory attention in 3-to 5-year-old children: An event-related potential study . Neuropsychologia , 44 , 2126 – 2138 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Sanders L. D. , Zobel B. H. ( 2012 ). Nonverbal spatially selective attention in 4-and 5-year-old children . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 2 , 317 – 328 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Schatz J. , Finke R. L. , Kellett J. M. , Kramer J. H. ( 2002 ). Cognitive functioning in children with sickle cell disease: A meta-analysis . Journal of Pediatric Psychology , 27 , 739 – 748 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Schatz J. , Roberts C. W. ( 2007 ). Neurobehavioral impact of sickle cell disease in early childhood . Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society , 13 , 933 – 43 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Sharma A. , Kraus N. , McGee T. J. , Nicol T. G. ( 1997 ). Developmental changes in P1 and N1 central auditory responses elicited by consonant-vowel syllables . Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section , 104 , 540 – 545 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Steen R. G. , Emudianughe T. , Hunte M. , Glass J. , Wu S. , Xiong X. , Reddick W. E. ( 2005 ). Brain volume in pediatric patients with sickle cell disease: Evidence of volumetric growth delay? American Journal of Neuroradiology , 26 , 455 – 462 . Google Scholar PubMed Steen R. G. , Hu X. J. , Elliott V. E. , Miles M. A. , Jones S. , Wang W. C. ( 2002 ). Kindergarten readiness skills in children with sickle cell disease: Evidence of early neurocognitive damage? Journal of Child Neurology , 17 , 111 – 116 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Stevens C. , Bavelier D. ( 2012 ). The role of selective attention on academic foundations: A cognitive neuroscience perspective . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 2 , S30 – S48 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Stevens C. , Lauinger B. , Neville H. ( 2009 ). Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: An event‐related brain potential study . Developmental Science , 12 , 634 – 646 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Stevens C. , Sanders L. , Neville H. ( 2006 ). Neurophysiological evidence for selective auditory attention deficits in children with specific language impairment . Brain Research , 1111 , 143 – 152 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Strait D. L. , Slater J. , Abecassis V. , Kraus N. ( 2014 ). Cortical response variability as a developmental index of selective auditory attention . Developmental Science , 17 , 175 – 186 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Tarazi R. A. , Grant M. L. , Ely E. , Barakat L. P. ( 2007 ). Neuropsychological functioning in preschool-age children with sickle cell disease: The role of illness-related and psychosocial factors . Child Neuropsychology , 13 , 155 – 172 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Taylor M. J. , Baldeweg T. ( 2002 ). Application of EEG, ERP and intracranial recordings to the investigation of cognitive functions in children . Developmental Science , 5 , 318 – 334 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Thompson R. J. , Gustafson K. E. , Bonner M. J. , Ware R. E. ( 2002 ). Neurocognitive development of young children with sickle cell disease through three years of age . Journal of Pediatric Psychology , 27 , 235 – 244 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Van Beijsterveldt C. , Van Baal G. ( 2002 ). Twin and family studies of the human electroencephalogram: A review and a meta-analysis . Biological Psychology , 61 , 111 – 138 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed van der Land V. , Hijmans C. T. , de Ruiter M. , Mutsaerts H. J. , Cnossen M. H. , Engelen M. , Majoie C. B. , Nederveen A. J. , Grootenhuis M. A. , Fijnvandraat K. ( 2015 ). Volume of white matter hyperintensities is an independent predictor of intelligence quotient and processing speed in children with sickle cell disease . British Journal of Haematology , 168 , 553 – 556 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Vichinsky E. P. , Neumayr L. D. , Gold J. I. , Weiner M. W. , Rule R. R. , Truran D. , Kasten J. , Eggleston B. , Kesler K. , McMahon L. , Orringer E. P. , Harrington T. , Kalinyak K. , De Castro L. M. , Kutlar A. , Rutherford C. J. , Johnson C. , Bessman J. D. , Jordan L. B. , Armstrong F. D. ; Neuropsychological Dysfunction and Neuroimaging Adult Sickle Cell Anemia Study Group . ( 2010 ). Neuropsychological dysfunction and neuroimaging abnormalities in neurologically intact adults with sickle cell anemia . JAMA , 303 , 1823 – 1831 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Wang W. , Enos L. , Gallagher D. , Thompson R. , Guarini L. , Vichinsky E. , Wright E. , Zimmerman R. , Armstrong F. D ; Cooperative Study of Sickle Cell Disease . ( 2001 ). Neuropsychologic performance in school-aged children with sickle cell disease: A report from the Cooperative Study of Sickle Cell Disease . The Journal of Pediatrics , 139 , 391 – 397 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Watkins K. , Hewes D. , Connelly A. , Kendall B. , Kingsley D. , Evans J. , Gadian D. G. , Vargha-Khadem F. , Kirkham F. ( 1998 ). Cognitive deficits associated with frontal‐lobe infarction in children with sickle cell disease . Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology , 40 , 536 – 543 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Wechsler D. ( 2002 ). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligenceTM: Third edition (WPPSITM-III) : Sydney, NSW : Pearson . Wolfe C. D. , Bell M. A. ( 2007 ). The integration of cognition and emotion during infancy and early childhood: Regulatory processes associated with the development of working memory . Brain and Cognition , 65 , 3 – 13 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Yurgelun-Todd D. ( 2007 ). Emotional and cognitive changes during adolescence . Current Opinion in Neurobiology , 17 , 251 – 257 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Zelazo P. D. , Anderson J. E. , Richler J. , Wallner‐Allen K. , Beaumont J. L. , Weintraub S. ( 2013 ). II. NIH toolbox cognition battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention . Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 78 , 16 – 33 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed © The Author 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society of Pediatric Psychology. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected] This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/about_us/legal/notices) http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png Journal of Pediatric Psychology Oxford University Press

Altered Neurophysiological Processing of Auditory Attention in Preschool Children With Sickle Cell Disease

Journal of Pediatric Psychology , Volume 43 (8) – Sep 1, 2018

Loading next page...
 
/lp/ou_press/altered-neurophysiological-processing-of-auditory-attention-in-W6d2b6E5ES

References (74)

Publisher
Oxford University Press
Copyright
© The Author 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society of Pediatric Psychology. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected]
ISSN
0146-8693
eISSN
1465-735X
DOI
10.1093/jpepsy/jsx115
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

Abstract Objective Sickle cell disease (SCD) is a genetic red blood cell disorder that often leads to stroke and executive dysfunction in school-age children and adults. This study aimed to characterize the development of the neural correlates of selective attention, an early component of executive function, in preschool children with SCD. Methods Auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded while children attended to a story stream in one ear and ignored a second story in the other ear interchangeably. In total, 12 patients (mean age = 5.5, 7 males) and 22 typically developing children (mean age = 4.4, 10 males) were included in the final analyses. Results By 100 ms, more positive ERP amplitudes were observed for attended relative to unattended stimuli in typically developing children but not those with SCD, suggesting deficits in the ability to focus attention. Reduced attention effects were associated with lower performance intellectual quotient Conclusion There are deficits in early attention modulation in young children with SCD. academic functioning, attention, developmental disabilities, neuropsychology, sickle cell disease Sickle cell disease (SCD) is a blood disorder that can result in a global pattern of diffuse brain injury, thought in part to be secondary to chronic anemia and hypoxia (Baldeweg et al., 2006; Steen et al., 2005). Clinical stroke, a focal neurological event lasting >24 hr, and silent stroke, which results in small lesions that can only be observed through structural magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), are commonly reported in patients with SCD, but volumetric differences can also be observed in quantitative imaging studies even when structural MRI appears normal (Jordan & DeBaun, 2017; van der Land et al., 2015). The frontal lobes have a protracted period of development and the frontoparietal regions, supplied by the internal carotid artery and the middle and anterior cerebral arteries, are most commonly affected by stroke in SCD (Ohene-Frempong et al., 1998). There is also evidence for abnormal cerebral blood flow, despite normal MRI (Prohovnik, Hurlet-Jensen, Adams, De Vivo, & Pavlakis, 2009). As a result, the frontal lobe and its connections are thought to be the most susceptible areas to SCD-related pathology. The frontal cortex plays a prominent role in the brain network underlying executive skills, the cognitive domain most affected in children with SCD with or without visible tissue injury (Hogan, Telfer, Kirkham, & de Haan, 2013; Schatz, Finke, Kellett, & Kramer, 2002; Wang et al., 2001; Watkins et al., 1998). Neurocognitive deficits appear early in development impacting intellectual quotient (IQ) and school readiness; however, the developmental trajectory of executive skills in children with SCD compared with their peers remains unclear (Hogan et al., 2013; Noll et al., 2001; Steen et al., 2002; Tarazi, Grant, Ely, & Barakat, 2007; Thompson, Gustafson, Bonner, & Ware, 2002). Selective attention is the ability to enhance the processing of relevant stimuli while suppressing the processing of irrelevant or distracting stimuli (Desimone & Duncan, 1995; Hillyard, Hink, Schwent, & Picton, 1973). The ability to selectively attend to target stimuli and ignore irrelevant environmental information is present from early in life, but the speed and efficiency of these processes increase with age (Ridderinkhof & van der Stelt, 2000). Selective attention and the neural systems that underlie this process undergo significant development in the preschool years (Rueda, Posner, & Rothbart, 2004; Rueda, Rothbart, McCandliss, Saccomanno, & Posner, 2005). As well as laying down a foundation for the development of later emerging executive skills, selective attention also plays a role in other cognitive domains such as language, writing, and mathematics (Engle & Kane, 2004; Stevens & Bavelier, 2012), and is an important predictor of school readiness and academic achievement (Duncan et al., 2007; Rueda, Checa, & Rothbart, 2010). Few studies have attempted to study selective attention skills in detail in SCD. A recent study found specific selective attention deficits on the Test of Everyday Attention in a sample of adults with SCD as compared with matched controls, despite no evidence of MRI pathology (Vichinsky et al., 2010). Many developmental models of executive function depict it as a process of simpler skills, such as selective attention, emerging first which are then bootstrapped into more complex executive skills (Anderson, 2002). Understanding when deficits emerge may be a first step to preventing potential snowballing effects of early skill deficits impacting the emergence of more complex skills. The use of event-related potential (ERP) and electroencephalography (EEG) techniques offers a window into potential differences in the brain functions underlying executive skills such as selective attention. EEG is the measurement of continuous electrical activity in the brain, while the ERP is a voltage change that is time-locked to a specific stimulus. An ERP is acquired by averaging multiple trials, so that the EEG signal of interest can be isolated from other brain activity (Taylor & Baldeweg, 2002). ERP components are averaged waveforms that have been attributed to a certain cognitive or sensory process (Downes, Bathelt, & de Haan, 2017). ERPs have high temporal resolution, are not prone to examiner bias, and offer the opportunity to conduct robust investigations of executive functions using a multi-level methodology (Astle & Scerif, 2009). Hillyard and colleagues first identified the ERP index of selective auditory attention in adults using a dichotic listening task where the adults were instructed to pay attention to a series of tones presented in one ear and to ignore tones in the other ear (Hillyard et al., 1973). In response to these tones, adults typically show an early positivity (P1) followed by a negative component (N1) at about 100 ms and a second positivity (P2) at ∼200 ms (Nager, Estorf, & Münte, 2006). Selective attention is observed in adults as a larger N1 for the attended stimuli relative to the unattended stimuli. Instead of the P1-N1-P2 response observed in adults, children tend to show an elongated broad positivity that starts at ∼100 ms in the same conditions (Sharma, Kraus, McGee, & Nicol, 1997). ERP research using dichotic listening paradigms has shown that children as young as 3 years can selectively attend to one auditory source while ignoring another (Sanders et al., 2006; Sanders & Zobel, 2012). In response to probe stimuli, preschool children from 3-year-olds have been found to produce a broad positivity that extends from 100 to ∼300 ms, while 6–8-year-olds show a shorter positivity from 100 to 200 ms (Sanders et al., 2006). This positivity is observed to have larger amplitudes in response to attended stimuli. The extended effect in younger children could reflect a prolonged influence of attention on neural processing and variability between and within children (Stevens & Bavelier, 2012). In a recent study, the transition from the neurophysiological response observed in young children to the adult P1-N1-P2 response was investigated and it was found that the P1-N1-P2 complex emerges in early adolescence (Karns, Isbell, Giuliano, & Neville, 2015). Initial evidence of the earlier P1 can be observed from 10 years of age but is not present in younger children (Karns et al., 2015). The P1-N1-P2 complex has a protracted developmental course and changes in functional development likely reflect the slow development of the frontoparietal network (Ponton et al., 2000; Yurgelun-Todd, 2007). The aim of this study is to explore potential neurophysiological differences in selective auditory attention in preschool children with SCD using an ecologically valid dichotic listening paradigm that was developed based on previous studies with preschool children (Isbell, Stevens, Hampton Wray, Bell, & Neville, 2016; Sanders et al., 2006). It is hypothesized that children with SCD will show less pronounced amplitudes as compared with the typically developing children in the attended condition and thereby demonstrate less of an attention effect. The attention effect, or the mean amplitude difference between the attended and the unattended condition, has been previously explored in this age range as an index of attention control (Isbell, Hampton Wray, & Neville, 2016). Given that the ERP response elicited in this task is an internal and measurable aspect of cognitive control (Van Beijsterveldt & Van Baal, 2002), a secondary aim is to look at associations between neural modulation and performance on behavioral tasks of nonverbal IQ and executive attention. It is hypothesized that the children with the poorest executive scores on the neuropsychological assessments will show the smallest attention effect in the ERP response. Methods Participants Patients were informed of the study by their consultant hematologist at Barts NHS Trust during their routine clinical visit if they met the inclusionary criteria. Inclusionary criteria for the patient group and the control group included being aged between 36 and 72 months, fluent in English, and no history of stroke, brain MRI investigations, known neurological issues, other disorders, or preterm delivery. A diagnosis of SCD based on genotype was required. The participation rate for patients was ∼85%. Reasons for nonparticipation included unavailability or a lack of response after referral. Three families cancelled the scheduled session. Control children were recruited through the same clinics as the patients and through local advertisement. Twenty-four patients (23 HbSS genotype and 1 HbSC genotype; full-scale IQ [FSIQ] = 98.8) and 38 typically developing control children (FSIQ = 108.7) were recruited. All children spoke English as their first language. Mean averaged maximum velocity in the middle cerebral artery for patients at the most recent clinical visit was 155.3 (21.3) cm/s; no patients had abnormal transcranial Doppler recordings (velocity >200 cm/s). Five patients were currently on blood transfusion, four patients were being treated with hydroxyurea, and no patients reported experiencing current pain. Thirty-four EEG data sets were available for analysis after preprocessing (12 patients [1 HbSC] and 22 comparison children; Table I). Two patients and five controls were excluded because of EEG system error. Additionally, two patients and five controls were excluded because of removal of sensor net and/or headphones during the session. One control child had thick braids that prevented successful application of the apparatus. It was disclosed during assessment that a control child was born extremely preterm and so was excluded from analysis. Additionally, one patient was diagnosed with specific language impairment (SLI) between recruitment and assessment, and one patient did not pass the training phase because of poor comprehension of instruction. Six patients and four controls were removed for not meeting the minimum criteria of 25 stimuli per condition after preprocessing. Final groups, including 12 patients and 22 control children, were matched for age, full-scale IQ, socioeconomic status, and gender (Table I), although only 36% of the control group was matched for ethnicity (Black British). Table I. Group Descriptives Variable Patient group (n = 12) Control group (n = 22) p-value Age (mean; SD) 5.0 (0.96) 4.4 (0.92) .1 FSIQ (M; SD) 101.2 (10.5) 109.0 (14.3) .1 VIQ 101.1 (10.9) 112.4 (11.5) .01 PIQ 99.3 (12.3) 105.6 (15.5) .2 NIHTB attentional control 94.7 (24.4) 102.3 (17.5) (n = 17) .3 DDTP omissions 23.4 (19.6) (n = 10) 23.4 (15.7) (n = 13) .9 DDTP commissions 72.8 (121.6) (n = 10) 12.03 (21.7) (n = 13) .1 Male 7 10 .4 Ethnicity Black British 12 8 White British 10 Asian British/Other 4 Socioeconomic status (by income) .7 Lowest 3 4 Middle 6 10 Highest 3 6 No information 0 2 Maternal education .5 Secondary level 5 7 Third level 6 10 No information 1 5 Variable Patient group (n = 12) Control group (n = 22) p-value Age (mean; SD) 5.0 (0.96) 4.4 (0.92) .1 FSIQ (M; SD) 101.2 (10.5) 109.0 (14.3) .1 VIQ 101.1 (10.9) 112.4 (11.5) .01 PIQ 99.3 (12.3) 105.6 (15.5) .2 NIHTB attentional control 94.7 (24.4) 102.3 (17.5) (n = 17) .3 DDTP omissions 23.4 (19.6) (n = 10) 23.4 (15.7) (n = 13) .9 DDTP commissions 72.8 (121.6) (n = 10) 12.03 (21.7) (n = 13) .1 Male 7 10 .4 Ethnicity Black British 12 8 White British 10 Asian British/Other 4 Socioeconomic status (by income) .7 Lowest 3 4 Middle 6 10 Highest 3 6 No information 0 2 Maternal education .5 Secondary level 5 7 Third level 6 10 No information 1 5 Note. Pearsons’s chi-square used for category comparison and independent t-tests for comparison of continuous variables. DDTP = Doggie Deletion Task for Preschoolers; FSIQ = full-scale IQ; NIHTB = NIH toolbox; PIQ= performance IQ; VIQ = verbal IQ. Some of the participants did not complete the paper-based DDTP task because of task modification in the pilot phase (seven control children), timing issues (two patients/one control child), and experimenter error (one control child). Five of the control children did not complete the NIHTB task because of equipment issues. Table I. Group Descriptives Variable Patient group (n = 12) Control group (n = 22) p-value Age (mean; SD) 5.0 (0.96) 4.4 (0.92) .1 FSIQ (M; SD) 101.2 (10.5) 109.0 (14.3) .1 VIQ 101.1 (10.9) 112.4 (11.5) .01 PIQ 99.3 (12.3) 105.6 (15.5) .2 NIHTB attentional control 94.7 (24.4) 102.3 (17.5) (n = 17) .3 DDTP omissions 23.4 (19.6) (n = 10) 23.4 (15.7) (n = 13) .9 DDTP commissions 72.8 (121.6) (n = 10) 12.03 (21.7) (n = 13) .1 Male 7 10 .4 Ethnicity Black British 12 8 White British 10 Asian British/Other 4 Socioeconomic status (by income) .7 Lowest 3 4 Middle 6 10 Highest 3 6 No information 0 2 Maternal education .5 Secondary level 5 7 Third level 6 10 No information 1 5 Variable Patient group (n = 12) Control group (n = 22) p-value Age (mean; SD) 5.0 (0.96) 4.4 (0.92) .1 FSIQ (M; SD) 101.2 (10.5) 109.0 (14.3) .1 VIQ 101.1 (10.9) 112.4 (11.5) .01 PIQ 99.3 (12.3) 105.6 (15.5) .2 NIHTB attentional control 94.7 (24.4) 102.3 (17.5) (n = 17) .3 DDTP omissions 23.4 (19.6) (n = 10) 23.4 (15.7) (n = 13) .9 DDTP commissions 72.8 (121.6) (n = 10) 12.03 (21.7) (n = 13) .1 Male 7 10 .4 Ethnicity Black British 12 8 White British 10 Asian British/Other 4 Socioeconomic status (by income) .7 Lowest 3 4 Middle 6 10 Highest 3 6 No information 0 2 Maternal education .5 Secondary level 5 7 Third level 6 10 No information 1 5 Note. Pearsons’s chi-square used for category comparison and independent t-tests for comparison of continuous variables. DDTP = Doggie Deletion Task for Preschoolers; FSIQ = full-scale IQ; NIHTB = NIH toolbox; PIQ= performance IQ; VIQ = verbal IQ. Some of the participants did not complete the paper-based DDTP task because of task modification in the pilot phase (seven control children), timing issues (two patients/one control child), and experimenter error (one control child). Five of the control children did not complete the NIHTB task because of equipment issues. Selective Auditory Attention Task The ERP paradigm and processing pipeline were piloted with adults and two preschool-age children. Several measures were taken to make this task more age-appropriate including the development of training and practice phases. Custom static low-detail visual cues for on-screen presentation were developed to match the story to be attended. Arrows were created for on-screen presentation to reduce memory load. Fifteen short stories from Aesop’s classical fables were adapted to be age-appropriate. All stories were recorded in a male and a female voice, so that the child was always attending to a male and a female voice concurrently, helping them to differentiate between the two auditory streams. Sound recordings were edited using Audacity software to remove gaps >100 ms and to make the recordings comparable for loudness. Story narrators were instructed to keep their tone and pitch at a consistent level. Auditory and visual stimuli were presented through age-appropriate adjustable headphones and on a Dell Optiplex (Dell Inc., TX) computer screen running Windows XP using a script programmed via Matlab 2012 R2012b (The MathWorks, MA) and Psychtoolbox V3 (Kleiner et al., 2007). The child watched a cartoon while the net was positioned. This was followed by a training session. The child was instructed to touch the ear that corresponded with the side that the arrow on the screen was pointing toward. After the training phase, the child undertook a practice session before EEG recording began. Participants were cued to selectively attend to one of two simultaneously presented stories that differed in location (left/right), voice (male/female), and content. The stimuli of interest, pure tone bursts of white noise with a length of 500 ms, were randomly inserted into the attended and unattended streams at the same loudness level as the stories. At the beginning of each story the child heard “Are you ready?” and the researcher pressed a key to proceed. After each story, the child was asked questions relating to the attended story. The offset latency for the pure tone bursts of white noise was measured as 23 ms using the Electrical Geodesics, Inc. latency-testing device. There was a maximum of 14 story trials that were each up to 1 min in duration. The interstimulus interval was 1.5 s. If the child expressed a request to end the testing session, the researcher asked if they would like to attempt one more story trial and terminated the session if the child did not agree. Table II shows the average number of story trials completed for each group. Table II. Group Comparison on Behavioral Task Performance and Number of Usable Events Variable Patient group Control group p-value Total number of story trials completed M (SD) 10.7 (3.7) 10.8 (3.2) .92 Percentage of correct/attended story trials M (SD) 82.5 (18) 89.7 (17) .27 Number of events before processing M (SD) 107.5 (35) 123.4 (38) .17 Number of events after processing M (SD; N) 81 (35; 12) 86 (23; 22) .63 Variable Patient group Control group p-value Total number of story trials completed M (SD) 10.7 (3.7) 10.8 (3.2) .92 Percentage of correct/attended story trials M (SD) 82.5 (18) 89.7 (17) .27 Number of events before processing M (SD) 107.5 (35) 123.4 (38) .17 Number of events after processing M (SD; N) 81 (35; 12) 86 (23; 22) .63 Table II. Group Comparison on Behavioral Task Performance and Number of Usable Events Variable Patient group Control group p-value Total number of story trials completed M (SD) 10.7 (3.7) 10.8 (3.2) .92 Percentage of correct/attended story trials M (SD) 82.5 (18) 89.7 (17) .27 Number of events before processing M (SD) 107.5 (35) 123.4 (38) .17 Number of events after processing M (SD; N) 81 (35; 12) 86 (23; 22) .63 Variable Patient group Control group p-value Total number of story trials completed M (SD) 10.7 (3.7) 10.8 (3.2) .92 Percentage of correct/attended story trials M (SD) 82.5 (18) 89.7 (17) .27 Number of events before processing M (SD) 107.5 (35) 123.4 (38) .17 Number of events after processing M (SD; N) 81 (35; 12) 86 (23; 22) .63 ERP Recording and Analysis EEG data were obtained and recorded using NetStation V4.1.2 (Electrical Geodesics Inc., OR) on Mac OS 10.3.9 software. A NetAmps 200 amplifier and HydroCel Geodesic EEG were recorded using the Electrical Geodesics sensor net system from 128 electrodes and digitized at 250 Hz with a bandwidth of 0.1–100 Hz. A ground electrode was in place and the vertex electrode (Cz) was used as an online reference. Channel impedances were adjusted where necessary and appropriate to levels <50 kΩ. An electroculogram was recorded for the detection of eye-related artifacts. Electrodes were positioned above and below and to the side of both eyes. Offline, data were filtered with finite impulse response filters at a high-pass frequency of 0.1 Hz and a low-pass frequency of 30 Hz in EEGLAB 11.0.3 (Delorme et al., 2011). The EEG signal was epoched at 200 ms before the stimulus event (the pure tone bursts of white noise) to 600 ms after stimulus presentation. Automatic epoch rejection of bad epochs occurred at a threshold of plus or minus 100 microvolts. The average voltage of the 200 ms segment before stimulus onset was set as the baseline. Visual inspection was used to remove artifacts such as eye blinks, saccades, muscle activity, and skin potentials (Luck, 2005). The data were rereferenced from the vertex reference to an average montage. The time window of interest (100–300 ms) was chosen based on a review of the relevant literature for this age (Coch et al., 2005; Karns et al., 2015; Sanders, Stevens, Coch, & Neville, 2006; Sanders & Zobel, 2012; Stevens, Sanders, & Neville, 2006; Stevens et al., 2009). The mean peak amplitude for this time window was investigated. Trials were averaged together to acquire a single averaged segment for the ignore and attend condition for each participant. A criterion of a minimum of 25 artifact-free trials in each condition after all preprocessing steps was imposed. This figure is within the range of minimum number of trials required in similar ERP studies with young children (Coch, Sanders, & Neville, 2005; de Haan, Pascalis, & Johnson, 2002; Sanders et al., 2006). It has been recommended that studies with young children should include at least 10–20 trials per condition to obtain a reliable estimation of the ERP component under investigation (Cuevas, Cannon, Yoo, & Fox, 2014; DeBoer, Scott, Nelson, & de Haan, 2007). Each child completed up to 14 story sessions, so participants had up to 160 events (12–15 per story session) before processing. Children were reminded to fixate on the screen and to sit still between each story to maximize the number of artifact-free trials and were offered a teddy bear to hold if they struggled to stay still. To further improve signal-to-noise ratio, several channels were combined for channel-level analyses (Coch et al., 2005; Sanders & Zobel, 2012; Strait, Slater, Abecassis, & Kraus, 2014). Four electrode groupings (Figure 1) are defined based on previous studies with similar age groups and paradigms and a topographical investigation of the current population (Coch et al., 2005; Isbell, Stevens et al., 2016; Sanders et al., 2006). Figure 1. View largeDownload slide Electrode sites. The Geodesic Sensor Net 128 channel layout in accordance to the 10–20 system of electrode placement. Data were averaged across four channels at each site to increase the signal-to-noise ratio. The four channel cluster sites are located over the medial frontal, left frontal, right frontal, and frontocentral sites and are illustrated through the shaded dots. Figure 1. View largeDownload slide Electrode sites. The Geodesic Sensor Net 128 channel layout in accordance to the 10–20 system of electrode placement. Data were averaged across four channels at each site to increase the signal-to-noise ratio. The four channel cluster sites are located over the medial frontal, left frontal, right frontal, and frontocentral sites and are illustrated through the shaded dots. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence The Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (WPPSI-III-UK) (population mean = 100, SD = 15) was administered to obtain IQ (Wechsler, 2002). The IQ scores for the final groups were slightly higher than the original group means for both groups, although executive scores were similar in the final group (Table I) to that observed for the original groups. Children were required to have a verbal IQ >75 to proceed to the ERP task. This threshold was implemented to ensure that children could comprehend task instruction. The chosen cutoff was based on language cutoffs used in similar studies that used ERP paradigms to investigate attention in preschool-age children (Stevens, Lauinger, & Neville, 2009). NIH Toolbox Test of Attention Control The standardized NIH toolbox (NIHTB) test of attention control (population mean = 100, SD = 15) was administered. Poorer scores in the NIHTB task reflect longer reaction time and incorrect responses. The NIHTB task has been validated from 3 years of age (Zelazo et al., 2013). Five control participants had missing NIHTB data because of technical difficulties during data collection. Doggie Deletion Task for Preschoolers The Doggie Deletion Task for Preschoolers (DDTP), a revised cancellation task, was also used to measure attention control (Byrne, Bawden, DeWolfe, & Beattie, 1998). More omissions and commissions on the DDTP reflect poorer attention control. The DDTP task was previously developed with typically developing children at the London Babylab (Downes et al., 2016). Procedure Ethical approval was obtained from the local NHS committee (Ref: 13/LO/0962), and site-specific approval was obtained from UCL Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health and Barts NHS Trust. Written informed consent was obtained from each parent and verbal assent was obtained from each child. The testing session took place in the London Babylab, UCL Great Ormond Street Institute of Child Health. All consent procedures and data collection were conducted in English by the same researcher (M.D.). All children first completed the WPPSI-III-UK, followed by the DDTP and the NIHTB test of attention control, in that order. After a scheduled break, the child then completed the EEG session, which lasted ∼30 min. Results ERP Behavioral Results There were no group differences between the total number of completed story trials, number of correctly attended story trials, number of events before preprocessing, or final number of events (Table II). Group Differences As expected, the control group showed a broad positivity peaking at ∼150–200 ms poststimulus onset. However, this was not observed for the patient group who showed a less pronounced and inconsistent amplitude (Figure 2). Figure 2. View largeDownload slide ERP waveforms for both groups. Grand average ERP plots. Plots show attended (broken lines) and unattended (solid lines) waveforms for the SCD group over the right frontal (A), left frontal (B), centrofrontal (C), and frontomedial (D) sites and for the typically developing children over the right frontal (E), left frontal (F), centrofrontal (G), and frontomedial (H) sites, showing differences in the area of interest (shaded gray area). Note. ERP = event-related potential; SCD = sickle cell disease. Figure 2. View largeDownload slide ERP waveforms for both groups. Grand average ERP plots. Plots show attended (broken lines) and unattended (solid lines) waveforms for the SCD group over the right frontal (A), left frontal (B), centrofrontal (C), and frontomedial (D) sites and for the typically developing children over the right frontal (E), left frontal (F), centrofrontal (G), and frontomedial (H) sites, showing differences in the area of interest (shaded gray area). Note. ERP = event-related potential; SCD = sickle cell disease. Overall group differences for attended and unattended conditions were further explored across the four frontal clusters of interest using a multivariate analysis of variance (ANOVA). Consistent with previous studies, the control group showed the attention effect (a larger response to the attended signal) to be greatest at the frontomedial site in comparison with the other three sites of interest; however, this did not reach significance (F(2.2, 46.19) = 2.164, p = .10). In contrast to the control group, the patients did not show as strong an effect for the attended condition at any of the sites (see Figure 3). The control group showed a significant difference in the mean peak between the attended and unattended condition (t = 2.2, p = .04), but this was not observed for the patients (t = .60, p = .56). The frontomedial electrode grouping was chosen from the four clusters of interest for further analysis. Mean amplitudes were analyzed using a two-way univariate ANOVA with group (patient vs. control) and condition (attended vs. unattended) as the between-subject factors and the mean amplitudes at the frontomedial site as the within-subject factor. The two mechanisms underlying attention modulation in this task, enhancement of the amplitude in the attended condition and suppression of the amplitude in the unattended condition, were analyzed. Significant group differences were observed between groups for amplitude in the attended condition at the frontomedial site with the control group showing larger mean amplitudes (t = 2.2, p = .03), but no group differences were observed for the unattended condition (t = −.12, p = .91). Table III shows that, although there was greater mean amplitudes for the control group in the attended condition on the other sites, this did not reach significance (F(4, 29) = 1.72, p = .16), and there was no trend observed at any site for the unattended condition. Table III. Mean Amplitude of the Early Frontal Positivity (100–300 ms) in the Medial Frontal, Left Frontal, Right Frontal, and Central Sites for Both Conditions Group Condition Medial frontal Left frontal Right frontal Frontocentral M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) Patient group Attended −0.229 (0.57) 0.381 (0.47) −0.071 (0.59) 0.386 (0.41) Unattended 0.249 (.49) 0.462 (0.43) 0.282 (0.38) 0.649 (0.43) Control group Attended 1.637 (0.58) 1.389 (0.48) 0.662 (0.29) 1.234 (0.49) Unattended 0.167 (.46) 0.369 (0.57) 0.282 (0.37) 0.555 (0.41) Group Condition Medial frontal Left frontal Right frontal Frontocentral M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) Patient group Attended −0.229 (0.57) 0.381 (0.47) −0.071 (0.59) 0.386 (0.41) Unattended 0.249 (.49) 0.462 (0.43) 0.282 (0.38) 0.649 (0.43) Control group Attended 1.637 (0.58) 1.389 (0.48) 0.662 (0.29) 1.234 (0.49) Unattended 0.167 (.46) 0.369 (0.57) 0.282 (0.37) 0.555 (0.41) Note. Significant group differences (p < .05) are shown in bold, and trends for group differences (p < .1) are shown in italics. Table III. Mean Amplitude of the Early Frontal Positivity (100–300 ms) in the Medial Frontal, Left Frontal, Right Frontal, and Central Sites for Both Conditions Group Condition Medial frontal Left frontal Right frontal Frontocentral M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) Patient group Attended −0.229 (0.57) 0.381 (0.47) −0.071 (0.59) 0.386 (0.41) Unattended 0.249 (.49) 0.462 (0.43) 0.282 (0.38) 0.649 (0.43) Control group Attended 1.637 (0.58) 1.389 (0.48) 0.662 (0.29) 1.234 (0.49) Unattended 0.167 (.46) 0.369 (0.57) 0.282 (0.37) 0.555 (0.41) Group Condition Medial frontal Left frontal Right frontal Frontocentral M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) M (SE) Patient group Attended −0.229 (0.57) 0.381 (0.47) −0.071 (0.59) 0.386 (0.41) Unattended 0.249 (.49) 0.462 (0.43) 0.282 (0.38) 0.649 (0.43) Control group Attended 1.637 (0.58) 1.389 (0.48) 0.662 (0.29) 1.234 (0.49) Unattended 0.167 (.46) 0.369 (0.57) 0.282 (0.37) 0.555 (0.41) Note. Significant group differences (p < .05) are shown in bold, and trends for group differences (p < .1) are shown in italics. Figure 3. View largeDownload slide Topographical maps for both groups. Topographic two-dimensional voltage maps show scalp-potential distributions averaged over a 200 ms time-window. Figures indicate magnitude and ranges of ERPs elicited for (A) attended condition, (B) ignored condition and (C) the attention effect (difference wave = attended–unattended) for SCD patients and (D) attended condition, (E) ignored condition, and (F) attention effect for typically developing control children at 100–300 ms. Maxima corresponding to frontal effects are evident for the typically developing children in the attended condition (D) but are not as perceptible for the control children (A). A larger attention effect or difference wave in the frontal region can be observed for the controls (F) than for the patients (C). Note. ERP = event-related potential; SCD = sickle cell disease. Figure 3. View largeDownload slide Topographical maps for both groups. Topographic two-dimensional voltage maps show scalp-potential distributions averaged over a 200 ms time-window. Figures indicate magnitude and ranges of ERPs elicited for (A) attended condition, (B) ignored condition and (C) the attention effect (difference wave = attended–unattended) for SCD patients and (D) attended condition, (E) ignored condition, and (F) attention effect for typically developing control children at 100–300 ms. Maxima corresponding to frontal effects are evident for the typically developing children in the attended condition (D) but are not as perceptible for the control children (A). A larger attention effect or difference wave in the frontal region can be observed for the controls (F) than for the patients (C). Note. ERP = event-related potential; SCD = sickle cell disease. The Attention Effect and Removal of Outliers Near-significant group differences were observed for the magnitude of the attention effect (the difference between the attended and unattended conditions) at the frontomedial site (t = 1.8, p = .07). Group analyses were repeated to ensure that the three outliers in the negative range for the attention effect in the control group (all 3-year-olds suggesting more variability in the youngest children) and the main outlier in the patient group were not having effect on group differences. The group difference for the attention effect (attended–unattended) reached significance (p = .006) after removal of outliers. The post hoc group difference observed for the attended condition also increased in significance (p = .013), although the lack of a group difference observed for the unattended condition remained the same. Associations With Cognitive Measures There was no relation observed between performance IQ (PIQ) and the attention effect using Pearson’s correlations, although a greater positivity in the attended condition was significantly associated with PIQ (r = .483, p = .004; Figure 4A). When the groups were separated, this association remained significant for the patients (r = .619, p = .040) but became a near-significant trend for the control children (r = .415, p = .055). Associations with omissions and commissions on the DDTP were explored using Spearman’s correlations (as scores were not normally distributed), and it was found that children with larger peaks in the unattended condition made more omissions across both groups (rho = .465, p = .02, n = 23; Figure 4B) and for the control group (rho = .583, p = .036) and patient group separately (rho = .717, p = .045). No associations with commissions were observed. The significance of the DDTP findings did not change when outliers were removed. No significant correlations emerged for the NIH attention control task (Table IV). Table IV. Associations Between the Behavioral Variables and the Neurophysiological Correlates of Attention Control Measure Patient group Control group Total Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition DDTP omissions* .150 .517 .717* .149 .501 .583* .128 .449 .465* DDTP commissions* .192 −.268 −.603 .186 .081 −.025 −.047 −.258 −.235 NIH inhibitory control .242 .485 .174 .023 −.309 −.364 .165 .097 −.144 Measure Patient group Control group Total Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition DDTP omissions* .150 .517 .717* .149 .501 .583* .128 .449 .465* DDTP commissions* .192 −.268 −.603 .186 .081 −.025 −.047 −.258 −.235 NIH inhibitory control .242 .485 .174 .023 −.309 −.364 .165 .097 −.144 * Spearman’s correlations used because of nonnormal distribution of the DDTP variables; *p < .05. Table IV. Associations Between the Behavioral Variables and the Neurophysiological Correlates of Attention Control Measure Patient group Control group Total Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition DDTP omissions* .150 .517 .717* .149 .501 .583* .128 .449 .465* DDTP commissions* .192 −.268 −.603 .186 .081 −.025 −.047 −.258 −.235 NIH inhibitory control .242 .485 .174 .023 −.309 −.364 .165 .097 −.144 Measure Patient group Control group Total Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition Attention effect (attend–unattend) Attended condition Unattended condition DDTP omissions* .150 .517 .717* .149 .501 .583* .128 .449 .465* DDTP commissions* .192 −.268 −.603 .186 .081 −.025 −.047 −.258 −.235 NIH inhibitory control .242 .485 .174 .023 −.309 −.364 .165 .097 −.144 * Spearman’s correlations used because of nonnormal distribution of the DDTP variables; *p < .05. Figure 4. View largeDownload slide ERP and behavior. The relations between ERP and cognitive tasks. (A) Association between the ERP for the attended condition and PIQ and (B) association between the ERP for the unattended condition and number of omissions. Note. ERP = event-related potential; PIQ = performance IQ. Figure 4. View largeDownload slide ERP and behavior. The relations between ERP and cognitive tasks. (A) Association between the ERP for the attended condition and PIQ and (B) association between the ERP for the unattended condition and number of omissions. Note. ERP = event-related potential; PIQ = performance IQ. Discussion Previous research with preschool children in special populations who have known executive deficits has found evidence for altered neural processing in auditory attention modulation (Stevens et al., 2006; 2009). Here, we extend this research to children with SCD. The main finding is that children with SCD show a less pronounced positive amplitude to stimuli in the attended stream and have a poorer “difference score” or attention effect. This study provides novel evidence for specific deficits in attention modulation in the neurophysiological response of young children with SCD. These results align with the behavioral findings of poorer executive function, particularly attention control, in older children with SCD (Daly, Kral, & Tarazi, 2011). The current findings are also in line with two previous ERP studies in SCD that found more diminished and variable ERP responses on tasks of executive skills (Colombatti et al., 2015; Hogan et al., 2006). Hogan and colleagues measured ERP components related to performance monitoring in children with SCD between 11- and 23-year-olds, with and without evidence of silent lesions, and found that children with SCD showed some evidence for executive deficits in comparison with typically developing controls, even in the absence of stroke, as demonstrated by a reduced error-related negativity amplitude, which reflects reduced unconscious processing of errors (Downes, Bathelt, & de Haan, 2017). They also administered a battery of behavioral executive tasks where both sickle groups showed significantly poorer scores across several domains, including selective attention. Colombatti and colleagues investigated the P3 using an auditory oddball paradigm in children with SCD between 6- and 15-year-olds who had no history of stroke. The P3, a positive ERP component that reflects information processing during tasks that involve attending to and discriminating between target stimuli and distracting stimuli, was found to be more protracted and variable in the children with SCD. In the present study, there was no difference between groups in the amount of trials completed or the amount of correctly attended stories, despite the difference in the underlying neural processes, highlighting the sensitivity of ERPs to processing differences. Stevens, Sanders, and Neville (2006) reported similar findings for their cohort of children with SLI, who also showed equivalent behavioral performance. Hogan et al. (2006) also reported no group differences in task performance on their performance-monitoring task in children with SCD, despite neurophysiological differences. The pattern for larger amplitudes in the attended condition for the control group, and their absence in the SCD patient group, suggests that the SCD group had difficulties with signal enhancement rather than distractor suppression in attention modulation (Stevens et al., 2009; Stevens, Sanders, & Neville, 2006). Although attenuated ERPs were anticipated for patients in the attended condition, the lack of any significant response was not expected. Previous research on auditory attention allocation in children with autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) has also found it to be absent, attenuated, or inconsistent in multiple studies, even when compared with typically developing children (Donkers et al., 2015; Gomes et al., 2012; Loiselle, Stamm, Maitinsky, & Whipple, 1980). The lack of a response in the attended condition may suggest that patients are allocating limited attention resources, are less automatic in the allocation of these resources, or alternatively, that there is a dampening of information, more limited activation, or immature neural synchronization (Gilley, Sharma, Dorman, & Martin, 2005). Thus, children with SCD may have less established neural sources of attentional modulation, and the lack of neurophysiological modulation reflects poorly attuned attentional control. In the current study, there was an association observed between nonverbal IQ and the ERP amplitude in the attended condition, but not for the attention effect. A relation between the attention effect and nonverbal IQ has been reported in preschool children from low SES backgrounds, while additional studies have shown a relationship between the ERP amplitude in the unattended condition and parent reports of executive function (Isbell, Stevens et al., 2016; Lackner et al., 2013). Associations with the ERP were only observed for one of the two attention control tasks in our battery, where children with larger peaks in the unattended condition made more omission errors. Missing data or differences in task demands could have contributed to the lack of relation with behavior performance on the NIHTB task. Behavioral tasks measure multiple steps of cognitive processing, while ERP measures have the advantage of breaking down these steps into a series of components, which makes it more difficult to draw associations between the two. The behavioral attention tasks were also limited as they were both visual; an auditory task may have been more informative for direct comparison. Limitations One limitation of this study is the lack of neuroimaging, which means that although patients with a known history of stroke were excluded, some children may have had undetected silent stroke. Another limitation of this study is the small population. Nevertheless, the current patient population size was larger than the only previous published ERP studies in SCD (Colombatti et al., 2015; Hogan et al., 2006). A further limitation of this study was the high attrition rate, which resulted in only 34 data sets in the final analysis, representing only 55% of the children recruited for the study. High attrition rates of up to 45% in EEG and ERP studies are typical for this age range and are related to refusal to wear the EEG cap and excessive movement during the session (Cuevas et al., 2012; Morasch & Bell, 2011; Wolfe & Bell, 2007). Finally, as observed in previous ERP attention studies with this young age range, there was variability within each group (Isbell, Stevens et al., 2016; Stevens, Sanders, & Neville, 2006). Five patients (42%) and three children from the control group (14%) had attention effects >1 SD below the control group mean. Noisy ERP data in younger participants could have precluded the detection of stronger relations with behavioral measures. Future Investigations An advantage of the current paradigm is that it can be applied across a wide age range allowing for the developmental tracking of neural markers of attention (Strait et al., 2014). Future studies should apply this paradigm in older school-age children and adolescents to ascertain whether the lack of response in the current study is a developmental delay that eventually catches up over time or whether it is an early indicator of an altered course of functional neural development. Additionally, there is currently no published evidence of classical attention ERP paradigms, such as the auditory oddball paradigm, in infants and young children with SCD. Future research applying this paradigm with young children will help elucidate whether the early attention deficit in the current study is also evident in more basic attention experiments, or unique to the complex stimuli in the current study that require the exertion of more executive control. It is important to determine the role of early attention deficits in the development of well-established later executive deficits in older children with SCD (Schatz & Roberts, 2007). Similar to Barkley’s developmental model of executive function for children with ADHD, where primary deficits in inhibition are proposed to have knock-on effects for later emerging executive domains, it may be that poor attention control in young children with SCD impairs the emergence of higher-order executive functions (Barkley, 1997). Recent research suggests that ERPs can be used to index improvements in cognitive interventions with preschool children, potentially indicating better recruitment of neural systems important for selective attention (Espinet, Anderson, & Zelazo, 2013; Isbell, Stevens et al., 2016; Neville et al., 2013; Rueda, Checa, & Cómbita, 2012). Future research should consider using ERP responses in dichotic listening tasks as an end point in treatment trials of young children with SCD. Conclusion Taken together, these findings contribute to the elucidation of differences in the development of the neural underpinnings of selective attention in preschool children with SCD. Children with SCD specifically show poorer signal enhancement with attention. Further research is warranted to investigate potential differences in source localization and to determine whether group differences would be evident on other attention paradigms, such as the oddball paradigm. Future applications of the current paradigm in older children with SCD are also required to determine whether this group difference can still be observed. Nevertheless, the current study provides initial evidence for an altered neurophysiological response to selective attention in children with SCD, pinpointing a lack of signal enhancement, and also contributes further evidence for the relationship between behavioral performance and neural markers of attention. Acknowledgments The authors would like to thank the families who participated in this study. Funding This research was funded by the Child Health Research Charitable Incorporated Organisation and supported by the National Institute for Health Research Biomedical Research Centre at Great Ormond Street Hospital for Children NHS Foundation Trust and University College London and the National Institute for Health Research: Research for Patient Benefit Funding stream (PB-PG-1112-29099). Conflicts of interest: None declared. References Anderson P. ( 2002 ). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood . Child Neuropsychology , 8 , 71 – 82 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Astle D. E. , Scerif G. ( 2009 ). Using developmental cognitive neuroscience to study behavioral and attentional control . Developmental Psychobiology , 51 , 107 – 118 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Baldeweg T. , Hogan A. M. , Saunders D. E. , Telfer P. , Gadian D. G. , Vargha-Khadem F. , Kirkham F. J. ( 2006 ). Detecting white matter injury in sickle cell disease using voxel-based morphometry . Annals of Neurology , 59 , 662 – 672 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Barkley R. A. ( 1997 ). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, self-regulation, and time: Toward a more comprehensive theory . Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics , 18 , 271 – 279 . Google Scholar PubMed Byrne J. M. , Bawden H. N. , DeWolfe N. A. , Beattie T. L. ( 1998 ). Clinical assessment of psychopharmacological treatment of preschoolers with ADHD . Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology , 20 , 613 – 627 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Coch D. , Sanders L. D. , Neville H. J. ( 2005 ). An event-related potential study of selective auditory attention in children and adults . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 17 , 605 – 622 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Colombatti R. , Ermani M. , Rampazzo P. , Manara R. , Montanaro M. , Basso G. , Meneghetti G. , Sainati L. ( 2015 ). Cognitive evoked potentials and neural networks are abnormal in children with sickle cell disease and not related to the degree of anaemia, pain and silent infarcts . British Journal of Haematology , 169 , 597 – 600 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Cuevas K. , Cannon E. N. , Yoo K. , Fox N. A. ( 2014 ). The infant EEG mu rhythm: Methodological considerations and best practices . Developmental Review , 34 , 26 – 43 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Cuevas K. , Hubble M. , Bell M. A. ( 2012 ). Early childhood predictors of post-kindergarten executive function: Behavior, parent report, and psychophysiology . Early Education and Development , 23 , 59 – 73 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Daly B. , Kral M. C. , Tarazi R. A. ( 2011 ). The role of neuropsychological evaluation in pediatric sickle cell disease . The Clinical Neuropsychologist , 25 , 903 – 925 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed DeBoer T. , Scott L. S. , Nelson C. A. , de Haan M. ( 2007 ). Methods for acquiring and analyzing infant eventrelated potentials . In M. de Haan (Ed.), Infant EEG and event-related potentials ( 5 – 37 ). East Sussex: Psychology Press. de Haan M. , Pascalis O. , Johnson M. ( 2002 ). Specialization of neural mechanisms underlying face recognition in human infants . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 14 , 199 – 209 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Delorme A. , Mullen T. , Kothe C. , Acar Z. A. , Bigdely-Shamlo N. , Vankov A. , Makeig S. ( 2011 ). EEGLAB, SIFT, NFT, BCILAB, and ERICA: New tools for advanced EEG processing . Computational Intelligence and Neuroscience , 2011 , 130714 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Desimone R. , Duncan J. ( 1995 ). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention . Annual Review of Neuroscience , 18 , 193 – 222 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Donkers F. C. , Schipul S. E. , Baranek G. T. , Cleary K. M. , Willoughby M. T. , Evans A. M. , Bulluck J. C. , Lovmo J. E. , Belger A. ( 2015 ). Attenuated auditory event-related potentials and associations with atypical sensory response patterns in children with autism . Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders , 45 , 506 – 523 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Downes M. ( 2016 ). The development and assessment of executive functioning in preschool children with and without sickle cell anaemia . Doctoral thesis, UCL (University College London). Downes M. , Bathelt J. , de Haan M. ( 2017 ). Event-related potential measures of executive functioning from preschool to adolescence . Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology , 59 , 581 – 590 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Duncan G. J. , Dowsett C. J. , Claessens A. , Magnuson K. , Huston A. C. , Klebanov P. , Pagani L. S. , Feinstein L. , Engel M. , Brooks-Gunn J. , Brooks-Gunn J. , Sexton H. , Duckworth K. , Japel C. ( 2007 ). School readiness and later achievement . Developmental Psychology , 43 , 1428 – 46 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Engle R. W. , Kane M. J. ( 2004 ). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control . Psychology of Learning and Motivation , 44 , 145 – 200 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Espinet S. D. , Anderson J. E. , Zelazo P. D. ( 2013 ). Reflection training improves executive function in preschool-age children: Behavioral and neural effects . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 4 , 3 – 15 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Gilley P. M. , Sharma A. , Dorman M. , Martin K. ( 2005 ). Developmental changes in refractoriness of the cortical auditory evoked potential . Clinical Neurophysiology , 116 , 648 – 657 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Gomes H. , Duff M. , Ramos M. , Molholm S. , Foxe J. J. , Halperin J. ( 2012 ). Auditory selective attention and processing in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder . Clinical Neurophysiology , 123 , 293 – 302 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Hillyard S. A. , Hink R. F. , Schwent V. L. , Picton T. W. ( 1973 ). Electrical signs of selective attention in the human brain . Science , 182 , 177 – 180 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Hogan A. M. , Telfer P. T. , Kirkham F. J. , de Haan M. ( 2013 ). Precursors of executive function in infants with sickle cell anemia . Journal of Child Neurology , 28 , 1197 – 1202 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Hogan A. M. , Vargha-Khadem F. , Saunders D. E. , Kirkham F. J. , Baldeweg T. ( 2006 ). Impact of frontal white matter lesions on performance monitoring: ERP evidence for cortical disconnection . Brain , 129 , 2177 – 2188 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Isbell E. , Hampton Wray A. , Neville H. J. ( 2016 ). Individual differences in neural mechanisms of selective auditory attention in preschoolers from lower socioeconomic status backgrounds: An event-related potentials study . Developmental Science , 19 , 865 – 880 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Isbell E. , Stevens C. , Hampton Wray A. , Bell T. , Neville H. J. ( 2016 ). 5-HTTLPR polymorphism is linked to neural mechanisms of selective attention in preschoolers from lower socioeconomic status backgrounds . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 22 , 36 – 47 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Jordan C. L. , DeBaun M. R. ( 2017 ). Cerebral hemodynamic assessment and neuroimaging across the lifespan in sickle cell disease . Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/abs/10.1177/0271678X17701763?url_ver=Z39.88-2003&rfr_id=ori:rid:crossref.org&rfr_dat=cr_pub%3dpubmed. Karns C. M. , Isbell E. , Giuliano R. J. , Neville H. J. ( 2015 ). Auditory attention in childhood and adolescence: An event-related potential study of spatial selective attention to one of two simultaneous stories . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 13 , 53 – 67 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Kleiner M. , Brainard D. , Pelli D. , Ingling A. , Murray R. , Broussard C. ( 2007 ). What’s new in Psychtoolbox-3 . Perception , 36 , 1 – 16 . Lackner C. L. , Santesso D. L. , Dywan J. , Wade T. J. , Segalowitz S. J. ( 2013 ). Electrocortical indices of selective attention predict adolescent executive functioning . Biological Psychology , 93 , 325 – 333 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Loiselle D. L. , Stamm J. S. , Maitinsky S. , Whipple S. C. ( 1980 ). Evoked potential and behavioral signs of attentive dysfunctions in hyperactive boys . Psychophysiology , 17 , 193 – 201 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Luck S. J. ( 2005 ). Ten simple rules for designing ERP experiments. In Handy T. C. (Ed.), Event-related potentials: A methods handbook . Cambridge : MIT Press . Morasch K. C. , Bell M. A. ( 2011 ). The role of inhibitory control in behavioral and physiological expressions of toddler executive function . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 108 , 593 – 606 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Nager W. , Estorf K. , Münte T. F. ( 2006 ). Crossmodal attention effects on brain responses to different stimulus classes . BMC Neuroscience , 7 , 31 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Neville H. J. , Stevens C. , Pakulak E. , Bell T. A. , Fanning J. , Klein S. , Isbell E. ( 2013 ). Family-based training program improves brain function, cognition, and behavior in lower socioeconomic status pre-schoolers . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America , 110 , 12138 – 12143 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Noll R. B. , Stith L. , Gartstein M. A. , Ris M. D. , Grueneich R. , Vannatta K. , Kalinyak K. ( 2001 ). Neuropsychological functioning of youths with sickle cell disease: Comparison with non-chronically ill peers . Journal of Pediatric Psychology , 26 , 69 – 78 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Ohene-Frempong K. , Weiner S. J. , Sleeper L. A. , Miller S. T. , Embury S. , Moohr J. W. , Wethers D. L. , Pegelow C. H. , Gill F. M. ( 1998 ). Cerebrovascular accidents in sickle cell disease: Rates and risk factors . Blood , 91 , 288 – 294 . Google Scholar PubMed Ponton C. W. , Eggermont J. J. , Kwong B. , Don M. ( 2000 ). Maturation of human central auditory system activity: Evidence from multi-channel evoked potentials . Clinical Neurophysiology , 111 , 220 – 236 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Prohovnik I. , Hurlet-Jensen A. , Adams R. , De Vivo D. , Pavlakis S. G. ( 2009 ). Hemodynamic etiology of elevated flow velocity and stroke in sickle-cell disease . Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism , 29 , 803 – 810 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Ridderinkhof K. R. , van der Stelt O. ( 2000 ). Attention and selection in the growing child: Views derived from developmental psychophysiology . Biological Psychology , 54 , 55 – 106 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Rueda M. R. , Checa P. , Cómbita L. M. ( 2012 ). Enhanced efficiency of the executive attention network after training in preschool children: Immediate changes and effects after two months . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 2 , S192 – S204 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Rueda M. R. , Checa P. , Rothbart M. K. ( 2010 ). Contributions of attentional control to socioemotional and academic development . Early Education and Development , 21 , 744 – 764 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Rueda M. R. , Posner M. I. , Rothbart M. K. ( 2004 ). Attentional control and self-regulation. In K.D .Vohs & R.F. Baumeister (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (284–299). New York: The Guilford Press. Rueda M. R. , Rothbart M. K. , McCandliss B. D. , Saccomanno L. , Posner M. I. ( 2005 ). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America , 102 , 14931 – 14936 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Sanders L. D. , Stevens C. , Coch D. , Neville H. J. ( 2006 ). Selective auditory attention in 3-to 5-year-old children: An event-related potential study . Neuropsychologia , 44 , 2126 – 2138 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Sanders L. D. , Zobel B. H. ( 2012 ). Nonverbal spatially selective attention in 4-and 5-year-old children . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 2 , 317 – 328 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Schatz J. , Finke R. L. , Kellett J. M. , Kramer J. H. ( 2002 ). Cognitive functioning in children with sickle cell disease: A meta-analysis . Journal of Pediatric Psychology , 27 , 739 – 748 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Schatz J. , Roberts C. W. ( 2007 ). Neurobehavioral impact of sickle cell disease in early childhood . Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society , 13 , 933 – 43 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Sharma A. , Kraus N. , McGee T. J. , Nicol T. G. ( 1997 ). Developmental changes in P1 and N1 central auditory responses elicited by consonant-vowel syllables . Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology/Evoked Potentials Section , 104 , 540 – 545 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Steen R. G. , Emudianughe T. , Hunte M. , Glass J. , Wu S. , Xiong X. , Reddick W. E. ( 2005 ). Brain volume in pediatric patients with sickle cell disease: Evidence of volumetric growth delay? American Journal of Neuroradiology , 26 , 455 – 462 . Google Scholar PubMed Steen R. G. , Hu X. J. , Elliott V. E. , Miles M. A. , Jones S. , Wang W. C. ( 2002 ). Kindergarten readiness skills in children with sickle cell disease: Evidence of early neurocognitive damage? Journal of Child Neurology , 17 , 111 – 116 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Stevens C. , Bavelier D. ( 2012 ). The role of selective attention on academic foundations: A cognitive neuroscience perspective . Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience , 2 , S30 – S48 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Stevens C. , Lauinger B. , Neville H. ( 2009 ). Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: An event‐related brain potential study . Developmental Science , 12 , 634 – 646 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Stevens C. , Sanders L. , Neville H. ( 2006 ). Neurophysiological evidence for selective auditory attention deficits in children with specific language impairment . Brain Research , 1111 , 143 – 152 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Strait D. L. , Slater J. , Abecassis V. , Kraus N. ( 2014 ). Cortical response variability as a developmental index of selective auditory attention . Developmental Science , 17 , 175 – 186 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Tarazi R. A. , Grant M. L. , Ely E. , Barakat L. P. ( 2007 ). Neuropsychological functioning in preschool-age children with sickle cell disease: The role of illness-related and psychosocial factors . Child Neuropsychology , 13 , 155 – 172 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Taylor M. J. , Baldeweg T. ( 2002 ). Application of EEG, ERP and intracranial recordings to the investigation of cognitive functions in children . Developmental Science , 5 , 318 – 334 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS Thompson R. J. , Gustafson K. E. , Bonner M. J. , Ware R. E. ( 2002 ). Neurocognitive development of young children with sickle cell disease through three years of age . Journal of Pediatric Psychology , 27 , 235 – 244 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Van Beijsterveldt C. , Van Baal G. ( 2002 ). Twin and family studies of the human electroencephalogram: A review and a meta-analysis . Biological Psychology , 61 , 111 – 138 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed van der Land V. , Hijmans C. T. , de Ruiter M. , Mutsaerts H. J. , Cnossen M. H. , Engelen M. , Majoie C. B. , Nederveen A. J. , Grootenhuis M. A. , Fijnvandraat K. ( 2015 ). Volume of white matter hyperintensities is an independent predictor of intelligence quotient and processing speed in children with sickle cell disease . British Journal of Haematology , 168 , 553 – 556 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Vichinsky E. P. , Neumayr L. D. , Gold J. I. , Weiner M. W. , Rule R. R. , Truran D. , Kasten J. , Eggleston B. , Kesler K. , McMahon L. , Orringer E. P. , Harrington T. , Kalinyak K. , De Castro L. M. , Kutlar A. , Rutherford C. J. , Johnson C. , Bessman J. D. , Jordan L. B. , Armstrong F. D. ; Neuropsychological Dysfunction and Neuroimaging Adult Sickle Cell Anemia Study Group . ( 2010 ). Neuropsychological dysfunction and neuroimaging abnormalities in neurologically intact adults with sickle cell anemia . JAMA , 303 , 1823 – 1831 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Wang W. , Enos L. , Gallagher D. , Thompson R. , Guarini L. , Vichinsky E. , Wright E. , Zimmerman R. , Armstrong F. D ; Cooperative Study of Sickle Cell Disease . ( 2001 ). Neuropsychologic performance in school-aged children with sickle cell disease: A report from the Cooperative Study of Sickle Cell Disease . The Journal of Pediatrics , 139 , 391 – 397 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Watkins K. , Hewes D. , Connelly A. , Kendall B. , Kingsley D. , Evans J. , Gadian D. G. , Vargha-Khadem F. , Kirkham F. ( 1998 ). Cognitive deficits associated with frontal‐lobe infarction in children with sickle cell disease . Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology , 40 , 536 – 543 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Wechsler D. ( 2002 ). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligenceTM: Third edition (WPPSITM-III) : Sydney, NSW : Pearson . Wolfe C. D. , Bell M. A. ( 2007 ). The integration of cognition and emotion during infancy and early childhood: Regulatory processes associated with the development of working memory . Brain and Cognition , 65 , 3 – 13 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Yurgelun-Todd D. ( 2007 ). Emotional and cognitive changes during adolescence . Current Opinion in Neurobiology , 17 , 251 – 257 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed Zelazo P. D. , Anderson J. E. , Richler J. , Wallner‐Allen K. , Beaumont J. L. , Weintraub S. ( 2013 ). II. NIH toolbox cognition battery (CB): Measuring executive function and attention . Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 78 , 16 – 33 . Google Scholar CrossRef Search ADS PubMed © The Author 2017. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society of Pediatric Psychology. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: [email protected] This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/about_us/legal/notices)

Journal

Journal of Pediatric PsychologyOxford University Press

Published: Sep 1, 2018

There are no references for this article.