Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 14-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

A Family Study of Obsessive-compulsive Disorder

A Family Study of Obsessive-compulsive Disorder BackgroundThe causes of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) are as yet unknown. Evidence of familial aggregation is one approach for investigating the role of genetics in the etiology of this condition. The current study was conducted to determine if OCD is familial and to investigate possible familial subtypes.MethodsEighty case probands were identified in 5 specialty OCD clinics and 73 community control probands were identified by random-digit dialing. These probands and their first-degree relatives (343 case and 300 control relatives) were blinded to group and evaluated by psychiatrists and doctoral-level clinical psychologists using semistructured instruments. Final diagnoses were assigned by a blinded-consensus procedure. The results were analyzed using logistic regression by the method of generalized estimating equations.ResultsThe lifetime prevalence of OCD was significantly higher in case compared with control relatives (11.7% vs 2.7%) (P<.001). Case relatives had higher rates of both obsessions and compulsions; however, this finding is more robust for obsessions. Age at onset of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in the case proband was strongly related to familiality (odds ratio, 0.92; confidence interval, 0.85-0.99) (P=.05); no case of OCD symptoms was detected in the relatives of probands whose age at onset of symptoms was 18 years or older. Probands with tics or obsessive-compulsive personality disorder were not more likely to have relatives with OCD than those without these features.ConclusionsObsessive-compulsive disorder is a familial disorder. Obsessions are more specific to the phenotype than are compulsions. Age at onset of OCD is valuable in characterizing a familial subtype.OBSESSIVE-COMPULSIVE disorder (OCD) is a psychiatric condition first described more than 100 years ago.The pathognomonic features of the disorder are persistent, intrusive, senseless thoughts and impulses (obsessions) and repetitive, intentional behaviors (compulsions). Patients with the disorder recognize that their thoughts and behaviors are excessive and unreasonable, and they struggle to resist them. The lifetime prevalence of OCD is estimated to be 1% to 3%, based on population-based surveys conducted in many communities nationally and internationally.Although the disorder affects individuals of all ages, the period of greatest risk is from childhood to middle adulthoodPatients experience a chronic or episodic course with exacerbations that can substantially impair social, occupational, and academic functioning; according to the World Health Organization, OCD is among the 10 most disabling medical conditions worldwide.The role of heredity in OCD has long been suspected by clinicians.Several twin studies have found that the concordance for obsessive-compulsive symptoms is substantially greater in monozygotic twin pairs (80%-87%) than dizygotic twin pairs (47%-50%).Many family studies of OCD have been conducted, although most had methodological limitations, such as failure to use structured or semistructured diagnostic instruments and noninclusion of control groups.The results from more recent controlled family studies, which used contemporary diagnostic criteria and assessment methods, have been inconsistent. Pauls et alfound higher morbid risks of OCD in first-degree relatives of OCD probands vs first-degree relatives of psychiatrically normal controls (10% vs 1.9%). However, Black et aldid not find differences in the rates of OCD in case and control relatives, and they concluded that an anxiety diathesis rather than OCD per se may be inherited. Given the importance of this issue, additional research was warranted.The current study was conducted to extend knowledge regarding the familial nature of OCD. It was designed to improve on those that preceded it, and provide as rigorous a test of the hypothesis as possible. Care was taken to study probands from multiple treatment sites to reduce potential selection bias. Community controls were selected carefully to be comparable with the probands. Comprehensive diagnostic assessments were performed by psychiatrists and doctorate-level clinical psychologists on adults and children using age-appropriate diagnostic methods. The primary goal of the study was to determine whether OCD is familial, and to investigate clinical characteristics that might influence the familial aggregation of the disorder.SUBJECTS AND METHODSSTUDY SAMPLEIn the family study, we selected 99 adult OCD probands (aged ≥18 years) from 5 specialty OCD treatment centers in the Baltimore, Md, and Washington, DC, area. All adult patients with a diagnosis of OCD who were first evaluated in these centers in the 3 years prior to the initiation of this study were rostered (N=418). A sample from each site was randomly selected to participate in the study, and these patients were contacted by their treating clinician to request participation. Interviews were completed for 80 of these case probands and 343 of their first-degree relatives. Control probands were identified by a random-digit dialing procedure, conducted by a survey research contractor (Battelle Corp, Baltimore), which individually matched control to case probands on sex, race, and age (within 10 years) and on whether medical care had been received in the preceding 1 year. Recruitment staff then contacted the control probands and their first-degree relatives. Interviews were completed for 73 control probands and 300 of their first-degree relatives.Case probands were included if they met DSM-IVcriteria for OCD as diagnosed by the consensus procedure. They also needed to score greater than 15 on the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scaleduring the worst episode. The protocol excluded probands diagnosed with schizophrenia, mental retardation, dementia, or Tourette disorder, or if OCD occurred exclusively during a major depressive episode. Control probands also were excluded if any of the above diagnoses were present. In addition, if no member of the family other than the proband consented to participate, that family was excluded.DIAGNOSTIC PROCEDURESAfter informed consent was obtained, the subjects were interviewed by clinicians (psychiatrists or doctorate-level clinical psychologists) who had undergone extensive training in the diagnostic procedure. Most interviews were conducted in person in the research offices or subjects' homes, at locations across the United States. Interviews were audiotaped for training, quality control, reliability, and diagnostic consensus purposes. Interviewers were blinded as to whether subjects were probands or relatives or whether they were from case or control families. In addition to conducting direct interviews, the clinicians contacted a knowledgeable informant for each subject to obtain collateral information for diagnostic evaluations. Forty-seven direct interviews could not be conducted in person; these were conducted by telephone. All probands were interviewed in person. One hundred twenty-two case relatives (36%) and 69 control relatives (23%) were not available for interview, because they were deceased, refused to participate, or the proband refused to allow contact; for these subjects, informant interviews were conducted with 2 individuals familiar with the subject. Children aged 8 to 15 years were evaluated using similar procedures, except that personality was not assessed, and their interviews were conducted by clinicians who had experience interviewing children.Diagnostic instruments included the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia–Lifetime Anxiety,which was adapted for this study. The OCD section was elaborated to include the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale,as well as additional material on the course of symptoms. Sections on Tourette disorder, tic disorders, and impulse control disorders also were added. The Family Informant Schedule and Criteriawas adapted for interviewing collateral informants. The Structured Instrument for the Diagnosis of Personality Disorderswas employed for the assessment of personality disorders. The Revised NEO Personality Inventoryand Leyton Obsessional Inventorywere used as self-report measures of normal personality and obsessional features, respectively. Children were evaluated in a parallel procedure using comparable instruments (the Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia,the Children's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale,and Leyton Obsessional Inventory–Child Version).For every subject, a DSM-IVdiagnostic assignment form was completed; this form was designed to record the presence of all necessary diagnostic criteria and guide the assignment of diagnoses according to DSM-IVcriteria.The interrater reliability of the examiners' diagnostic procedure was studied in a sample of 30 subjects. There was good diagnostic agreement, as evidenced by a κ=0.81 for OCD and κ=1.0 for major depression; the intraclass correlation coefficient was 0.78 for compulsive personality disorder traits.All available diagnostic materials (evaluation by the clinical examiner, informant interview, clinical case summary, medical records, and audiotapes) were reviewed independently by 2 expert psychiatrists (G.N., M.R., J.B., R.H-S., M.G., or J.W.). The diagnosticians were blind as to whether the subject was a proband or relative and whether they were from a case or control family. All psychiatric diagnoses were made according to strict DSM-IVcriteria.If all the criteria required for having the disorder were met, then a "definite" diagnosis was given. If any necessary criterion was absent, then the diagnosis was considered "not present." If it seemed likely that the subject had the diagnosis, but the diagnosticians could not be certain of a given criterion (required for definite diagnosis), then the diagnosis was made at the "probable" level. If the diagnosticians could not be sure of the presence or absence of a given diagnosis, then that diagnosis was recorded as "unknown." Obsessions and compulsions (but not OCD) were diagnosed if these symptoms were present but did not meet the impairment criteria required for OCD.DATA ANALYSISSubjects with unknown diagnoses for the phenotypes of main interest in this article (OCD, obsessions, and compulsions) were excluded from both the numerator and the denominator in the analyses. Cases with probable diagnoses were treated in 2 ways: (1) when analyzing definite outcomes, these cases were considered as not present and excluded from the numerator but included in the denominator and (2) when analyzing definite and probable outcomes, these cases were considered as probable and included in the numerator and denominator. Although not reported here, we also reanalyzed the data considering the probable cases as "missing" and excluding them from both numerator and denominator; the magnitudes of the estimated odds ratios (ORs) were marginally stronger than the first method and considerably stronger than the second method described above.Demographic characteristics in case and control probands and case and control relatives, were compared with χ2or Fisher exact tests for categorical dataand tor median tests for continuous data. All tests were 2-tailed, with α=.05. The odds of OCD, obsessions, and compulsions in case vs control relatives were estimated using logistic regression by the method of generalized estimating equations, which accounts for within-family correlation among relatives.Specifically, ignoring within-family correlation may lead to substantially incorrect SE calculations for logistic regression coefficient estimates. This method provides correct SE estimates using conventional methods while acknowledging that part of the model (within-family correlation in this case) may be misspecified. Potential confounding factors (age of relative, sex of relative, and type of interview [direct or informant-only&rsqb;) were controlled by including terms for these variables in the regression models.RESULTSCHARACTERISTICS OF THE STUDY SAMPLEAs presented in Table 1, 53% of the case probands and 60% of the control probands were female. All but 1 proband in each group were white; 1 case and 1 control proband was Hispanic. Case and control probands were of similar ages. In the case probands, the age range was 18 to 88 years (median, 36 years; mean, 37 years); in the control probands, the age range was 18 to 79 years (median, 36 years; mean, 39 years). Both groups were highly educated. Most were college graduates, and 24% of the case probands and 22% of the control probands had graduate training. Despite these similarities, however, the case probands were significantly less likely to have ever married and, if married, to have children. The number of siblings in case-proband families (examined and not examined) (mean [SD&rsqb;, 2.1 [1.5&rsqb; siblings) was not significantly different than control proband families (mean [SD&rsqb;, 2.6 [1.8&rsqb; siblings) (t151=1.77; P=.08); however, case probands had significantly fewer children (for case probands: mean [SD&rsqb;, 0.5 [0.8&rsqb; children; for control probands: mean [SD&rsqb;, 1.3 [1.5&rsqb; children) (t151=4.25; P<.001).Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of Case and Control ProbandsCase Probands (n=80)Control Probands (n=73)Test Statistic*PSex, No. (%)Male38 (48)29 (40)χ21=0.94.33Female42 (53)44 (60)Mean (SD) age, y36.6 (11.6)38.5 (11.8)t151=0.98.33Schooling, No. (%)†College graduate53 (66)38 (52)High school graduate23 (29)30 (41)χ22=3.20.20Not a high school graduate4 (5)5 (7)Marital status, No. (%)Ever married42 (53)53 (73)χ21=6.55.01Never married38 (48)20 (27)Children, if ever married, No. (%)‡Yes27 (66)47 (89)χ21=7.19.007No14 (34)6 (11)*Degrees of freedom are presented as subscript values.†Using Bartholomew test for order.‡Unknown for one case proband.As presented in Table 2, case relatives were, on average, significantly older than control relatives; in case relatives, the age range was 9 to 94 years (median, 49 years; mean, 48 years); in control relatives, the age range was 8 to 91 years) (median, 43 years; mean, 45 years). There was a slight preponderance of females in the control relatives. The distribution of type of relative was significantly different between the 2 groups; children comprised a smaller proportion of case than control relatives. The mean (SD) number of first-degree relatives assessed (in addition to the proband) was 4.3 (1.8) for case families and 4.1 (2.2) for control families (t151=0.55; P=.59).Table 2. Demographic Characteristics of Case and Control First-Degree RelativesCase Relatives (n=343)Control Relatives (n=300)Test Statistic*PSex, No. (%)Male178 (51.9)135 (45.0)χ21=3.05.08Female165 (48.1)165 (55.0)Mean (SD) age, y48.1 (18.9)44.5 (18.8)t641=2.46.01Relatives interviewed, No. (%)Parents149 (43.5)108 (36.0)χ22=14.75.01Siblings163 (47.5)134 (44.7)Children31 (9.0)58 (19.3)*Degrees of freedom are shown as subscript values.PREVALENCE AND ODDS OF OBSESSIONS, COMPULSIONS, AND OCD IN RELATIVESAs presented in Table 3, the prevalence of definite OCD was substantially higher in case than control relatives (11.7% vs 2.7%). The prevalence of definite and probable OCD also was higher in case relatives (16.3% vs 5.7%). The prevalence of definite obsessions was 10.4% and 2.4% and 15.1% and 3.8% for definite and probable obsessions in the case and control relatives, respectively. Similar differences are found for the compulsions: 18% of the case relatives vs 6.1% of the control relatives were diagnosed with definite compulsions, and 25% of case relatives vs 12% of the control relatives had definite and probable compulsions.Table 3. Prevalence of Obsessions, Compulsions, and Obsessive-compulsive Disorder (OCD) in Relatives*PhenotypeCase Relatives Affected,† No. (%)Control Relatives Affected,‡ No. (%)Odds Ratio (95% CI)POCD (definite)38 (11.7)8 (2.7)4.7 (2.2-10.3).001OCD (definite and probable)53 (16.3)17 (5.7)3.1 (1.7-5.8).003Obsessions (definite)33 (10.4)7 (2.4)4.7 (2.0-10.8).003Obsessions (definite and probable)48 (15.1)11 (3.8)4.6 (2.4-8.8)<.001Compulsions (definite)58 (18.2)18 (6.1)3.4 (1.8-6.3)<.001Compulsions (definite and probable)79 (24.8)36 (12.2)2.4 (1.5-3.7)<.002*CI indicates confidence interval.†The number of case relatives with nonmissing diagnostic information for OCD was 326; for obsessions and for compulsions, 318.‡The number of control relatives with nonmissing diagnostic information for OCD was 297; for obsessions, 293; and for compulsions, 294.The odds of definite OCD is nearly 5 times greater in case relatives than control relatives (OR, 4.7; 95% confidence interval, 2.2-10.3) (P=.001) (Table 3). In addition, significant ORs were found for all definitions of the affected phenotype, indicating that first-degree relatives of cases met criteria for OCD-related phenotypes significantly more often than first-degree relatives of controls. In general, using more stringent criteria for affected status (ie, definite as opposed to definite and probable) resulted in larger ORs. Although there is a lower frequency of obsessions than compulsions in both case and control relatives, the magnitude of the association for obsessions is larger than that for compulsions.As described earlier, case and control relatives were different with respect to age (case relatives were older), sex of relatives interviewed (a larger proportion of case relatives were male), and the type of interview conducted (a greater proportion of case relatives had informant-only interviews). Moreover, the prevalence of OCD was inversely related to current age and was greater in relatives who were directly interviewed than in those who had informant-only interviews (data not shown). We therefore controlled for these potential confounding variables in the regression models. The OR of OCD was higher after adjusting for these factors (Table 4); adjustment also increased the ORs for obsessions and compulsions (data not shown).Table 4. Odds Ratios for Definite Obsessive-compulsive Disorder (OCD) in Relatives Using a Multivariable Model*Characteristic of RelativeOdds Ratio (95% CI)PGroupCase5.5 (2.6-11.8)<.001Control†1.0SexMale1.4 (0.8-2.7).26Female†1.0Type of interviewDirect2.6 (0.9-7.5).08Informant-only†1.0Age at interview (per year increase)0.98 (0.97-1.00).04*Determined using logistic regression by the method of generalized estimating equation. CI indicates confidence interval.†Reference group.PREVALENCE OF OCD IN CASE RELATIVES BY PROBAND CLINICAL CHARACTERISTICSThe age at onset of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in the 80 case probands ranged from 5 to 41 years. As shown in Figure 1, the median age at onset of symptoms was about 11 years; more than 75% of the probands had onset by age 14 years, and 90% by age 17 years. The mean (SD) age at onset in males (11.5 [5.6&rsqb; years; range, 5-30 years) was similar to that in females (12.0 [7.4&rsqb; years; range, 5-41 years) (t78=0.32; P=.75).Age at onset of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in case probands (N=80).Age at onset in case probands was dichotomized into early onset (5-17 years) and late onset (18-41 years) groups. The prevalence of OCD in the relatives of probands with early vs late onset was 38 (13.8%) of 276 and 0 (0%) of 49, respectively (P=.006 by Fisher exact test); thus, no cases of OCD were found in the case relatives of probands with late onset of obsessive-compulsive symptoms. In addition, the association between age at onset of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in the case probands and the prevalence of definite OCD in their relatives was examined by including this characteristic as a quantitative variable in a generalized estimating equation model. There was a significant inverse relationship between age at onset and the odds of OCD (OR, 0.92; 95% confidence interval, 0.85-0.99) (P=.02) (Table 5).Table 5. Odds Ratios for Definite Obsessive-compulsive Disorder (OCD) in Case Relatives by Proband Clinical Characteristics*Proband CharacteristicRelatives Affected, No. (%)Odds Ratio (95% CI)†PTics‡Present2 (6.7)0.73 (0.4-1.4).33Absent§35 (12.3)1.00OCPD&par;Present10 (11.0)0.90 (0.3-2.4).83Absent§25 (13.1)1.00Age at onset of OCD symptoms (per year increase)0.92 (0.85-0.99).05*CI indicates confidence interval; OCPD, obsessive-compulsive personality disorder.†Determined using logistic regression by the method of generalized estimating equation and controlling for relatives' age, sex, and type of interview.‡The number of relatives of probands with tics was 30; without tics, 284.§Reference group.&par;The number of relatives of probands with OCPD was 91; without OCPD, 191.Tics (single or multiple vocal or motor tics) were diagnosed in 8 (10.4%) of 77 case probands but no control probands (P=.008 by Fisher exact test). The prevalence of definite OCD was nonsignificantly lower in the relatives of probands with tics (6.7%) compared with the relative of probands without tics (12.3%) (P=.33). The prevalence of definite OCD was similar in relatives of case probands with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD) (10 [11%&rsqb; of 91) and without OCPD (25 [13.1%&rsqb; of 191) (P=.83) (Table 5).COMMENTThis study reports 3 main findings. First, OCD is familial. The first-degree relatives of probands diagnosed with OCD had a nearly 5-fold higher lifetime prevalence of OCD. This finding is more robust for a definite diagnosis of OCD (ie, meeting all the DSM-IVcriteria); however, the results are significant if probable diagnoses are included. The difference in rates between case and control relatives should be considered from the vantage point of prevalence rates of OCD in the general population; the estimated lifetime prevalence rate in the control relatives was consistent with those in published epidemiological studies.Our findings are generally in accord with the most recent family study of OCD.Second, obsessions are more specific to the familial aspect of the disorder than are compulsions. Relatives of case probands are at greater risk for both obsessions and compulsions than the relatives of controls; however, the magnitude of the association is substantially stronger for obsessions than compulsions. It should be noted that the frequency of compulsions in both case and control relatives is relatively high. This may indicate substantial heterogeneity for compulsions (ie, they may be, in some instances, innocuous behaviors unrelated to an OCD diathesis, or they may be difficult to distinguish clinically from behaviors that are similar in form). These latter behaviors may be associated with other medical conditions and ought not be classified in the same group as compulsions. If there is heterogeneity, then compulsions alone perhaps should not be included in the phenotype for studies of OCD. Additionally, the specification for the definition of compulsions in the DSM-IVmay benefit from refinement.Third, earlier age at onset of the symptoms of OCD indicates a more familial subgroup. The relationship between younger ages at onset and familiality of medical conditions is a well-recognized phenomenon.This finding has been reported previously for OCD,and the association was extremely significant in our sample; indeed, we found no occurrence of OCD in the relatives of probands who had an age at onset of OCD older than 17 years. This suggests that cases of OCD with early age at onset are more likely to yield information about the genetic origins of this disorder. Conversely, cases with older ages at onset, whose numbers are not inconsequential, may have different origins.There has been a long-standing clinical impression that OCD symptoms may emerge as neurotic manifestations in individuals with OCPD.We hypothesized that the presence of OCPD distinguishes a subgroup of cases of OCD with a higher risk of OCD in their relatives. However, the results do not substantiate this hypothesis: there was no difference in the familial risk of OCD based on the presence of OCPD in the case probands.Previous studies have found that OCD occurs more frequently in the families of those with Tourette disorder, and it has been suggested that the presence of comorbid tics defines a familial subtype of OCD.The results of the current study do not support this suggestion, although we recognize that the overall recurrence risk in families may be similar even if there are subtypes with different origins. Tics may be heterogeneous, with some related to OCD and some not. Because we excluded probands with Tourette disorder from the study sample, we may have excluded families with a tic-related subtype of OCD.We acknowledge several methodologic limitations of our study. First, all case probands were in treatment. A community sample of OCD cases would have been more desirable; however, given our own experience in epidemiological studies of OCD,we would have had to screen thousands of subjects to obtain a sufficient number of OCD probands to conduct this study. To minimize selection bias, subjects were rostered and randomly selected from each of the 5 treatment centers. In the sampling frame, we included all probands who presented for treatment during a specified period; subjects who dropped out of treatment were as eligible for selection as those who did not.Second, only adult probands were selected. This may have reduced the estimate of familial risk, given that our results indicate that younger age at onset is associated with greater familial risk. In addition, few Hispanic and no African American subjects participated in the study. The low frequency of African American cases in the participating centers is compatible with the clinical experience of other centers and with epidemiologic studies.Third, control probands may have agreed to participate based on their personal or family concerns about emotional difficulties. Although unaware that this was a study of OCD, control probands knew they were being asked to participate in a family study of psychological difficulties. The effect of such a bias would be to reduce the likelihood of differences in rates of psychopathology between case and control relatives, making comparisons between the 2 groups more conservative. Furthermore, the control probands were identified by a stringent sampling method within the community in which the case probands resided. Every effort was made to ensure that the control probands were similar to the cases in all respects other than the presence of a diagnosis of OCD. Thus, control relatives were not a "supernormal" control group but were representative of persons residing in the communities in which the cases lived.Fourth, not all the interviews in the study were direct and in person. We conducted most interviews in subjects' homes; however, in a minority of cases, a telephone interview was unavoidable. We maintained the same protocol for telephone interviews, including audiotaping and accessing informants; the rates of disorders did not differ between the in-person and telephone interviews. Some relatives could not be interviewed at all because of death or refusal to participate. We strove to include these relatives by conducting family informant interviews with 2 individuals knowledgeable about the subject in each instance. Overall, 71% of the relatives were directly interviewed.The finding that OCD is familial is a necessary, albeit not sufficient, condition for genetic etiology. The estimated λ (ie, the ratio of the rates of definite OCD in case and control relatives, respectively) of 4.33 is within the range of that for other psychiatric disorders, such as bipolar disorder and panic disorder, although substantially lower than others, such as autism.The results, together with those from previous twin studies, encourage a molecular genetic approach to identify a genetic etiology for OCD. Clarification of the range of psychopathology that is transmitted in these families, as well as identification of etiologically homogeneous clinical subtypes, will increase the ability of association studies and genome-wide searches to detect genes involved in the pathogenesis of OCD.GWestphahlÜber zwangsvorstellungen.Archiv Psychiatr Nervenkrankheiten.1878;8:734-750.MKarnoJMGoldingObsessive compulsive disorder.In: Robins LN, Regier DA, eds. Psychiatric Disorders in America. New York, NY: Free Press; 1991:204-219.MMWeissmanRCBlandGJCaninoSGreenwaldHGHwuCKyoonSCNewmanMAOakley-BrowneMRubio-StipecPJWickramaratneHUWittchenYEng-Kungfor the Cross National Collaborative GroupThe cross national epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder.J Clin Psychiatry.1994;55(suppl):S5-S10.GNestadtJFSamuelsAJRomanoskiMFFolsteinPRMcHughObsessions and compulsions in the community.Acta Psychiatr Scand.1994;89:219-224.GNestadtOJBienvenuGCaiJSamuelsWWEatonIncidence of obsessive-compulsive disorder in adults.J Nerv Ment Dis.1998;186:401-406.CLMurrayADLopezThe Global Burden of Disease: A Comprehensive Assessment of Mortality and Disability From Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors in 1990 and Projected.Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press; 1996.ALewisProblems of obsessional illness.Proc R Soc Med.1935;29:325-336.RWoodruffFMPittsMonozygotic twins with obsessional illness.Am J Psychiatry.1964;120:1075-1080.IMMarksMCroweEDreweJYoungWGDewhurstObsessive-compulsive neurosis in identical twins.Br J Psychiatry.1969;115:991-998.PMcGuffinDMawsonObsessive-compulsive neurosis: two identical twin pairs.Br J Psychiatry.1980;137:285-287.EMCryanGJButcherMGWebbObsessive-compulsive disorder and paraphilia in a monozygotic twin pair.Br J Psychiatry.1992;161:694-698.EInyoueSimilar and dissimilar manifestations of obsessive-compulsive neurosis in monozygotic twins.Am J Psychiatry.1965;121:1171-1175.GCareyIIGottesmanTwin and family studies of anxiety, phobic, and obsessive disorders.In: Klein DF, Rabkin JG, eds. Anxiety: New Research and Changing Concepts. New York, NY: Raven Press; 1981:117-136.MCLenaneSESwedoHLeonardDLPaulsWSceeryJLRapoportPsychiatric disorders in first-degree relatives of children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry.1990;29:407-412.MARiddleLScahillRKingMTHardinKETowbinSIOrtJFLeckmanDJCohenObsessive compulsive disorder in children and adolescents: phenomenology and family history.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry.1990;29:766-772.HLLeonardMCLenaneSESwedoDCRettewESGershonJLRapoportTics and Tourette's disorder: a 2- to 7-year followup of 54 obsessive-compulsive children.Am J Psychiatry.1992;149:1244-1251.LBellodiGSciutoGDiaferiaPRonchiESmeraldiPsychiatric disorders in the families of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder.Psychiatry Res.1992;42:111-120.DLPaulsJPAlsobrookWGoodmanSRasmussenJFLeckmanA family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder.Am J Psychiatry.1995;152:76-84.DWBlackRNoyes JrRBGoldsteinNBlumA family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder.Arch Gen Psychiatry.1992;49:362-368.WKGoodmanLHPriceSARasmussenCMazureRLFleischmannCLHillGRHeningerDSCharneyThe Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale, I: development, use, and reliability.Arch Gen Psychiatry.1989;46:1006-1011.SMannuzzaAJFyerDFKleinJEndicottSchedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia–Lifetime Version Modified for the Study of Anxiety Disorders (SADS-LA): rationale and conceptual development.J Psychiatr Res.1986;20:317-325.SMannuzzaAJFyerJEndicottDFKleinFamily Informant Schedule and Criteria.New York, NY: Anxiety Family Study Unit, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1985.BPfohlNBlumMZimmermanDStanglStructured Interview for DSM-III-R Personality–Revised (SIDP-R).Iowa City: University of Iowa College of Medicine; 1989.PTCosta JrRRMcCraeRevised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) Professional Manual.Odessa, Fla: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992.JCooperThe Leyton Obsessional Inventory.Psychol Med.1970;1:48-64.HOrvaschelJPuig-AntichSchedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Epidemiologic Version (K-SADS-E).Pittsburgh, Pa: Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic; 1987.LScahillMARiddleMMcSwiggin-HardinSIOrtRAKingWKGoodmanDCicchettiJFLeckmanChildren's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: reliability and validity.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry.1997;36:844-852.CJBergJLRapoportMFlamentThe Leyton Obsessional Inventory–Child Version.J Am Acad Child Psychiatry.1986;25:84-91.American Psychiatric AssociationDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.JLFleissStatistical Methods for Rates and Proportions.New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 1973.KYLiangAEPulverAnalysis of case-control/family sampling design.Genet Epidemiol.1996;13:253-270.BChildsCRScriverAge at onset and causes of disease.Perspect Biol Med.1986;29(3, pt 1):437-460.JSandlerAHazariThe obsessional: on the psychological classification of obsessional character traits and symptoms.Br J Med Psychol.1960;33:113-122.DLPaulsJFLeckmanThe inheritance of Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome and associated behaviors.N Engl J Med.1986;315:993-997.NIMH Genetics WorkgroupGenetics and Mental Disorders.Washington, DC: National Institute of Mental Health; 1997.Accepted for publication December 17, 1999.Our research was supported by grant R01 MH50214 from the National Institute of Mental Health (Dr Nestadt), and by grant RR00052 from the Division of Research Resources General Clinical Reseach Centers, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md.We thank the many families participating in the study; Jennifer Hahn, PhD, David Wellen, PhD, Ruth Zitner, PsyD, Dan McLeod, PhD, Malgorzata Lamacz, PhD, and Bernadette Cullen, MD, for conducting clinical evaluations; Dennis Murphy, MD, Ben Greenberg, MD, PhD, Sally Winston, PsyD, Donna Burns, and Dorinda Schultz, for providing access to their clinic patients; David Pauls, PhD, Kathleen Merikangas, PhD, Abby Fyer, MD, and Salvatore Mannuzza, PhD, for consultation on this study; and Dorothy Carpenter, Krista Vermillion, Margaret Dees, and Sandra Hensley, for their efforts in coordinating the study.Reprints: Gerald Nestadt, MD, MPH, Johns Hopkins Hospital, Meyer 4-181, 600 N Wolfe St, Baltimore, MD 21287-7228 (e-mail: gnestadt@mail.jhmi.edu). http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png JAMA Psychiatry American Medical Association

Loading next page...
 
/lp/american-medical-association/a-family-study-of-obsessive-compulsive-disorder-V5DPRvgqN0

References (71)

Publisher
American Medical Association
Copyright
Copyright 2000 American Medical Association. All Rights Reserved. Applicable FARS/DFARS Restrictions Apply to Government Use.
ISSN
2168-622X
eISSN
2168-6238
DOI
10.1001/archpsyc.57.4.358
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

BackgroundThe causes of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) are as yet unknown. Evidence of familial aggregation is one approach for investigating the role of genetics in the etiology of this condition. The current study was conducted to determine if OCD is familial and to investigate possible familial subtypes.MethodsEighty case probands were identified in 5 specialty OCD clinics and 73 community control probands were identified by random-digit dialing. These probands and their first-degree relatives (343 case and 300 control relatives) were blinded to group and evaluated by psychiatrists and doctoral-level clinical psychologists using semistructured instruments. Final diagnoses were assigned by a blinded-consensus procedure. The results were analyzed using logistic regression by the method of generalized estimating equations.ResultsThe lifetime prevalence of OCD was significantly higher in case compared with control relatives (11.7% vs 2.7%) (P<.001). Case relatives had higher rates of both obsessions and compulsions; however, this finding is more robust for obsessions. Age at onset of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in the case proband was strongly related to familiality (odds ratio, 0.92; confidence interval, 0.85-0.99) (P=.05); no case of OCD symptoms was detected in the relatives of probands whose age at onset of symptoms was 18 years or older. Probands with tics or obsessive-compulsive personality disorder were not more likely to have relatives with OCD than those without these features.ConclusionsObsessive-compulsive disorder is a familial disorder. Obsessions are more specific to the phenotype than are compulsions. Age at onset of OCD is valuable in characterizing a familial subtype.OBSESSIVE-COMPULSIVE disorder (OCD) is a psychiatric condition first described more than 100 years ago.The pathognomonic features of the disorder are persistent, intrusive, senseless thoughts and impulses (obsessions) and repetitive, intentional behaviors (compulsions). Patients with the disorder recognize that their thoughts and behaviors are excessive and unreasonable, and they struggle to resist them. The lifetime prevalence of OCD is estimated to be 1% to 3%, based on population-based surveys conducted in many communities nationally and internationally.Although the disorder affects individuals of all ages, the period of greatest risk is from childhood to middle adulthoodPatients experience a chronic or episodic course with exacerbations that can substantially impair social, occupational, and academic functioning; according to the World Health Organization, OCD is among the 10 most disabling medical conditions worldwide.The role of heredity in OCD has long been suspected by clinicians.Several twin studies have found that the concordance for obsessive-compulsive symptoms is substantially greater in monozygotic twin pairs (80%-87%) than dizygotic twin pairs (47%-50%).Many family studies of OCD have been conducted, although most had methodological limitations, such as failure to use structured or semistructured diagnostic instruments and noninclusion of control groups.The results from more recent controlled family studies, which used contemporary diagnostic criteria and assessment methods, have been inconsistent. Pauls et alfound higher morbid risks of OCD in first-degree relatives of OCD probands vs first-degree relatives of psychiatrically normal controls (10% vs 1.9%). However, Black et aldid not find differences in the rates of OCD in case and control relatives, and they concluded that an anxiety diathesis rather than OCD per se may be inherited. Given the importance of this issue, additional research was warranted.The current study was conducted to extend knowledge regarding the familial nature of OCD. It was designed to improve on those that preceded it, and provide as rigorous a test of the hypothesis as possible. Care was taken to study probands from multiple treatment sites to reduce potential selection bias. Community controls were selected carefully to be comparable with the probands. Comprehensive diagnostic assessments were performed by psychiatrists and doctorate-level clinical psychologists on adults and children using age-appropriate diagnostic methods. The primary goal of the study was to determine whether OCD is familial, and to investigate clinical characteristics that might influence the familial aggregation of the disorder.SUBJECTS AND METHODSSTUDY SAMPLEIn the family study, we selected 99 adult OCD probands (aged ≥18 years) from 5 specialty OCD treatment centers in the Baltimore, Md, and Washington, DC, area. All adult patients with a diagnosis of OCD who were first evaluated in these centers in the 3 years prior to the initiation of this study were rostered (N=418). A sample from each site was randomly selected to participate in the study, and these patients were contacted by their treating clinician to request participation. Interviews were completed for 80 of these case probands and 343 of their first-degree relatives. Control probands were identified by a random-digit dialing procedure, conducted by a survey research contractor (Battelle Corp, Baltimore), which individually matched control to case probands on sex, race, and age (within 10 years) and on whether medical care had been received in the preceding 1 year. Recruitment staff then contacted the control probands and their first-degree relatives. Interviews were completed for 73 control probands and 300 of their first-degree relatives.Case probands were included if they met DSM-IVcriteria for OCD as diagnosed by the consensus procedure. They also needed to score greater than 15 on the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scaleduring the worst episode. The protocol excluded probands diagnosed with schizophrenia, mental retardation, dementia, or Tourette disorder, or if OCD occurred exclusively during a major depressive episode. Control probands also were excluded if any of the above diagnoses were present. In addition, if no member of the family other than the proband consented to participate, that family was excluded.DIAGNOSTIC PROCEDURESAfter informed consent was obtained, the subjects were interviewed by clinicians (psychiatrists or doctorate-level clinical psychologists) who had undergone extensive training in the diagnostic procedure. Most interviews were conducted in person in the research offices or subjects' homes, at locations across the United States. Interviews were audiotaped for training, quality control, reliability, and diagnostic consensus purposes. Interviewers were blinded as to whether subjects were probands or relatives or whether they were from case or control families. In addition to conducting direct interviews, the clinicians contacted a knowledgeable informant for each subject to obtain collateral information for diagnostic evaluations. Forty-seven direct interviews could not be conducted in person; these were conducted by telephone. All probands were interviewed in person. One hundred twenty-two case relatives (36%) and 69 control relatives (23%) were not available for interview, because they were deceased, refused to participate, or the proband refused to allow contact; for these subjects, informant interviews were conducted with 2 individuals familiar with the subject. Children aged 8 to 15 years were evaluated using similar procedures, except that personality was not assessed, and their interviews were conducted by clinicians who had experience interviewing children.Diagnostic instruments included the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia–Lifetime Anxiety,which was adapted for this study. The OCD section was elaborated to include the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale,as well as additional material on the course of symptoms. Sections on Tourette disorder, tic disorders, and impulse control disorders also were added. The Family Informant Schedule and Criteriawas adapted for interviewing collateral informants. The Structured Instrument for the Diagnosis of Personality Disorderswas employed for the assessment of personality disorders. The Revised NEO Personality Inventoryand Leyton Obsessional Inventorywere used as self-report measures of normal personality and obsessional features, respectively. Children were evaluated in a parallel procedure using comparable instruments (the Kiddie-Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia,the Children's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale,and Leyton Obsessional Inventory–Child Version).For every subject, a DSM-IVdiagnostic assignment form was completed; this form was designed to record the presence of all necessary diagnostic criteria and guide the assignment of diagnoses according to DSM-IVcriteria.The interrater reliability of the examiners' diagnostic procedure was studied in a sample of 30 subjects. There was good diagnostic agreement, as evidenced by a κ=0.81 for OCD and κ=1.0 for major depression; the intraclass correlation coefficient was 0.78 for compulsive personality disorder traits.All available diagnostic materials (evaluation by the clinical examiner, informant interview, clinical case summary, medical records, and audiotapes) were reviewed independently by 2 expert psychiatrists (G.N., M.R., J.B., R.H-S., M.G., or J.W.). The diagnosticians were blind as to whether the subject was a proband or relative and whether they were from a case or control family. All psychiatric diagnoses were made according to strict DSM-IVcriteria.If all the criteria required for having the disorder were met, then a "definite" diagnosis was given. If any necessary criterion was absent, then the diagnosis was considered "not present." If it seemed likely that the subject had the diagnosis, but the diagnosticians could not be certain of a given criterion (required for definite diagnosis), then the diagnosis was made at the "probable" level. If the diagnosticians could not be sure of the presence or absence of a given diagnosis, then that diagnosis was recorded as "unknown." Obsessions and compulsions (but not OCD) were diagnosed if these symptoms were present but did not meet the impairment criteria required for OCD.DATA ANALYSISSubjects with unknown diagnoses for the phenotypes of main interest in this article (OCD, obsessions, and compulsions) were excluded from both the numerator and the denominator in the analyses. Cases with probable diagnoses were treated in 2 ways: (1) when analyzing definite outcomes, these cases were considered as not present and excluded from the numerator but included in the denominator and (2) when analyzing definite and probable outcomes, these cases were considered as probable and included in the numerator and denominator. Although not reported here, we also reanalyzed the data considering the probable cases as "missing" and excluding them from both numerator and denominator; the magnitudes of the estimated odds ratios (ORs) were marginally stronger than the first method and considerably stronger than the second method described above.Demographic characteristics in case and control probands and case and control relatives, were compared with χ2or Fisher exact tests for categorical dataand tor median tests for continuous data. All tests were 2-tailed, with α=.05. The odds of OCD, obsessions, and compulsions in case vs control relatives were estimated using logistic regression by the method of generalized estimating equations, which accounts for within-family correlation among relatives.Specifically, ignoring within-family correlation may lead to substantially incorrect SE calculations for logistic regression coefficient estimates. This method provides correct SE estimates using conventional methods while acknowledging that part of the model (within-family correlation in this case) may be misspecified. Potential confounding factors (age of relative, sex of relative, and type of interview [direct or informant-only&rsqb;) were controlled by including terms for these variables in the regression models.RESULTSCHARACTERISTICS OF THE STUDY SAMPLEAs presented in Table 1, 53% of the case probands and 60% of the control probands were female. All but 1 proband in each group were white; 1 case and 1 control proband was Hispanic. Case and control probands were of similar ages. In the case probands, the age range was 18 to 88 years (median, 36 years; mean, 37 years); in the control probands, the age range was 18 to 79 years (median, 36 years; mean, 39 years). Both groups were highly educated. Most were college graduates, and 24% of the case probands and 22% of the control probands had graduate training. Despite these similarities, however, the case probands were significantly less likely to have ever married and, if married, to have children. The number of siblings in case-proband families (examined and not examined) (mean [SD&rsqb;, 2.1 [1.5&rsqb; siblings) was not significantly different than control proband families (mean [SD&rsqb;, 2.6 [1.8&rsqb; siblings) (t151=1.77; P=.08); however, case probands had significantly fewer children (for case probands: mean [SD&rsqb;, 0.5 [0.8&rsqb; children; for control probands: mean [SD&rsqb;, 1.3 [1.5&rsqb; children) (t151=4.25; P<.001).Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of Case and Control ProbandsCase Probands (n=80)Control Probands (n=73)Test Statistic*PSex, No. (%)Male38 (48)29 (40)χ21=0.94.33Female42 (53)44 (60)Mean (SD) age, y36.6 (11.6)38.5 (11.8)t151=0.98.33Schooling, No. (%)†College graduate53 (66)38 (52)High school graduate23 (29)30 (41)χ22=3.20.20Not a high school graduate4 (5)5 (7)Marital status, No. (%)Ever married42 (53)53 (73)χ21=6.55.01Never married38 (48)20 (27)Children, if ever married, No. (%)‡Yes27 (66)47 (89)χ21=7.19.007No14 (34)6 (11)*Degrees of freedom are presented as subscript values.†Using Bartholomew test for order.‡Unknown for one case proband.As presented in Table 2, case relatives were, on average, significantly older than control relatives; in case relatives, the age range was 9 to 94 years (median, 49 years; mean, 48 years); in control relatives, the age range was 8 to 91 years) (median, 43 years; mean, 45 years). There was a slight preponderance of females in the control relatives. The distribution of type of relative was significantly different between the 2 groups; children comprised a smaller proportion of case than control relatives. The mean (SD) number of first-degree relatives assessed (in addition to the proband) was 4.3 (1.8) for case families and 4.1 (2.2) for control families (t151=0.55; P=.59).Table 2. Demographic Characteristics of Case and Control First-Degree RelativesCase Relatives (n=343)Control Relatives (n=300)Test Statistic*PSex, No. (%)Male178 (51.9)135 (45.0)χ21=3.05.08Female165 (48.1)165 (55.0)Mean (SD) age, y48.1 (18.9)44.5 (18.8)t641=2.46.01Relatives interviewed, No. (%)Parents149 (43.5)108 (36.0)χ22=14.75.01Siblings163 (47.5)134 (44.7)Children31 (9.0)58 (19.3)*Degrees of freedom are shown as subscript values.PREVALENCE AND ODDS OF OBSESSIONS, COMPULSIONS, AND OCD IN RELATIVESAs presented in Table 3, the prevalence of definite OCD was substantially higher in case than control relatives (11.7% vs 2.7%). The prevalence of definite and probable OCD also was higher in case relatives (16.3% vs 5.7%). The prevalence of definite obsessions was 10.4% and 2.4% and 15.1% and 3.8% for definite and probable obsessions in the case and control relatives, respectively. Similar differences are found for the compulsions: 18% of the case relatives vs 6.1% of the control relatives were diagnosed with definite compulsions, and 25% of case relatives vs 12% of the control relatives had definite and probable compulsions.Table 3. Prevalence of Obsessions, Compulsions, and Obsessive-compulsive Disorder (OCD) in Relatives*PhenotypeCase Relatives Affected,† No. (%)Control Relatives Affected,‡ No. (%)Odds Ratio (95% CI)POCD (definite)38 (11.7)8 (2.7)4.7 (2.2-10.3).001OCD (definite and probable)53 (16.3)17 (5.7)3.1 (1.7-5.8).003Obsessions (definite)33 (10.4)7 (2.4)4.7 (2.0-10.8).003Obsessions (definite and probable)48 (15.1)11 (3.8)4.6 (2.4-8.8)<.001Compulsions (definite)58 (18.2)18 (6.1)3.4 (1.8-6.3)<.001Compulsions (definite and probable)79 (24.8)36 (12.2)2.4 (1.5-3.7)<.002*CI indicates confidence interval.†The number of case relatives with nonmissing diagnostic information for OCD was 326; for obsessions and for compulsions, 318.‡The number of control relatives with nonmissing diagnostic information for OCD was 297; for obsessions, 293; and for compulsions, 294.The odds of definite OCD is nearly 5 times greater in case relatives than control relatives (OR, 4.7; 95% confidence interval, 2.2-10.3) (P=.001) (Table 3). In addition, significant ORs were found for all definitions of the affected phenotype, indicating that first-degree relatives of cases met criteria for OCD-related phenotypes significantly more often than first-degree relatives of controls. In general, using more stringent criteria for affected status (ie, definite as opposed to definite and probable) resulted in larger ORs. Although there is a lower frequency of obsessions than compulsions in both case and control relatives, the magnitude of the association for obsessions is larger than that for compulsions.As described earlier, case and control relatives were different with respect to age (case relatives were older), sex of relatives interviewed (a larger proportion of case relatives were male), and the type of interview conducted (a greater proportion of case relatives had informant-only interviews). Moreover, the prevalence of OCD was inversely related to current age and was greater in relatives who were directly interviewed than in those who had informant-only interviews (data not shown). We therefore controlled for these potential confounding variables in the regression models. The OR of OCD was higher after adjusting for these factors (Table 4); adjustment also increased the ORs for obsessions and compulsions (data not shown).Table 4. Odds Ratios for Definite Obsessive-compulsive Disorder (OCD) in Relatives Using a Multivariable Model*Characteristic of RelativeOdds Ratio (95% CI)PGroupCase5.5 (2.6-11.8)<.001Control†1.0SexMale1.4 (0.8-2.7).26Female†1.0Type of interviewDirect2.6 (0.9-7.5).08Informant-only†1.0Age at interview (per year increase)0.98 (0.97-1.00).04*Determined using logistic regression by the method of generalized estimating equation. CI indicates confidence interval.†Reference group.PREVALENCE OF OCD IN CASE RELATIVES BY PROBAND CLINICAL CHARACTERISTICSThe age at onset of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in the 80 case probands ranged from 5 to 41 years. As shown in Figure 1, the median age at onset of symptoms was about 11 years; more than 75% of the probands had onset by age 14 years, and 90% by age 17 years. The mean (SD) age at onset in males (11.5 [5.6&rsqb; years; range, 5-30 years) was similar to that in females (12.0 [7.4&rsqb; years; range, 5-41 years) (t78=0.32; P=.75).Age at onset of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in case probands (N=80).Age at onset in case probands was dichotomized into early onset (5-17 years) and late onset (18-41 years) groups. The prevalence of OCD in the relatives of probands with early vs late onset was 38 (13.8%) of 276 and 0 (0%) of 49, respectively (P=.006 by Fisher exact test); thus, no cases of OCD were found in the case relatives of probands with late onset of obsessive-compulsive symptoms. In addition, the association between age at onset of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in the case probands and the prevalence of definite OCD in their relatives was examined by including this characteristic as a quantitative variable in a generalized estimating equation model. There was a significant inverse relationship between age at onset and the odds of OCD (OR, 0.92; 95% confidence interval, 0.85-0.99) (P=.02) (Table 5).Table 5. Odds Ratios for Definite Obsessive-compulsive Disorder (OCD) in Case Relatives by Proband Clinical Characteristics*Proband CharacteristicRelatives Affected, No. (%)Odds Ratio (95% CI)†PTics‡Present2 (6.7)0.73 (0.4-1.4).33Absent§35 (12.3)1.00OCPD&par;Present10 (11.0)0.90 (0.3-2.4).83Absent§25 (13.1)1.00Age at onset of OCD symptoms (per year increase)0.92 (0.85-0.99).05*CI indicates confidence interval; OCPD, obsessive-compulsive personality disorder.†Determined using logistic regression by the method of generalized estimating equation and controlling for relatives' age, sex, and type of interview.‡The number of relatives of probands with tics was 30; without tics, 284.§Reference group.&par;The number of relatives of probands with OCPD was 91; without OCPD, 191.Tics (single or multiple vocal or motor tics) were diagnosed in 8 (10.4%) of 77 case probands but no control probands (P=.008 by Fisher exact test). The prevalence of definite OCD was nonsignificantly lower in the relatives of probands with tics (6.7%) compared with the relative of probands without tics (12.3%) (P=.33). The prevalence of definite OCD was similar in relatives of case probands with obsessive-compulsive personality disorder (OCPD) (10 [11%&rsqb; of 91) and without OCPD (25 [13.1%&rsqb; of 191) (P=.83) (Table 5).COMMENTThis study reports 3 main findings. First, OCD is familial. The first-degree relatives of probands diagnosed with OCD had a nearly 5-fold higher lifetime prevalence of OCD. This finding is more robust for a definite diagnosis of OCD (ie, meeting all the DSM-IVcriteria); however, the results are significant if probable diagnoses are included. The difference in rates between case and control relatives should be considered from the vantage point of prevalence rates of OCD in the general population; the estimated lifetime prevalence rate in the control relatives was consistent with those in published epidemiological studies.Our findings are generally in accord with the most recent family study of OCD.Second, obsessions are more specific to the familial aspect of the disorder than are compulsions. Relatives of case probands are at greater risk for both obsessions and compulsions than the relatives of controls; however, the magnitude of the association is substantially stronger for obsessions than compulsions. It should be noted that the frequency of compulsions in both case and control relatives is relatively high. This may indicate substantial heterogeneity for compulsions (ie, they may be, in some instances, innocuous behaviors unrelated to an OCD diathesis, or they may be difficult to distinguish clinically from behaviors that are similar in form). These latter behaviors may be associated with other medical conditions and ought not be classified in the same group as compulsions. If there is heterogeneity, then compulsions alone perhaps should not be included in the phenotype for studies of OCD. Additionally, the specification for the definition of compulsions in the DSM-IVmay benefit from refinement.Third, earlier age at onset of the symptoms of OCD indicates a more familial subgroup. The relationship between younger ages at onset and familiality of medical conditions is a well-recognized phenomenon.This finding has been reported previously for OCD,and the association was extremely significant in our sample; indeed, we found no occurrence of OCD in the relatives of probands who had an age at onset of OCD older than 17 years. This suggests that cases of OCD with early age at onset are more likely to yield information about the genetic origins of this disorder. Conversely, cases with older ages at onset, whose numbers are not inconsequential, may have different origins.There has been a long-standing clinical impression that OCD symptoms may emerge as neurotic manifestations in individuals with OCPD.We hypothesized that the presence of OCPD distinguishes a subgroup of cases of OCD with a higher risk of OCD in their relatives. However, the results do not substantiate this hypothesis: there was no difference in the familial risk of OCD based on the presence of OCPD in the case probands.Previous studies have found that OCD occurs more frequently in the families of those with Tourette disorder, and it has been suggested that the presence of comorbid tics defines a familial subtype of OCD.The results of the current study do not support this suggestion, although we recognize that the overall recurrence risk in families may be similar even if there are subtypes with different origins. Tics may be heterogeneous, with some related to OCD and some not. Because we excluded probands with Tourette disorder from the study sample, we may have excluded families with a tic-related subtype of OCD.We acknowledge several methodologic limitations of our study. First, all case probands were in treatment. A community sample of OCD cases would have been more desirable; however, given our own experience in epidemiological studies of OCD,we would have had to screen thousands of subjects to obtain a sufficient number of OCD probands to conduct this study. To minimize selection bias, subjects were rostered and randomly selected from each of the 5 treatment centers. In the sampling frame, we included all probands who presented for treatment during a specified period; subjects who dropped out of treatment were as eligible for selection as those who did not.Second, only adult probands were selected. This may have reduced the estimate of familial risk, given that our results indicate that younger age at onset is associated with greater familial risk. In addition, few Hispanic and no African American subjects participated in the study. The low frequency of African American cases in the participating centers is compatible with the clinical experience of other centers and with epidemiologic studies.Third, control probands may have agreed to participate based on their personal or family concerns about emotional difficulties. Although unaware that this was a study of OCD, control probands knew they were being asked to participate in a family study of psychological difficulties. The effect of such a bias would be to reduce the likelihood of differences in rates of psychopathology between case and control relatives, making comparisons between the 2 groups more conservative. Furthermore, the control probands were identified by a stringent sampling method within the community in which the case probands resided. Every effort was made to ensure that the control probands were similar to the cases in all respects other than the presence of a diagnosis of OCD. Thus, control relatives were not a "supernormal" control group but were representative of persons residing in the communities in which the cases lived.Fourth, not all the interviews in the study were direct and in person. We conducted most interviews in subjects' homes; however, in a minority of cases, a telephone interview was unavoidable. We maintained the same protocol for telephone interviews, including audiotaping and accessing informants; the rates of disorders did not differ between the in-person and telephone interviews. Some relatives could not be interviewed at all because of death or refusal to participate. We strove to include these relatives by conducting family informant interviews with 2 individuals knowledgeable about the subject in each instance. Overall, 71% of the relatives were directly interviewed.The finding that OCD is familial is a necessary, albeit not sufficient, condition for genetic etiology. The estimated λ (ie, the ratio of the rates of definite OCD in case and control relatives, respectively) of 4.33 is within the range of that for other psychiatric disorders, such as bipolar disorder and panic disorder, although substantially lower than others, such as autism.The results, together with those from previous twin studies, encourage a molecular genetic approach to identify a genetic etiology for OCD. Clarification of the range of psychopathology that is transmitted in these families, as well as identification of etiologically homogeneous clinical subtypes, will increase the ability of association studies and genome-wide searches to detect genes involved in the pathogenesis of OCD.GWestphahlÜber zwangsvorstellungen.Archiv Psychiatr Nervenkrankheiten.1878;8:734-750.MKarnoJMGoldingObsessive compulsive disorder.In: Robins LN, Regier DA, eds. Psychiatric Disorders in America. New York, NY: Free Press; 1991:204-219.MMWeissmanRCBlandGJCaninoSGreenwaldHGHwuCKyoonSCNewmanMAOakley-BrowneMRubio-StipecPJWickramaratneHUWittchenYEng-Kungfor the Cross National Collaborative GroupThe cross national epidemiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder.J Clin Psychiatry.1994;55(suppl):S5-S10.GNestadtJFSamuelsAJRomanoskiMFFolsteinPRMcHughObsessions and compulsions in the community.Acta Psychiatr Scand.1994;89:219-224.GNestadtOJBienvenuGCaiJSamuelsWWEatonIncidence of obsessive-compulsive disorder in adults.J Nerv Ment Dis.1998;186:401-406.CLMurrayADLopezThe Global Burden of Disease: A Comprehensive Assessment of Mortality and Disability From Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors in 1990 and Projected.Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press; 1996.ALewisProblems of obsessional illness.Proc R Soc Med.1935;29:325-336.RWoodruffFMPittsMonozygotic twins with obsessional illness.Am J Psychiatry.1964;120:1075-1080.IMMarksMCroweEDreweJYoungWGDewhurstObsessive-compulsive neurosis in identical twins.Br J Psychiatry.1969;115:991-998.PMcGuffinDMawsonObsessive-compulsive neurosis: two identical twin pairs.Br J Psychiatry.1980;137:285-287.EMCryanGJButcherMGWebbObsessive-compulsive disorder and paraphilia in a monozygotic twin pair.Br J Psychiatry.1992;161:694-698.EInyoueSimilar and dissimilar manifestations of obsessive-compulsive neurosis in monozygotic twins.Am J Psychiatry.1965;121:1171-1175.GCareyIIGottesmanTwin and family studies of anxiety, phobic, and obsessive disorders.In: Klein DF, Rabkin JG, eds. Anxiety: New Research and Changing Concepts. New York, NY: Raven Press; 1981:117-136.MCLenaneSESwedoHLeonardDLPaulsWSceeryJLRapoportPsychiatric disorders in first-degree relatives of children and adolescents with obsessive-compulsive disorder.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry.1990;29:407-412.MARiddleLScahillRKingMTHardinKETowbinSIOrtJFLeckmanDJCohenObsessive compulsive disorder in children and adolescents: phenomenology and family history.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry.1990;29:766-772.HLLeonardMCLenaneSESwedoDCRettewESGershonJLRapoportTics and Tourette's disorder: a 2- to 7-year followup of 54 obsessive-compulsive children.Am J Psychiatry.1992;149:1244-1251.LBellodiGSciutoGDiaferiaPRonchiESmeraldiPsychiatric disorders in the families of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder.Psychiatry Res.1992;42:111-120.DLPaulsJPAlsobrookWGoodmanSRasmussenJFLeckmanA family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder.Am J Psychiatry.1995;152:76-84.DWBlackRNoyes JrRBGoldsteinNBlumA family study of obsessive-compulsive disorder.Arch Gen Psychiatry.1992;49:362-368.WKGoodmanLHPriceSARasmussenCMazureRLFleischmannCLHillGRHeningerDSCharneyThe Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale, I: development, use, and reliability.Arch Gen Psychiatry.1989;46:1006-1011.SMannuzzaAJFyerDFKleinJEndicottSchedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia–Lifetime Version Modified for the Study of Anxiety Disorders (SADS-LA): rationale and conceptual development.J Psychiatr Res.1986;20:317-325.SMannuzzaAJFyerJEndicottDFKleinFamily Informant Schedule and Criteria.New York, NY: Anxiety Family Study Unit, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1985.BPfohlNBlumMZimmermanDStanglStructured Interview for DSM-III-R Personality–Revised (SIDP-R).Iowa City: University of Iowa College of Medicine; 1989.PTCosta JrRRMcCraeRevised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) Professional Manual.Odessa, Fla: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1992.JCooperThe Leyton Obsessional Inventory.Psychol Med.1970;1:48-64.HOrvaschelJPuig-AntichSchedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Epidemiologic Version (K-SADS-E).Pittsburgh, Pa: Western Psychiatric Institute and Clinic; 1987.LScahillMARiddleMMcSwiggin-HardinSIOrtRAKingWKGoodmanDCicchettiJFLeckmanChildren's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: reliability and validity.J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry.1997;36:844-852.CJBergJLRapoportMFlamentThe Leyton Obsessional Inventory–Child Version.J Am Acad Child Psychiatry.1986;25:84-91.American Psychiatric AssociationDiagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition.Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.JLFleissStatistical Methods for Rates and Proportions.New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 1973.KYLiangAEPulverAnalysis of case-control/family sampling design.Genet Epidemiol.1996;13:253-270.BChildsCRScriverAge at onset and causes of disease.Perspect Biol Med.1986;29(3, pt 1):437-460.JSandlerAHazariThe obsessional: on the psychological classification of obsessional character traits and symptoms.Br J Med Psychol.1960;33:113-122.DLPaulsJFLeckmanThe inheritance of Gilles de la Tourette's syndrome and associated behaviors.N Engl J Med.1986;315:993-997.NIMH Genetics WorkgroupGenetics and Mental Disorders.Washington, DC: National Institute of Mental Health; 1997.Accepted for publication December 17, 1999.Our research was supported by grant R01 MH50214 from the National Institute of Mental Health (Dr Nestadt), and by grant RR00052 from the Division of Research Resources General Clinical Reseach Centers, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Md.We thank the many families participating in the study; Jennifer Hahn, PhD, David Wellen, PhD, Ruth Zitner, PsyD, Dan McLeod, PhD, Malgorzata Lamacz, PhD, and Bernadette Cullen, MD, for conducting clinical evaluations; Dennis Murphy, MD, Ben Greenberg, MD, PhD, Sally Winston, PsyD, Donna Burns, and Dorinda Schultz, for providing access to their clinic patients; David Pauls, PhD, Kathleen Merikangas, PhD, Abby Fyer, MD, and Salvatore Mannuzza, PhD, for consultation on this study; and Dorothy Carpenter, Krista Vermillion, Margaret Dees, and Sandra Hensley, for their efforts in coordinating the study.Reprints: Gerald Nestadt, MD, MPH, Johns Hopkins Hospital, Meyer 4-181, 600 N Wolfe St, Baltimore, MD 21287-7228 (e-mail: gnestadt@mail.jhmi.edu).

Journal

JAMA PsychiatryAmerican Medical Association

Published: Apr 1, 2000

There are no references for this article.