TY - JOUR AU1 - Sini, Siltanen, AU2 - Erja, Portegijs, AU3 - Milla, Saajanaho, AU4 - Taina, Poranen-Clark, AU5 - Anne, Viljanen, AU6 - Merja, Rantakokko, AU7 - Taina, Rantanen, AB - Abstract Background We studied the combined effects of cognitive performance and lower extremity function on self-reported walking modifications and walking difficulty and on self-reported walking difficulty incidence over a 2-year follow-up. Methods A total of 848 community-dwelling older people aged 75–90 years participated at baseline, 816 at the 1-year follow-up, and 761 at the 2-year follow-up. Baseline lower extremity function was measured with the Short Physical Performance Battery (<10 vs. ≥10) and cognitive performance with the Mini–Mental State Examination (<24 vs. ≥24). Difficulty in walking 2 km was self-reported and categorized into no difficulties, no difficulties but walking modifications, and prevalent difficulties. Data were analyzed with multinomial and Cox regressions and a mediation analysis. Results At baseline, 33% reported no walking difficulties, 25% walking modifications, and 42% walking difficulty. Poorer lower extremity function and lower cognition increased the odds for walking difficulty. For those with both, the odds were almost eightfold higher for walking difficulty and threefold higher for walking modifications compared with having neither. Poorer lower extremity function mediated the association between low cognition and poorer perceived walking ability. Of those with no walking difficulty at baseline, 31% developed walking difficulty during the follow-up, the risk being almost twofold higher among those with poorer lower extremity function at baseline (hazard ratio = 1.82, 95% confidence interval = 1.28–2.59). Conclusion Older people with poorer lower extremity function and cognitive performance are likely to have walking difficulties, rendering them especially vulnerable to further disability. Cognitive performance should be considered in interventions aimed at preventing mobility disability. Cognition, Physical function, Physical performance, Mobility limitation Walking ability is important for accessing community amenities (1) and for maintaining independence in old age (2). Self-report measures of walking provide information about one’s mobility in the everyday environment and usually express the degree of difficulty perceived when walking a specific distance (1). Perceived walking ability relates closely to actual walking behavior (1,3). Self-report measures may also identify people who modify their walking, that is, change their way of walking (4). Modifications in walking, such as slowing down or pausing for rest during performance, may indicate declining functional capacity even in the absence of frank walking difficulty (4). People with walking modifications represent an intermediate level of functional capacity when compared with those reporting walking difficulty and with those reporting no walking difficulty and no modifications (4,5). However, modifications also denote adaptive compensatory practices, as these may help older people to maintain their life-space mobility and participation in out-of-home activities regardless of their physical decline (6). Pathology or aging may cause impairments in physical and cognitive capacity (7). Moreover, it has been pointed out that decline in physical capacity often co-occurs with decline in cognition (8). Three recent systematic reviews and meta-analyses reported on the associations of indicators of mobility with measures of cognition (9–11). Older people with poorer physical performance do worse in cognitive tests (9), and vice versa (10), and are at risk for developing dementia (11). This may stem from a common biologic aging process, which manifests as deficits in both physical and cognitive performance (9). Furthermore, it has been suggested that deficits may manifest first in physical performance as, at least in part, they are easier to observe than cognitive changes (12,13). The combined deterioration of physical and cognitive performance may underlie perceived walking modifications and walking difficulties in old age. Two international workshop reports have suggested that research on mobility should incorporate cognitive measures and that cognition and mobility should be regarded as a combined research entity (14,15). Nevertheless, to our knowledge, the cognitive and physical domains have not hitherto been studied together as predictors of perceived walking difficulties. The purpose of this study, therefore, was to investigate the combined associations of cognitive performance and lower extremity function with self-reported walking modifications and walking difficulty cross-sectionally and with incidence of perceived walking difficulty over a 2-year follow-up. In addition, we studied whether lower extremity function mediates the association between cognition and perceived walking ability. Methods Study Design This study forms part of the Life-Space Mobility in Old Age (LISPE), which was a 2-year (2012–2014) prospective cohort study targeting community-dwelling older people conducted at the University of Jyväskylä, Finland. Participants were recruited from a random sample of 2,550 people aged 75–90 years drawn from the national population register and living in the municipalities of Jyväskylä and Muurame in Central Finland. Of this number, 848 persons were eligible (living independently and able to communicate) and willing to participate in the at-home personal interview (16). Of these, 816 participated in the 1-year follow-up and 761 in the 2-year follow-up implemented by phone. Reasons for dropout over the 2-year period were death (n = 41), institutionalization (n = 15), impaired ability to communicate (n = 12), moving outside the study area (n = 6), declined health (n = 5), unwillingness to continue (n = 6), and not being reached (n = 2) (17). The Ethical Committee of the University of Jyväskylä approved the LISPE study. The study protocol followed the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. Participants were informed about the study, and each participant signed an informed consent before the assessments. Variables Perceived walking difficulty and walking modifications Participants were asked if they perceived difficulties in walking 2 km (2,4) at baseline and at the 1- and 2-year follow-ups. The response options were as follows: “able without difficulty,” “able with some difficulty,” “able with a great deal of difficulty,” “unable without the help of another person,” and “unable to manage even with help.” Those who reported being able to manage without any difficulty were asked whether they have modified their way of walking (4). The question was “Have you noticed any of the following changes in walking 2 km?” The response options were yes/no, and the items were decreased walking frequency, given up walking the distance, use of an assistive device, slower walking pace, and pausing for rest during the performance. Participants were categorized as follows: (a) no walking difficulties (reporting neither difficulty nor modifications), (b) walking modifications (reporting no walking difficulty but at least one modification), and (c) walking difficulties (reporting some or a great deal of difficulties or being unable to perform). Lower extremity function Lower extremity function was assessed at baseline with the Short Physical Performance Battery (SPPB) (18). The battery includes tests for standing balance (feet together, semi-tandem, full tandem), walking (normal gait speed for 2.44 m), and chair rise (five times). Each task was scored from 0 to 4, yielding a sum score of between 0 and 12 points (4), with higher scores indicating better performance. The sum score was calculated and scaled if at least two of the three tests were completed (19). In total, nine participants (1.0%) did not complete the tests due to refusal (n = 4), being in a wheelchair (n = 4), or a proscription from a doctor (n = 1). The sum scores were not normally distributed and thus were categorized. Participants with an SPPB score of 10 or higher (n = 529) were considered as having good physical performance and formed the reference group. Because only few participants had very low scores (<4 points, n = 28), no further categorization was possible, and hence, those with a score less than 10 were assigned to the category of poorer lower extremity function (n = 310) (20). Cognitive performance Cognitive performance was assessed at baseline with the Mini–Mental State Examination (MMSE), which is a brief screening test for cognitive impairment (21). It consists of 11 items measuring orientation, registration, attention, calculation, recall, and language. The maximum sum score is 30, higher scores indicating better performance. The sum score was scaled if the respondent was not able to perform all the test tasks, for example, due to visual impairment. Because the MMSE scores were not normally distributed in this study, we used the established cut point of 24 (22) to identify the participants with cognitive decline (MMSE < 24, n = 150). The remainder formed the reference category (MMSE ≥ 24, n = 698). Covariates Age and sex were drawn from the national population register, and year of education was self-reported. Morbidity was evaluated as the number of physician-diagnosed self-reported chronic conditions. A list of 22 diseases was presented, followed by an open-ended question asking about other conditions (19). Depressive symptoms were assessed with the 20-item Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) (23). Vision was assessed subjectively with the question: “How well can you see from a distance?” The response options were dichotomized into (a) good (those who answered “well” and “reasonably well”) and (b) declined (those who answered “poorly”) (24). Statistical Analyses Means, SD, and percentages were used to describe the participants’ baseline characteristics, and crude differences were tested with the chi-square test and one-way analysis of variance. The cross-sectional odds for walking modifications or walking difficulties versus no difficulty were calculated with multinomial regression analyses. The preliminary regression analyses revealed an interaction between the MMSE and SPPB scores and walking modifications and difficulties (p < .001). Therefore, we first added the SPPB and MMSE into the models separately and thereafter together to assess the combined influences of poorer lower extremity function and lower cognitive performance. All models were first adjusted for age and sex and then additionally for years of education, number of chronic conditions, depressive symptoms, and vision. The model including only cognition was adjusted for lower extremity performance, and vice versa. In addition, a latent factor mediator analysis was conducted to assess whether SPPB mediated the association between MMSE and walking ability at baseline. The mediation analysis was adjusted for age and sex. The incidence of walking modifications was too low for meaningful analysis. Thus, we used Cox regression to analyze the relative risk for incident walking difficulties among those who did not report walking difficulties at baseline (n = 492). People were censored at the time they reported walking difficulty or at the end of the follow-up, whichever happened first. Those who had missing data on walking ability in one or the other of the assessments were categorized and censored according to the answer that was available. In addition, the Cox regression analysis was stratified according to baseline modifications. Finally, Little’s Missing Completely at Random (MCAR) test was used to analyze study attrition. All analyses were performed with SPSS Statistics 24 for Windows except for the mediation analysis, where MPlus version 5.21 was used. Results Mean baseline age was 80.6 (SD = 4.3), and 62% of the participants were women. At baseline, 33% (n = 280) reported no walking difficulties, 25% (n = 212) reported walking modifications, and 42% (n = 356) reported walking difficulties. Those without walking difficulties had the best cognitive performance and lower extremity function, whereas those with walking difficulties had the poorest. Those reporting walking modifications had intermediate scores (Table 1). In addition, those with walking difficulties were older, less educated, more often women, and had more chronic conditions, depressive symptoms, and more often poor vision (p ≤ .005 for all) than those reporting no walking difficulties. These variables were chosen as covariates. The baseline characteristics according to lower extremity function and cognitive performance categories are presented in Supplementary Material. Table 1. Baseline Characteristics of the Participants According to Walking Ability Category (N = 848) No Walking Difficulties Walking Modifications Walking Difficulties Characteristics n = 280 n = 212 n = 356 Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p Value Age 78.4 (3.7) 79.8 (4.3) 81.7 (4.1) <.001a Years of education 10.3 (4.5) 9.9 (3.7) 8.8 (4.0) <.001a Number of chronic conditions 3.2 (2.0) 4.2 (2.3) 5.4 (2.4) <.001a CES-D score 7.3 (5.7) 9.5 (6.0) 11.6 (7.5) <.001a MMSE score 26.6 (2.5) 26.1 (2.8) 25.8 (3.0) .003a SPPB score 10.8 (1.4) 10.1 (1.9) 8.4 (3.0) <.001a Sex (female, %) 53.9 58.5 70.5 <.001b Vision (poor, %) 1.4 2.8 6.2 .005b No Walking Difficulties Walking Modifications Walking Difficulties Characteristics n = 280 n = 212 n = 356 Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p Value Age 78.4 (3.7) 79.8 (4.3) 81.7 (4.1) <.001a Years of education 10.3 (4.5) 9.9 (3.7) 8.8 (4.0) <.001a Number of chronic conditions 3.2 (2.0) 4.2 (2.3) 5.4 (2.4) <.001a CES-D score 7.3 (5.7) 9.5 (6.0) 11.6 (7.5) <.001a MMSE score 26.6 (2.5) 26.1 (2.8) 25.8 (3.0) .003a SPPB score 10.8 (1.4) 10.1 (1.9) 8.4 (3.0) <.001a Sex (female, %) 53.9 58.5 70.5 <.001b Vision (poor, %) 1.4 2.8 6.2 .005b Note: CES-D = Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; MMSE = Mini–Mental state Examination; SPPB = Short Physical Performance Battery. aTested with one-way analysis of variance. bTested with chi-square test. View Large Table 1. Baseline Characteristics of the Participants According to Walking Ability Category (N = 848) No Walking Difficulties Walking Modifications Walking Difficulties Characteristics n = 280 n = 212 n = 356 Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p Value Age 78.4 (3.7) 79.8 (4.3) 81.7 (4.1) <.001a Years of education 10.3 (4.5) 9.9 (3.7) 8.8 (4.0) <.001a Number of chronic conditions 3.2 (2.0) 4.2 (2.3) 5.4 (2.4) <.001a CES-D score 7.3 (5.7) 9.5 (6.0) 11.6 (7.5) <.001a MMSE score 26.6 (2.5) 26.1 (2.8) 25.8 (3.0) .003a SPPB score 10.8 (1.4) 10.1 (1.9) 8.4 (3.0) <.001a Sex (female, %) 53.9 58.5 70.5 <.001b Vision (poor, %) 1.4 2.8 6.2 .005b No Walking Difficulties Walking Modifications Walking Difficulties Characteristics n = 280 n = 212 n = 356 Mean (SD) Mean (SD) Mean (SD) p Value Age 78.4 (3.7) 79.8 (4.3) 81.7 (4.1) <.001a Years of education 10.3 (4.5) 9.9 (3.7) 8.8 (4.0) <.001a Number of chronic conditions 3.2 (2.0) 4.2 (2.3) 5.4 (2.4) <.001a CES-D score 7.3 (5.7) 9.5 (6.0) 11.6 (7.5) <.001a MMSE score 26.6 (2.5) 26.1 (2.8) 25.8 (3.0) .003a SPPB score 10.8 (1.4) 10.1 (1.9) 8.4 (3.0) <.001a Sex (female, %) 53.9 58.5 70.5 <.001b Vision (poor, %) 1.4 2.8 6.2 .005b Note: CES-D = Centre for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale; MMSE = Mini–Mental state Examination; SPPB = Short Physical Performance Battery. aTested with one-way analysis of variance. bTested with chi-square test. View Large Table 2 shows that poor cognitive performance was not associated with walking modifications but increased the odds for walking difficulties at baseline. Poorer lower extremity function increased the odds for both walking modifications and walking difficulties. The highest odds for walking difficulties were observed among those with concurrent poorer lower extremity function and poor cognition. A parallel, but less pronounced association was seen for walking modifications. In addition, those with good lower extremity function but poor cognition were likely to have walking difficulties, but only when the model was adjusted for all the covariates. The mediation analysis showed that the association of higher cognitive capacity with better-perceived walking ability was mediated through better lower extremity function. The direct association between MMSE and perceived walking ability was not significant (see Figure 1). Table 2. Cross-sectional Analyses of Lower Extremity Function and Cognitive Performance Separately and Combined With Self-reported Walking Modifications and Walking Difficulties Among Community-Dwelling Older People Aged 75–90 Years at Baseline (n = 827) Walking Modifications Walking Difficulties Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) MMSE (<24 vs. ≥24) 1.62 (0.97–2.73) 1.73 (1.00–2.99) 1.57 (0.90–2.75)a 1.97 (1.22–3.17) 2.11 (1.25–3.57) 1.81 (1.05–3.14)a SPPB (<10 vs. ≥10) 2.39 (1.55–3.71) 2.07 (1.32–3.26) 2.02 (1.28–3.18)b 7.07 (4.71–10.62) 5.24 (3.39–8.10) 5.06 (3.26–7.84)b SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.50 (0.79–2.82) 1.71 (0.88–3.33) — 1.78 (0.95–3.35) 2.04 (1.03–4.05) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE ≥ 24c 2.39 (1.47–3.87) 2.13 (1.29–3.51) — 7.18 (4.58–11.25) 5.38 (3.32–8.73) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE < 24c 3.08 (1.26–7.53) 2.65 (1.05–6.69) — 9.70 (4.30–21.86) 7.67 (3.27–18.04) — Walking Modifications Walking Difficulties Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) MMSE (<24 vs. ≥24) 1.62 (0.97–2.73) 1.73 (1.00–2.99) 1.57 (0.90–2.75)a 1.97 (1.22–3.17) 2.11 (1.25–3.57) 1.81 (1.05–3.14)a SPPB (<10 vs. ≥10) 2.39 (1.55–3.71) 2.07 (1.32–3.26) 2.02 (1.28–3.18)b 7.07 (4.71–10.62) 5.24 (3.39–8.10) 5.06 (3.26–7.84)b SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.50 (0.79–2.82) 1.71 (0.88–3.33) — 1.78 (0.95–3.35) 2.04 (1.03–4.05) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE ≥ 24c 2.39 (1.47–3.87) 2.13 (1.29–3.51) — 7.18 (4.58–11.25) 5.38 (3.32–8.73) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE < 24c 3.08 (1.26–7.53) 2.65 (1.05–6.69) — 9.70 (4.30–21.86) 7.67 (3.27–18.04) — Notes: CI = confidence interval; OR = odds ratio; SPPB = Short Physical Performance Battery; MMSE = Mini–Mental State Examination. Reference category: no walking difficulties. Multinomial regression model 1: adjusted for age and sex. Model 2: adjusted for age, sex, years of education, number of chronic conditions, depressive symptoms, and vision. Model 3: adjusted for age, sex, years of education, number of chronic conditions, depressive symptoms, vision, and aSPPB, and bMMSE. cVersus SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE ≥ 24. Statistically significant values are bolded. View Large Table 2. Cross-sectional Analyses of Lower Extremity Function and Cognitive Performance Separately and Combined With Self-reported Walking Modifications and Walking Difficulties Among Community-Dwelling Older People Aged 75–90 Years at Baseline (n = 827) Walking Modifications Walking Difficulties Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) MMSE (<24 vs. ≥24) 1.62 (0.97–2.73) 1.73 (1.00–2.99) 1.57 (0.90–2.75)a 1.97 (1.22–3.17) 2.11 (1.25–3.57) 1.81 (1.05–3.14)a SPPB (<10 vs. ≥10) 2.39 (1.55–3.71) 2.07 (1.32–3.26) 2.02 (1.28–3.18)b 7.07 (4.71–10.62) 5.24 (3.39–8.10) 5.06 (3.26–7.84)b SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.50 (0.79–2.82) 1.71 (0.88–3.33) — 1.78 (0.95–3.35) 2.04 (1.03–4.05) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE ≥ 24c 2.39 (1.47–3.87) 2.13 (1.29–3.51) — 7.18 (4.58–11.25) 5.38 (3.32–8.73) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE < 24c 3.08 (1.26–7.53) 2.65 (1.05–6.69) — 9.70 (4.30–21.86) 7.67 (3.27–18.04) — Walking Modifications Walking Difficulties Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) OR (95% CI) MMSE (<24 vs. ≥24) 1.62 (0.97–2.73) 1.73 (1.00–2.99) 1.57 (0.90–2.75)a 1.97 (1.22–3.17) 2.11 (1.25–3.57) 1.81 (1.05–3.14)a SPPB (<10 vs. ≥10) 2.39 (1.55–3.71) 2.07 (1.32–3.26) 2.02 (1.28–3.18)b 7.07 (4.71–10.62) 5.24 (3.39–8.10) 5.06 (3.26–7.84)b SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.50 (0.79–2.82) 1.71 (0.88–3.33) — 1.78 (0.95–3.35) 2.04 (1.03–4.05) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE ≥ 24c 2.39 (1.47–3.87) 2.13 (1.29–3.51) — 7.18 (4.58–11.25) 5.38 (3.32–8.73) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE < 24c 3.08 (1.26–7.53) 2.65 (1.05–6.69) — 9.70 (4.30–21.86) 7.67 (3.27–18.04) — Notes: CI = confidence interval; OR = odds ratio; SPPB = Short Physical Performance Battery; MMSE = Mini–Mental State Examination. Reference category: no walking difficulties. Multinomial regression model 1: adjusted for age and sex. Model 2: adjusted for age, sex, years of education, number of chronic conditions, depressive symptoms, and vision. Model 3: adjusted for age, sex, years of education, number of chronic conditions, depressive symptoms, vision, and aSPPB, and bMMSE. cVersus SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE ≥ 24. Statistically significant values are bolded. View Large Figure 1. View largeDownload slide Unstandardized path coefficients with 95% confidence intervals of a cross-sectional, age- and sex-adjusted, latent factor mediator model for Mini–Mental State Examination (MMSE), Short Physical Performance Battery (SPPB), and perceived walking ability (Walking ability) among community-dwelling older participants (N = 848). Figure 1. View largeDownload slide Unstandardized path coefficients with 95% confidence intervals of a cross-sectional, age- and sex-adjusted, latent factor mediator model for Mini–Mental State Examination (MMSE), Short Physical Performance Battery (SPPB), and perceived walking ability (Walking ability) among community-dwelling older participants (N = 848). Of the 492 participants who did not report walking difficulties at baseline, 153 (31%) developed walking difficulty during the 2-year follow-up. Table 3 shows that poorer lower extremity function both alone and together with good cognition increased the risk for incident walking difficulty. When the analysis was stratified according to baseline walking modifications, those without walking modifications but with poorer lower extremity function and poor cognition were at risk of incident walking difficulty (hazard ratio = 2.75, 95% confidence interval = 1.01–4.17). Among those reporting walking modifications, the results remained nearly unchanged (data not shown). Study attrition was analyzed with Little’s MCAR test, which showed that missing data were not missing completely at random (χ2 = 9.756, df = 1, p = .002). Thus, it was assumed that the mobility of those who reported no walking difficulties at baseline but dropped out during the follow-up due to death or institutionalization (n = 22) would have declined. Therefore, we categorized them as having developed walking difficulty to determine whether that would alter the results. The results supported our main findings (data not shown). Table 3. Baseline Lower Extremity Function and Cognitive Performance as Separate and Combined Predictors of Walking Difficulty Incidence Over a 2-Year Follow-up Among Community-Dwelling Older People With No Walking Difficulties Regardless of Walking Modifications at Baseline (n = 492) Developing Walking Difficulty in 2 y Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 HR (95% CI) HR (95% CI) HR (95% CI) MMSE (<24 vs. ≥24) 1.28 (0.84–1.95) 1.28 (0.83–1.98) 1.06 (0.68–1.67)a SPPB (<10 vs. ≥10) 1.85 (1.32–2.60) 1.84 (1.30–2.60) 1.82 (1.28–2.59)b SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.47 (0.86–2.52) 1.59 (0.92–2.76) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE ≥ 24c 2.10 (1.45–3.06) 2.20 (1.50–3.22) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.50 (0.77–2.92) 1.38 (0.70–2.71) — Developing Walking Difficulty in 2 y Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 HR (95% CI) HR (95% CI) HR (95% CI) MMSE (<24 vs. ≥24) 1.28 (0.84–1.95) 1.28 (0.83–1.98) 1.06 (0.68–1.67)a SPPB (<10 vs. ≥10) 1.85 (1.32–2.60) 1.84 (1.30–2.60) 1.82 (1.28–2.59)b SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.47 (0.86–2.52) 1.59 (0.92–2.76) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE ≥ 24c 2.10 (1.45–3.06) 2.20 (1.50–3.22) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.50 (0.77–2.92) 1.38 (0.70–2.71) — Note: CI = confidence interval; HR = hazard ratio; SPPB = Short Physical Performance Battery; MMSE = Mini–Mental State Examination. Reference category: no walking difficulties. Cox regression model 1: adjusted for age and sex. Model 2: adjusted for age, sex, years of education, number of chronic conditions, depressive symptoms, and vision. Model 3: adjusted for age, sex, years of education, number of chronic conditions, depressive symptoms, vision, and aSPPB, and bMMSE. cVersus SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE ≥ 24. View Large Table 3. Baseline Lower Extremity Function and Cognitive Performance as Separate and Combined Predictors of Walking Difficulty Incidence Over a 2-Year Follow-up Among Community-Dwelling Older People With No Walking Difficulties Regardless of Walking Modifications at Baseline (n = 492) Developing Walking Difficulty in 2 y Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 HR (95% CI) HR (95% CI) HR (95% CI) MMSE (<24 vs. ≥24) 1.28 (0.84–1.95) 1.28 (0.83–1.98) 1.06 (0.68–1.67)a SPPB (<10 vs. ≥10) 1.85 (1.32–2.60) 1.84 (1.30–2.60) 1.82 (1.28–2.59)b SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.47 (0.86–2.52) 1.59 (0.92–2.76) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE ≥ 24c 2.10 (1.45–3.06) 2.20 (1.50–3.22) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.50 (0.77–2.92) 1.38 (0.70–2.71) — Developing Walking Difficulty in 2 y Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 HR (95% CI) HR (95% CI) HR (95% CI) MMSE (<24 vs. ≥24) 1.28 (0.84–1.95) 1.28 (0.83–1.98) 1.06 (0.68–1.67)a SPPB (<10 vs. ≥10) 1.85 (1.32–2.60) 1.84 (1.30–2.60) 1.82 (1.28–2.59)b SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.47 (0.86–2.52) 1.59 (0.92–2.76) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE ≥ 24c 2.10 (1.45–3.06) 2.20 (1.50–3.22) — SPPB < 10 and MMSE < 24c 1.50 (0.77–2.92) 1.38 (0.70–2.71) — Note: CI = confidence interval; HR = hazard ratio; SPPB = Short Physical Performance Battery; MMSE = Mini–Mental State Examination. Reference category: no walking difficulties. Cox regression model 1: adjusted for age and sex. Model 2: adjusted for age, sex, years of education, number of chronic conditions, depressive symptoms, and vision. Model 3: adjusted for age, sex, years of education, number of chronic conditions, depressive symptoms, vision, and aSPPB, and bMMSE. cVersus SPPB ≥ 10 and MMSE ≥ 24. View Large Discussion The findings of this study suggest that older people with both poorer lower extremity function and low cognitive performance are more likely to have walking difficulties than those with neither or only the other, making them especially vulnerable to further disability. Poor lower extremity function as a risk factor for mobility modifications and disability has been reported earlier (4,7,18). Our results expand those findings by showing that the odds for reporting walking modifications or difficulties were highest among those with poorer lower extremity function and poor cognitive performance. The results are in line with previous findings on the association between mobility limitations and cognitive decline (9,10) but extend them by showing that the association is evident already in the early phase of mobility decline. Recently, reports have shown that cognitive decline, together with slowed gait, in older people without dementia or mobility disability (25) increases the risk for dementia (25,26), mortality (27), and falls (28) when compared with people without this combination. In addition, the combination of objectively measured cognitive decline and mobility limitations has been found to increase the risk for institutionalization (29). Our cross-sectional results are in line with these studies but extend them for walking difficulties and support the view that studying mobility and cognition together improves the accuracy of risk prediction (9,14,15,26,30). Our longitudinal results, however, were only partially parallel to those obtained in cross-sectional analyses, and most of the differences centered on cognitive capacity. Neither poor cognitive functioning nor poorer lower extremity function in the presence of low cognitive capacity was associated with increased risk for new walking difficulties. Previous studies have found that persons with more severe cognitive decline may under-report difficulties in everyday functioning (31). Our results indirectly supported this by showing an increased risk for walking difficulty solely among those with poorer lower extremity function and high MMSE score at baseline. Unfortunately, data were not available to examine whether those who initially had lower cognitive capacity exhibited further decline, which might underlie the absence of self-reported walking difficulties among them. Another explanation is the high prevalence of walking difficulties at baseline among those with poorer lower extremity function and low cognitive capacity. Of the 73 people in this category, 50 already had walking difficulties at baseline and were excluded from the prospective analyses. Consequently, we cannot rule out the possibility that the risk of incident walking difficulty is an underestimation in this group. We found that the association between cognition and walking difficulties was attenuated when the model was adjusted for lower extremity function. Furthermore, the mediation analysis showed that the association of lower cognition with poorer walking ability was mediated by poorer lower extremity function. Thus, our findings suggest that poorer lower extremity function partially explains the association between cognitive decline and walking difficulties. A recent study indicated that lower extremity function also partially explains the association between executive function, a higher order cognitive function, and life-space mobility, a correlate of self-reported 2-km walking ability (32,33). The strong associations of lower extremity function with walking difficulties and walking modifications may be explained by age-related losses in muscle strength and postural balance, which are biomechanical prerequisites for walking (1,34,35). We chose to study older people aged 75–90 years at baseline because that is usually the time of life when people start to experience decline in mobility and cognition. In addition to investigating the cross-sectional associations of cognition and lower extremity function with perceived walking modifications and difficulties, we conducted longitudinal analyses because the deterioration of walking ability is a process (2,4,36). The combined effects have been studied previously by comparing people with the combination of poor physical and poor cognitive performance with people without this combination (25,27). We were also interested in those with deficits in only one or other of the predictors. Thus, we categorized the participants based on clinical thresholds for lower extremity function and cognition and used the different combinations of these in our analyses. This enabled us to consider different profiles of functioning; however, it also prevented subgroup analyses because the groups were small and statistical power low. Finally, we chose MMSE and SPPB as indicators of cognitive and physical performance, as they are well-established measures of overall cognitive and physical performance. However, in the future, it might be beneficial to study executive function in addition to overall cognition because executive function is a more sensitive measure for detecting early cognitive decline than MMSE (37). In addition, executive function is closely related to walking (12) because it is controlled by prefrontal brain areas, which are important for motor control especially in old age (38). Strengths and Limitations The strengths of this study concern the study design and the measures used. First, we had a rather large population-based sample with little missing data. To our knowledge, this was the first study to examine the combined associations of lower extremity function and cognitive performance with perceived walking ability. Moreover, we used both well-established subjective and objective measures, the validity and reliability of which have been tested (4,18,20,21). Earlier research has tended to study walking objectively, whereas, given the stated need for assessment of real-world mobility (39), subjective measures may provide more comprehensive information about older people’s mobility in their everyday environment (1). Furthermore, self-reports are an economical way to assess mobility in large samples (4). Using self-reported walking difficulties as an outcome also allowed us to study mobility modifications. Modifications precede walking difficulties but are signs of declining function and health (4). They have not yet been studied thoroughly and may not have been detected in traditional walking assessments. This study has its limitations. First, we might not have covered the whole phenomenon of mobility modification. We used a structured question, which may not have included all possible changes in walking, to measure walking modifications. However, the measure has been validated (4). Furthermore, SPPB and MMSE were dichotomized in the analyses. Dichotomization reduces variability in data, rules out detection of dose–response relationships, and may lead to underestimation of associations. However, using established cutoff criteria makes the results more understandable and, in the present study, made it possible to examine the combined influences of lower extremity function and cognitive performance on walking ability. The MMSE cut point of 24 is commonly used to identify risk for dementia (22), but some people with impaired cognition may have scored above that cut point. It has been discovered that highly educated persons, especially, would need a higher cut point to represent their normal level of cognition (40). Another limitation is that we only used baseline characteristics as predictors of incident walking difficulties. Therefore, the analysis does not take into account competing risks of new comorbidities or subsequent cognitive decline or lower extremity impairments. However, the level of attrition was low, which may support the validity of the findings. Conclusion The findings of this study indicate that older people who concurrently manifest poorer lower extremity function and cognitive performance have the highest odds for walking difficulty. Thus, they are especially vulnerable to further disability. However, future studies should establish whether current methods to assess mobility among older people with cognitive impairment are adequate. Moreover, our results suggest that in addition to lower extremity function, cognitive performance should be taken into account when developing interventions aiming at preventing mobility disability. Furthermore, studying cognition and mobility as a combined research entity may improve the accuracy of risk prediction for mobility disability. Supplementary Material Supplementary data is available at The Journals of Gerontology, Series A: Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences online. Funding This study was supported by the European Research Council (grant number 693045 to T.R.); the Academy of Finland (grant number 255403 to T.R. and 285747 to M.R.); and the Ministry of Education and Culture (to T.R. and M.R.). The financial sponsors were not involved in the design, implementation, analyses, or reporting of the results. Conflict of Interest T.R. serves on the Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences editorial board. Otherwise, the authors declare no conflicts of interest. References 1. Rantanen T . Promoting mobility in older people . J Prev Med Public Health . 2013 ; 46 ( suppl 1 ): S50 – S54 . doi: 10.3961/jpmph.2013.46.S.S50 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 2. Rantakokko M , Mänty M , Rantanen T . Mobility decline in old age . Exerc Sport Sci Rev . 2013 ; 41 : 19 – 25 . doi: 10.1097/JES.0b013e3182556f1e Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 3. Angel R , Ostir GV , Frisco ML , Markides KS . Comparison of a self-reported and a performance-based assessment of mobility in the Hispanic established population for epidemiological studies of the elderly . Res Aging . 2000 ; 22 : 715 – 737 . doi: 10.1177/0164027500226006 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS 4. Mänty M , Heinonen A , Leinonen R , et al. Construct and predictive validity of a self-reported measure of preclinical mobility limitation . Arch Phys Med Rehabil . 2007 ; 88 : 1108 – 1113 . doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2007.06.016 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 5. Young Y , Boyd CM , Guralnik JM , Fried LP . Does self-reported function correspond to objective measures of functional impairment ? J Am Med Dir Assoc . 2010 ; 11 : 645 – 653 . doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2009.12.084 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 6. Rantakokko M , Portegijs E , Viljanen A , Iwarsson S , Rantanen T . Task modifications in walking postpone decline in life-space mobility among community-dwelling older people: a 2-year follow-up study . J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci . 2017;72(9):1252–1256 . doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw348 7. Verbrugge LM , Jette AM . The disablement process . Soc Sci Med . 1994 ; 38 : 1 – 14 . doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(94)90294-1 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 8. Montero-Odasso M , Verghese J , Beauchet O , Hausdorff JM . Gait and cognition: a complementary approach to understanding brain function and the risk of falling . J Am Geriatr Soc . 2012 ; 60 : 2127 – 2136 . doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.04209.x Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 9. Clouston SA , Brewster P , Kuh D , et al. The dynamic relationship between physical function and cognition in longitudinal aging cohorts . Epidemiol Rev . 2013 ; 35 : 33 – 50 . doi: 10.1093/epirev/mxs004 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 10. Demnitz N , Esser P , Dawes H , et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of cross-sectional studies examining the relationship between mobility and cognition in healthy older adults . Gait Posture . 2016 ; 50 : 164 – 174 . doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.08.028 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 11. Kueper JK , Speechley M , Lingum NR , Montero-Odasso M . Motor function and incident dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis . Age Ageing . 2017 ; 46 : 729 – 738 . doi: 10.1093/ageing/afx084 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 12. Best JR , Liu-Ambrose T , Boudreau RM , et al. ; Health, Aging and Body Composition Study . An evaluation of the longitudinal, bidirectional associations between gait speed and cognition in older women and men . J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci . 2016 ; 71 : 1616 – 1623 . doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw066 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 13. Dumurgier J , Artaud F , Touraine C , et al. Gait speed and decline in gait speed as predictors of incident dementia . J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci . 2017 ; 72 : 655 – 661 . doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw110 Google Scholar PubMed 14. Montero-Odasso M , Bherer L , Studenski S , et al. Mobility and cognition in seniors. Report from the 2008 Institute of Aging (CIHR) Mobility and Cognition Workshop . Can Geriatr J . 2015 ; 18 : 159 – 167 . doi: 10.5770/cgj.18.188 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 15. Varma VR , Hausdorff JM , Studenski SA , et al. Aging, the central nervous system, and mobility in older adults: interventions . J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci . 2016 ; 71 : 1451 – 1458 . doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw080 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 16. Rantanen T , Portegijs E , Viljanen A , et al. Individual and environmental factors underlying life space of older people – study protocol and design of a cohort study on Life-Space Mobility in Old Age (LISPE) . BMC Public Health . 2012 ; 12 : 1018 . doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-1018 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 17. Rantakokko M , Portegijs E , Viljanen A , Iwarsson S , Kauppinen M , Rantanen T . Changes in life-space mobility and quality of life among community-dwelling older people: a 2-year follow-up study . Qual Life Res . 2016 ; 25 : 1189 – 1197 . doi: 10.1007/s11136-015-1137-x Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 18. Guralnik JM , Simonsick EM , Ferrucci L , et al. A short physical performance battery assessing lower extremity function: association with self-reported disability and prediction of mortality and nursing home admission . J Gerontol . 1994 ; 49 : M85 – M94 . doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.2.M85 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 19. Portegijs E , Rantakokko M , Mikkola TM , Viljanen A , Rantanen T . Association between physical performance and sense of autonomy in outdoor activities and life-space mobility in community-dwelling older people . J Am Geriatr Soc . 2014 ; 62 : 615 – 621 . doi: 10.1111/jgs.12763 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 20. Guralnik JM , Ferrucci L , Simonsick EM , Salive ME , Wallace RB . Lower-extremity function in persons over the age of 70 years as a predictor of subsequent disability . N Engl J Med . 1995 ; 332 : 556 – 561 . doi: 10.1056/NEJM199503023320902 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 21. Folstein MF , Folstein SE , McHugh PR . “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician . J Psychiatr Res . 1975 ; 12 : 189 – 198 . doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 22. Creavin ST , Wisniewski S , Noel-Storr AH , et al. Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) for the detection of dementia in clinically unevaluated people aged 65 and over in community and primary care populations . Cochrane Database Syst Rev . 2016;1:CD01114 . doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD011145.pub2 23. Radloff LS . The CES-D scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population . Appl Psychol Meas . 1977 ; 1 : 385 – 401 . doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS 24. Viljanen A , Kulmala J , Rantakokko M , Koskenvuo M , Kaprio J , Rantanen T . Fear of falling and coexisting sensory difficulties as predictors of mobility decline in older women . J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci . 2012 ; 67 : 1230 – 1237 . doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls134 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 25. Verghese J , Wang C , Lipton RB , Holtzer R . Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and the risk of dementia . J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci . 2013 ; 68 : 412 – 418 . doi: 10.1093/gerona/gls191 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 26. Montero-Odasso MM , Barnes B , Speechley M , et al. Disentangling cognitive-frailty: results from the gait and brain study . J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci . 2016 ; 71 : 1476 – 1482 . doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw044 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 27. Ayers E , Verghese J . Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and risk of mortality in older adults . Alzheimers Dement . 2016 ; 12 : 556 – 564 . doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2015.08.167 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 28. Callisaya ML , Ayers E , Barzilai N , et al. Motoric cognitive risk syndrome and falls risk: a multi-center study . J Alzheimers Dis . 2016 ; 53 : 1043 – 1052 . doi: 10.3233/JAD-160230 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 29. von Bonsdorff M , Rantanen T , Laukkanen P , Suutama T , Heikkinen E . Mobility limitations and cognitive deficits as predictors of institutionalization among community-dwelling older people . Gerontology . 2006 ; 52 : 359 – 365 . doi: 10.1159/000094985 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 30. Chhetri JK , Chan P , Vellas B , Cesari M . Motoric cognitive risk syndrome: predictor of dementia and age-related negative outcomes . Front Med (Lausanne) . 2017 ; 4 : 166 . doi: 10.3389/fmed.2017.00166 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 31. Farias ST , Mungas D , Jagust W . Degree of discrepancy between self and other-reported everyday functioning by cognitive status: dementia, mild cognitive impairment, and healthy elders . Int J Geriatr Psychiatry . 2005 ; 20 : 827 – 834 . doi: 10.1002/gps.1367 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 32. Poranen-Clark T , von Bonsdorff MB , Rantakokko M , et al. Executive function and life-space mobility in old age . Aging Clin Exp Res . 2018;30:145 . doi: 10.1007/s40520-017-0762-3 33. Poranen-Clark T , von Bonsdorff MB , Rantakokko M , et al. The temporal association between executive function and life-space mobility in old age . J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci . 2018;73(6):835–839 . doi: 10.1093/gerona/glx217 34. Rantanen T , Guralnik JM , Ferrucci L , et al. Coimpairments as predictors of severe walking disability in older women . J Am Geriatr Soc . 2001 ; 49 : 21 – 27 . doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.49005.x Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 35. Rantanen T , Guralnik JM , Ferrucci L , Leveille S , Fried LP . Coimpairments: strength and balance as predictors of severe walking disability . J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci . 1999 ; 54 : M172 – M176 . doi: 10.1093/gerona/54.4.M172 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 36. Guralnik JM , Ferrucci L , Balfour JL , Volpato S , Di Iorio A . Progressive versus catastrophic loss of the ability to walk: implications for the prevention of mobility loss . J Am Geriatr Soc . 2001 ; 49 : 1463 – 1470 . doi: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2001.4911238.x Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 37. Juby A , Tench S , Baker V . The value of clock drawing in identifying executive cognitive dysfunction in people with a normal Mini-Mental State Examination score . CMAJ . 2002 ; 167 : 859 – 864 . Google Scholar PubMed 38. Seidler RD , Bernard JA , Burutolu TB , et al. Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects . Neurosci Biobehav Rev . 2010 ; 34 : 721 – 733 . doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 39. Rosso AL , Studenski SA , Chen WG , et al. Aging, the central nervous system, and mobility . J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci . 2013 ; 68 : 1379 – 1386 . doi: 10.1093/gerona/glt089 Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed 40. O’Bryant SE , Humphreys JD , Smith GE , et al. Detecting dementia with the Mini-Mental State Examination in highly educated individuals . Arch Neurol . 2008 ; 65 : 963 – 967 . doi: 10.1001/archneur.65.7.963 Google Scholar PubMed © The Author(s) 2018. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com. This article is published and distributed under the terms of the Oxford University Press, Standard Journals Publication Model (https://academic.oup.com/journals/pages/open_access/funder_policies/chorus/standard_publication_model) TI - The Combined Effect of Lower Extremity Function and Cognitive Performance on Perceived Walking Ability Among Older People: A 2-Year Follow-up Study JF - The Journals of Gerontology Series A: Biomedical Sciences and Medical Sciences DO - 10.1093/gerona/gly103 DA - 2018-10-08 UR - https://www.deepdyve.com/lp/oxford-university-press/the-combined-effect-of-lower-extremity-function-and-cognitive-ww36cPGsiG SP - 1568 VL - 73 IS - 11 DP - DeepDyve ER -