TY - JOUR AU - Peralta,, Mericarmen AB - Abstract Media exposure is correlated with child obesity, yet the family behaviors underlying this link remain poorly understood. Using data from a sample of U.S. parents and their preschoolers, this study assessed parent and child exposure to 5 different media along with child dietary intake. Child healthy-meal schemas were measured with the Placemat Protocol, a novel play-based pretend meal assembly activity. Child and parent commercial TV viewing predicted greater obesogenic dietary intake for children in food-secure but not food-insecure households. Child commercial TV viewing also predicted a greater proportion of energy-dense to total foods in children's pretend healthy meals. Discussion focuses on food insecurity as a potential moderator of marketing effects and calls for continued research on child meal schema development. Although the rate of child obesity in the United States has leveled out since 2003, it remains high among preschoolers (Ogden, Carroll, Kit, & Flegal, 2014). Defining overweight as a body weight at or above the 85th percentile of the peer population and obesity as the portion at or above the 95th percentile, overweight and obesity together increased from 5.0 to 13.9% among 2- to 5-year-olds from 1976 to 2004 (CDC, 2007). In 2011–2012, more than 20% of U.S. 2- to 5-year-olds were overweight or obese (Ogden et al., 2014), and overweight between ages 2 and 5 is a robust predictor of adult obesity (McCarthy et al., 2007). The preschool years are a sensitive window of development because the adiposity rebound (i.e., the point in life when a person's weight at a given height hits its lowest point and begins to rise again) in children who later become obese is roughly age 3, compared with age 6 among those who grow up normal-weight (Rolland-Cachera, Deheeger, Maillot, & Bellisle, 2006). Children's social and family contexts constrain and influence their food choices (Harrison et al., 2011). U.S. children spend $200 billion annually, mostly on food products (Institute of Medicine, 2006). Children ages 2 to 14 influence an additional $500 billion in family purchases (Institute of Medicine, 2006), over 75% of which are grocery items (McNeal, 1998). Most TV advertisements targeting children sell foods high in energy and low in vital macronutrients and micronutrients (Gantz, Schwartz, Angelini, & Rideout, 2007; Harrison & Marske, 2005), and TV viewing predicts poorer nutritional knowledge and reasoning among 6-year-olds (Harrison, 2005). These findings suggest that children become receptive to commercial media information and misinformation about food and eating even before kindergarten. Parents (Wethington & Johnson-Askew, 2009) and childcare providers (Kim & Dev, 2011) are the primary gatekeepers of preschooler food intake, so adults exert more control over preschoolers' daily food intake than do preschoolers themselves. However, between ages 3 and 5, children begin relating foods to health (Singleton, Achterberg, & Shannon, 1992). By school age, their beliefs about food guide children's independent food choices and eating behaviors (Kandiah & Jones, 2002; Lakshman, Sharp, Ong, & Forouhi, 2010). Children as young as 6 who have completed nutrition education curricula display greater nutritional knowledge and more healthful independent food intake (Baskale & Bahar, 2011) and food preferences (Kandiah & Jones, 2002). As children's independence over their dietary intake increases with entry into formal schooling, it is important to gauge preschoolers' food schemas to understand what they think “healthy” meals are and how sociocultural factors such as family commercial media exposure may influence these perceptions. Communication researchers have a hand in the effort to understand and limit sociocultural influences on early childhood overweight and obesity because we specialize in analyzing media and interpersonal messages and the influence of these messages on individuals' and families' beliefs and behaviors. The collection of factors that influence family food buying behaviors and children's energy intake and output is enormous (Harrison et al., 2011) and beyond the scope of this article, but as research consistently points to obesogenic food marketing as a key sociocultural factor, the relationship between commercial media exposure and child weight remains a research priority. Commercial TV and child obesogenic dietary intake Research linking children's exposure to media, especially TV, to an increased risk of child overweight and obesity is now plentiful. Jordan and Robinson (2008) evaluated evidence for four mechanisms—reduced resting metabolic rate, displaced physical activity, obesogenic food purchasing in response to food marketing, and appetitive priming in response to food marketing and portrayals—and concluded that the most supported mechanisms were the latter two. Through food marketing in commercial media, viewers are urged to buy foods that seem rewarding to eat. Once those foods are in the pantry, the same marketing messages (and other environmental cues) may prime cravings. Experimental research on appetitive priming (i.e., an externally cued desire for food to satisfy hedonic hunger, not produced by a physical deficit in energy; see Lowe & Butryn, 2007) revealed that children exposed to TV food advertising for salty snacks ate more of a nonadvertised salty snack during exposure than children exposed to advertisements for healthful foods or nonfood products (Harris, Bargh, & Brownell, 2009). In recognition of the power of marketing, the biological effects of food advertising on children's ability to exert control over their eating have become a pediatric research priority (Gearhardt & Brownell, 2013). Before appetitive priming can lead to consumption, food must be purchased. The chief theoretical process assumed in most research on food marketing's effects is social learning, specifically behavioral modeling urging audiences to buy on the basis of anticipated gratification (Bandura, 2002). Exposure to advertising for obesogenic foods is typically hypothesized to increase the likelihood of parents buying such foods and of children urging their parents to buy them. Research supports this hypothesis (Hingle & Kunkel, 2012) for both the chief purchasers (parents) and their children. For example, Harrison and Liechty (2012) and Miller, Taveras, Rifas-Shiman, and Gillman (2008) reported that preschoolers' TV viewing predicted increased child intake of obesogenic foods and reduced intake of fruits and vegetables, which receive virtually no food advertising (Gantz et al., 2007). Despite evidence for both modeling and priming, Hingle and Kunkel (2012) argued that the mechanisms connecting family media exposure to obesogenic food consumption remain unclear, in part because the dynamics of requesting, demanding, buying, bargaining, instructing, compromising, sneaking, hoarding, stealing, and otherwise engaging with food are rarely observed in vivo by researchers. To the extent that children are learning about food from family negotiations and exchanges of information, and to the extent that commercial media have some role in this process, an empirical connection should exist between child commercial media exposure, child dietary intake, and children's perceptions of healthy foods and meals. However, parent media exposure likely matters as well. Preschoolers are not as skilled as older children at remembering food brands and asking for them by name (Oates, Blades, & Gunter, 2002), so the power of TV advertising to influence parent buying through young children is limited. Although most studies on the media-obesity link have only measured child media exposure (Jordan & Robinson, 2008), it is helpful to measure both parent and child media exposure, especially since the TV viewing habits (Barradas, Fulton, Blanck, & Huhman, 2007) and dietary habits (Sonneville et al., 2012) of parents and children are correlated. Food insecurity as contextual variable A household variable of central interest in this article is food insecurity, which influences the quantity and types of foods parents buy. Food insecurity exists when nutritious and safe foods are limited or when the ability to acquire them in socially acceptable ways is limited or uncertain, with more severe forms resulting in persistent child hunger (Bickel, Nord, Price, Hamilton, & Cook, 2000). Food insecurity is associated with increased overweight and obesity among adults, but the link is inconsistent among children (Dinour, Bergen, & Yeh, 2007). Most food-insecure families have limited incomes; according to the United States Department of Agriculture (USDA), those with gross monthly incomes at or below 130% of the U.S. poverty guideline ($2,498 for a family of four in 2013) qualify for the Supplemental Nutrition Assistance Program (SNAP), formerly the Food Stamp Program (USDA, 2013a). Families who do not qualify for SNAP may also be food-insecure if the family budget is perceived to be inadequate to cover the family's dietary needs. With respect to their receptivity to food marketing, food-secure and food-insecure parents may both be influenced by obesogenic food marketing, but in different ways. Food-secure families are likely to be influenced in the traditional, research-supported way: Greater exposure should be associated with greater consumption based on the theoretical mechanisms (modeling and priming) described earlier in this article. When family resources are perceived to be plentiful enough that there is no threat of starvation, food advertising should have more potential to lead to buying with fewer budgetary disincentives. Thus, for food-secure parents, we hypothesized that both parent and child exposure to commercial TV would predict (H1a) increased child intake of obesogenic foods and (H1b) decreased child intake of nonobesogenic foods (fruits and vegetables). Following this reasoning, it seems that anxiety over resources would limit effects of food advertising for food-insecure parents because there is little perceived “wiggle room” in the family budget to satisfy advertising-cued hedonic hunger without threatening the goal of meeting the family's nutritional needs on a limited income. In spite of this, food-insecure families may remain susceptible to the persuasive effects of food marketing and may even be uniquely susceptible during vulnerable times of their income cycle. Dinour et al. (2007) proposed a conceptual framework linking the monthly food-stamp cycle to a feast-famine pattern based on evidence that episodic food insecurity can lead to binge eating when food is plentiful, typically when the monthly allowance is received (Towensend, Peerson, Love, Achterberg, & Murphy, 2001). As this allowance is fixed, feasting early in the month increases the risk of famine later. The experience of hunger predisposes the family to feast again the next month, repeating the cycle. Even food-insecure families that do not participate in the SNAP program may experience this cycle, leading to greater attraction to and consumption of energy-dense foods (Harris et al., 2009) and rendering food advertising particularly potent during the famine period. Given these competing rationales, we asked whether parent and child exposure to commercial TV would be related to child obesogenic dietary intake (RQ1a) and nonobesogenic dietary intake (RQ1b) for food-insecure families. It is unclear whether there is any reason to expect the relationship between commercial TV exposure and dietary intake to be stronger for food-secure than food-insecure families or vice versa. Given the lack of published research on food insecurity as a moderator in the media-obesity relationship, we asked (RQ2) whether food insecurity would moderate the relationship between child and parent commercial TV exposure and child obesogenic and nonobesogenic food intake, as evidenced by significantly different relationships for food-secure and food-insecure families. Commercial TV and child healthy-meal schemas To parallel our hypotheses for child dietary intake, we also hypothesized that (H2) commercial TV viewing would predict child healthy-meal schemas emphasizing more obesogenic (energy-dense) foods. Researchers have attempted to map food-schema development in children as young as 3 (Holub & Musher-Eizenman, 2010; Nguyen, 2007; Nguyen, Gordon, & McCullough, 2011). Defining healthy food schemas as evaluative categories that include items sharing the same value-laden assessment (e.g., “healthy” vs. “junky”) and operationalizing correct classification of foods such as apples as “healthy” and cookies as “junky,” these investigators have documented emerging nutrition knowledge among preschoolers. This knowledge is just better than chance (59%) at age 3 and improves to almost 80% by age 7 (Nguyen, 2007). Holub and Musher-Eizenman (2010) asked 69 3- to 6-year-olds to assemble pretend meals of four foods and one drink using photographs of 21 different foods and drinks. Within these constraints, children's “healthy” meals featured less estimated fat and calories than their unhealthy meals, although few children were able to explain their choices verbally in nutritional terms. These foundational studies indicate that evaluative schematic cognitions about “healthy” foods and meals begin developing during the preschool years and are likely influenced along with family buying and consumption behaviors by sociocultural agents such as media food marketing. To update the verbal and photographic schema-assessment methods of past research, we opted to measure child healthy-meal schemas with a pretend-play procedure (Harrison & Peralta, 2013) involving realistic three-dimensional food replicas. The schematic construct of interest in this study paralleled the “healthy” versus “junky” distinction identified by Nguyen and colleagues (Nguyen, 2007; Nguyen et al., 2011), since a food's apparent identity as a health food or a junk food is a concrete, tangible indicator of food/nutritional quality from a young child's perceptually dependent perspective. Half of the food models in the task were energy-dense (“junky”) and half were nutrient-dense (“healthy”), so we defined “healthiness” as a lower proportion of energy-dense to total food items in pretend meals. There was no reason to expect food insecurity to play a moderating role for children's meal schemas. Method Participants This study was approved by the University of Michigan Health Sciences and Behavioral Sciences IRB prior to recruitment and data collection. Recruitment was based on a sample with unequal probability of selection among licensed preschools in a three-county area in Michigan that (a) were registered with the state Bureau of Children and Adult Licensing, a condition for receiving federal food assistance, (b) were located within 50 miles of the study center in one of nine urban or rural areas selected to maximize racial and economic diversity, and (c) enrolled a minimum of 10 children in the targeted age range. These criteria identified 25 eligible preschools, 18 of which agreed to participate (response rate = 72%). Parents of all 4- to 6-year-old children at each preschool were invited to complete a questionnaire for $50.00 and asked to provide permission for their children to be interviewed. Parents of more than one eligible child were asked to choose the oldest. Participants were 278 parents (93% female) reporting on their children. In addition, biometric (body mass) and meal schema data were obtained via direct interaction with 247 of participating parents' children (50% female). Child age based on school-reported birthdates ranged from 40 to 76 months (M = 56.3, SD = 5.3), with 97% (232) between 48 and 72 months. All analyses involving data collected directly from children were restricted to the 247 complete parent–child pairs. All analyses involving data supplied exclusively by parents utilized the whole parent sample of 278. According to parent reports, 29% (68) of children were non-Hispanic Black, 59% (145) were non-Hispanic White, 7% (17) were Asian, 4% (11) were Hispanic, and 2% (6) were Native American Indian. In analyses, race/ethnicity was coded as Black versus non-Black due to higher obesity rates and greater TV viewing among African Americans than Anglo Americans (Crespo et al., 2001). About 38% (89) of parents were unmarried and 37% (87) reported receiving WIC assistance;1 26% (62) had enrolled their children in Head Start. For parent body mass index (BMI, a ratio representing weight for height), parents reported their height in feet and inches or centimeters and their weight in pounds or kilograms. Nonmetric responses were converted to metric and parent BMI was calculated by dividing weight in kg by height in cm squared. Education was measured by having parents check one of seven categories ranging from no formal education to postgraduate work; 46.5% (114) had obtained less than a 4-year college degree. Parents indicated monthly household income by checking 1 of 14 categories from 0 to =$6000; the median category was $2501–$3000 ($30,012–$36,000 annually). About 42% of the sample reported an annual income at or below the U.S. poverty threshold of $23,550 for a family of four in 2013 (United States Department of Health and Human Services, 2013). Procedure and measures Parents Parents completed questionnaires online or on paper. The demographic variables described above were measured for use as controls because they are related to media use (Roberts & Foehr, 2004). Parent media exposure variables included estimated weekly personal exposure to commercial TV, digitally recorded (DVR'd) TV with commercials skipped over, DVDs/VHS tapes, video games, websites, and books. To distinguish commercial TV from DVR'd TV, commercial TV exposure was measured first with the item “About how many minutes or hours do you usually watch television with commercials (not digitally recorded television)?” DVR'd TV exposure was then measured with the item “DVR (digitally recorded TV without commercials): About how many minutes or hours do you usually watch?” Similar items were used for video games, websites, and books. Parents reported the estimated minutes/hours they used each medium during separate dayparts on a typical weekday (in the morning, while their child is at preschool, after their child comes home from preschool, and after dinner) and a typical weekend day (in the morning, after lunch, and after dinner; see Harrison & Liechty, 2012). Response options included 0 minutes, 15 minutes, 30 minutes, 1 hour, 2 hours, 3 hours, 4 hours, and 5+ hours. Hours were converted to minutes and scores for dayparts were added, with the weekday total multiplied by 5 added to the weekend day total multiplied by 2, yielding a weekly exposure score in minutes for each medium. These scores were divided by 60 to yield weekly exposure in hours. To assess child media exposure, the same set of questions and calculations were repeated for “your child.” Parents completed their child's media exposure items after they had completed their own. Child dietary intake was estimated with parental reports of children's consumption of seven foods (fruits, vegetables, sugared beverages, French fries, fast food, candy/sweets, and salty snacks) using items from the United States Department of Education's Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B) interview protocol for 2-year-olds (United States Department of Education, 2008). The protocol began: “The next questions ask about food your child ate or drank during the past 7 days. Think about all the meals and snacks your child had from the time he/she got up until he/she went to bed. Be sure to include food your child ate at home, preschool, restaurants, play dates, anywhere else, and over the weekend.” Response options included 0 = my child did not eat/drink any _____ during the past 7 days, 1 = once a day, 2 = twice a day, 3 = three times a day, 4 = four or more times a day, 5 = one to three times during the past 7 days, 6 = four to six times during the past 7 days, and 7 = don't know. Responses 5 and 6 were recoded as fractions representing average times per day (i.e., one to three times/week = 0.2857 times/day and four to six times/week = 0.7143 times/day). Responses of 7 (don't know) were treated as missing data. Each individual item began “How many times did your child eat (drink)_____” and named the food in question. An index of obesogenic dietary intake was created by adding scores for sugared beverages, French fries, fast food, candy/sweets, and salty snacks; a complementary index of nonobesogenic dietary intake was created by adding scores for fruits and vegetables. Food insecurity was measured with the Stage 1 Screen from the USDA Guide to Measuring Household Food Security (Bickel et al., 2000). These items measure parental perceptions about adequate finances and adequate meal quality given financial constraints. The five items are “I worried whether our food would run out before we got money to buy more”; “The food that we bought just didn't last, and we didn't have money to get more”; “We couldn't afford to eat balanced meals”; “We relied on only a few kinds of low-cost food to feed the children because we were running out of money to buy food”; and “We couldn't feed the children a balanced meal because we couldn't afford that.” Response options ranged from 0 (never true in the last 12 months) to 4 (often true in the last 12 months). Internal consistency for the five items was α = .88. About half (155 or 52%) of our parent sample answered “never true” for all five items and were coded “food secure”; the other half (141, or 48%) expressed at least some food insecurity and were coded “food insecure.” Children Children were interviewed in person at their preschools by trained research assistants. Healthy-meal schemas were assessed with the Placemat Protocol (Harrison & Peralta, 2013; see Figure 1). The Placemat is a 4′ × 5′ laminated mat with two copies each of 36 highly realistic food replicas in child-sized portions, arranged in categories (vegetables, fruits, grains, dairy, meat and other proteins, and beverages) consistent with the USDA MyPlate initiative (USDA, 2012). Half of the items in each category are lower in calories and higher in micronutrients (nutrient-dense) and half are higher in calories and lower in micronutrients (energy-dense). Replicas represented the unbranded food items consumed most frequently nationwide by U.S. preschoolers (Skinner et al., 1998).2 Figure 1 Open in new tabDownload slide A 6-year-old girl using Placemat Protocol to complete preferred (top) and healthy (bottom) pretend meals. Figure 1 Open in new tabDownload slide A 6-year-old girl using Placemat Protocol to complete preferred (top) and healthy (bottom) pretend meals. Each child was asked, “Are you hungry right now?” Response options included no, a little, or a lot. The researcher then seated the child on the mat in front of a 10'' white plate, named the foods on the mat, and helped the child assemble the first meal (preferred foods). Children were told, “We're going to make a pretend meal together, using the foods and drinks here. You can put a little food on the plate or a lot, but don't put any more than you think you could eat at one time. Let's start by making a meal of your FAVORITE foods. If you could pick any of these foods and drinks— the ones you like best—which would you pick?” When the child was finished, the plate was cleared and the procedure was repeated for the healthy meal: “Now we're going to make a HEALTHY meal. I want you to pick foods and drinks you think are healthy.” The researcher avoided defining “healthy” to gauge children's schemas without priming specific schematic content. To the 42 children who spontaneously asked what “healthy” means, the researcher offered the prompt “good for your body.” All children constructed the preferred meal followed by the healthy meal, consistent with the preferred-before-healthy procedure used by Holub and Musher-Eizenman (2010). Energy-dense and nutrient-dense items were counted on each plate, and the proportion of energy-dense to total food items was calculated for each meal. Approximately 1–2 weeks after completing the Placemat Protocol, child height and weight were measured twice with a portable digital scale and stadiometer following Centers for Disease Control & Prevention (CDC) standardized protocol (CDC, 2010). The weight measures correlated at r = .999 and the height measures at r = .998. BMI percentile was tabulated using the CDC's BMI Tool for Schools program (National Center for Health Statistics, 2000a, 2000b). About 3% of the child sample were in the underweight range (<5th percentile), 73% were in the normal range (5th to 85th percentile), 23% were in the overweight/obese range (≥85th percentile), and 9% were in the exclusively obese range (≥95th percentile). Results Descriptive statistics Means, standard deviations, and zero-order correlations for key variables are displayed separately for parent (Table 1) and child (Table 2) media exposure variables. Because parent BMI, parent education, parent and child race/ethnicity, and household income were correlated with media exposure and dietary intake, they were entered as controls in analyses. Table 1 Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations for Family Variables and Parent Media Variables (Parent N = 296, Child N = 247) Measure . 1 . 2 . 3 . 4 . 5 . 6 . 7 . 8 . 9 . 10 . 11 . 12 . 13 . 14 . 15 . 16 . M 27.88 0.26 4.52 7.97 59.92 2.23 3.21  14.79 6.50  5.64  1.42  9.74  8.25  0.62  0.39  0.48 SD  7.68 0.44 1.17 4.81 28.50 2.08 1.69  14.80 9.28  10.42  3.96  11.39   10.51   0.22  0.25  0.50 1. Parent BMI  .25 −.22 −.37   .20  .05 .00   .30 .10 .22 .01 .10 .21 .02 .16 .26 2. Parent race/ethnicity −.43 −.53   .01  .22 .05   .37 .09 .31 .30 .07 .26 −.15   .25 .18 3. Parent education  .67 −.20 −.28 .02 −.38 −.16  −.31  −.32  .09 −.12  .05 −.30  −.35  4. Household income −.23 −.22 .08 −.36 −.03  −.32  −.21  −.06  −.32  .02 −.23  −.46  5. Child BMI percentile   .12 .04  .12 .08 .08 .13 .05 .04 .13 .04 .15 6. Child obesogenic dietary intake .02  .24 .08 .28 .20 .16 .01 .01 .08 −.02  7. Child fruit/vegetable intake −.08 .00 .02 −.01  −.05  .05 .03 −.02  −.10  8. Parent commercial TV exposure .31 .36 .28 .23 .15 −.07  .17 .04 9. Parent DVR'd TV exposure .14 .11 .11 .05 −.02  .14 −.08  10. Parent DVD/VHS exposure .17 .28 .25 −.08  .12 .08 11. Parent video game exposure −.02  .00 −.06  .04 .00 12. Parent website exposure .10 −.01  −.06  −.05  13. Parent book exposure .08 .06 .25 14. Proportion energy-dense foods, child preferred meal −.02 −.00  15. Proportion energy-dense foods, child healthy meal .18 16. Food insecurity Measure . 1 . 2 . 3 . 4 . 5 . 6 . 7 . 8 . 9 . 10 . 11 . 12 . 13 . 14 . 15 . 16 . M 27.88 0.26 4.52 7.97 59.92 2.23 3.21  14.79 6.50  5.64  1.42  9.74  8.25  0.62  0.39  0.48 SD  7.68 0.44 1.17 4.81 28.50 2.08 1.69  14.80 9.28  10.42  3.96  11.39   10.51   0.22  0.25  0.50 1. Parent BMI  .25 −.22 −.37   .20  .05 .00   .30 .10 .22 .01 .10 .21 .02 .16 .26 2. Parent race/ethnicity −.43 −.53   .01  .22 .05   .37 .09 .31 .30 .07 .26 −.15   .25 .18 3. Parent education  .67 −.20 −.28 .02 −.38 −.16  −.31  −.32  .09 −.12  .05 −.30  −.35  4. Household income −.23 −.22 .08 −.36 −.03  −.32  −.21  −.06  −.32  .02 −.23  −.46  5. Child BMI percentile   .12 .04  .12 .08 .08 .13 .05 .04 .13 .04 .15 6. Child obesogenic dietary intake .02  .24 .08 .28 .20 .16 .01 .01 .08 −.02  7. Child fruit/vegetable intake −.08 .00 .02 −.01  −.05  .05 .03 −.02  −.10  8. Parent commercial TV exposure .31 .36 .28 .23 .15 −.07  .17 .04 9. Parent DVR'd TV exposure .14 .11 .11 .05 −.02  .14 −.08  10. Parent DVD/VHS exposure .17 .28 .25 −.08  .12 .08 11. Parent video game exposure −.02  .00 −.06  .04 .00 12. Parent website exposure .10 −.01  −.06  −.05  13. Parent book exposure .08 .06 .25 14. Proportion energy-dense foods, child preferred meal −.02 −.00  15. Proportion energy-dense foods, child healthy meal .18 16. Food insecurity Note: Parent race/ethnicity coded Black = 1, non-Black = 0. Food insecurity coded insecure = 1, secure = 0. Means for dietary intake indicate number of times consumed per day. Coefficients significant at p < .05 are noted in bold. Open in new tab Table 1 Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations for Family Variables and Parent Media Variables (Parent N = 296, Child N = 247) Measure . 1 . 2 . 3 . 4 . 5 . 6 . 7 . 8 . 9 . 10 . 11 . 12 . 13 . 14 . 15 . 16 . M 27.88 0.26 4.52 7.97 59.92 2.23 3.21  14.79 6.50  5.64  1.42  9.74  8.25  0.62  0.39  0.48 SD  7.68 0.44 1.17 4.81 28.50 2.08 1.69  14.80 9.28  10.42  3.96  11.39   10.51   0.22  0.25  0.50 1. Parent BMI  .25 −.22 −.37   .20  .05 .00   .30 .10 .22 .01 .10 .21 .02 .16 .26 2. Parent race/ethnicity −.43 −.53   .01  .22 .05   .37 .09 .31 .30 .07 .26 −.15   .25 .18 3. Parent education  .67 −.20 −.28 .02 −.38 −.16  −.31  −.32  .09 −.12  .05 −.30  −.35  4. Household income −.23 −.22 .08 −.36 −.03  −.32  −.21  −.06  −.32  .02 −.23  −.46  5. Child BMI percentile   .12 .04  .12 .08 .08 .13 .05 .04 .13 .04 .15 6. Child obesogenic dietary intake .02  .24 .08 .28 .20 .16 .01 .01 .08 −.02  7. Child fruit/vegetable intake −.08 .00 .02 −.01  −.05  .05 .03 −.02  −.10  8. Parent commercial TV exposure .31 .36 .28 .23 .15 −.07  .17 .04 9. Parent DVR'd TV exposure .14 .11 .11 .05 −.02  .14 −.08  10. Parent DVD/VHS exposure .17 .28 .25 −.08  .12 .08 11. Parent video game exposure −.02  .00 −.06  .04 .00 12. Parent website exposure .10 −.01  −.06  −.05  13. Parent book exposure .08 .06 .25 14. Proportion energy-dense foods, child preferred meal −.02 −.00  15. Proportion energy-dense foods, child healthy meal .18 16. Food insecurity Measure . 1 . 2 . 3 . 4 . 5 . 6 . 7 . 8 . 9 . 10 . 11 . 12 . 13 . 14 . 15 . 16 . M 27.88 0.26 4.52 7.97 59.92 2.23 3.21  14.79 6.50  5.64  1.42  9.74  8.25  0.62  0.39  0.48 SD  7.68 0.44 1.17 4.81 28.50 2.08 1.69  14.80 9.28  10.42  3.96  11.39   10.51   0.22  0.25  0.50 1. Parent BMI  .25 −.22 −.37   .20  .05 .00   .30 .10 .22 .01 .10 .21 .02 .16 .26 2. Parent race/ethnicity −.43 −.53   .01  .22 .05   .37 .09 .31 .30 .07 .26 −.15   .25 .18 3. Parent education  .67 −.20 −.28 .02 −.38 −.16  −.31  −.32  .09 −.12  .05 −.30  −.35  4. Household income −.23 −.22 .08 −.36 −.03  −.32  −.21  −.06  −.32  .02 −.23  −.46  5. Child BMI percentile   .12 .04  .12 .08 .08 .13 .05 .04 .13 .04 .15 6. Child obesogenic dietary intake .02  .24 .08 .28 .20 .16 .01 .01 .08 −.02  7. Child fruit/vegetable intake −.08 .00 .02 −.01  −.05  .05 .03 −.02  −.10  8. Parent commercial TV exposure .31 .36 .28 .23 .15 −.07  .17 .04 9. Parent DVR'd TV exposure .14 .11 .11 .05 −.02  .14 −.08  10. Parent DVD/VHS exposure .17 .28 .25 −.08  .12 .08 11. Parent video game exposure −.02  .00 −.06  .04 .00 12. Parent website exposure .10 −.01  −.06  −.05  13. Parent book exposure .08 .06 .25 14. Proportion energy-dense foods, child preferred meal −.02 −.00  15. Proportion energy-dense foods, child healthy meal .18 16. Food insecurity Note: Parent race/ethnicity coded Black = 1, non-Black = 0. Food insecurity coded insecure = 1, secure = 0. Means for dietary intake indicate number of times consumed per day. Coefficients significant at p < .05 are noted in bold. Open in new tab Table 2 Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations for Family Variables and Child Media Variables (Parent N = 296, Child N = 247) Measure . 1 . 2 . 3 . 4 . 5 . 6 . 7 . 8 . 9 . 10 . 11 . 12 . 13 . 14 . 15 . 16 . M 27.88  0.29 4.52 7.97 59.92  2.23  3.21 10.88  4.20  4.98  2.48  1.37  7.94  0.62  0.39  0.48 SD 7.68  0.46 1.17 4.81 28.50  2.08  1.69 12.15  7.10  7.60  5.30  3.71  7.22  0.22  0.25  0.50 1. Parent BMI .29 −.22 −.37 .20   .05   .00 .24   .07 .21   .10   .03   .10   .02 .16 .26 2. Child race/ethnicity −.42 −.52  .01 .23   .02 .40   .09 .24 .14 .12 .28  −.11 .25 .18 3. Parent education .67 −.20 −.28   .02 −.40  −.12 −.30 −.20 −.19 −.20   .05 −.30 −.35 4. Household income −.23 −.22   .08 −.38   .05 −.25 −.17 −.16 −.31   .02 −.23 −.46 5. Child BMI percentile   .12   .04 .14   .02 .22 .17   .08   .06 .13   .04 .15 6. Child obesogenic dietary intake   .02 .34 .15 .26 .32 .20   .11   .01   .08  −.02 7. Child fruit/vegetable intake −.18  −.03  −.05 −.16  −.12 .18   .03  −.02  −.10 8. Child commercial TV exposure .19 .41 .43 .36 .17  −.06 .23   .05 9. Child DVRed TV exposure .34 .17 .35   .06  −.01   .06   .00 10. Child DVD/VHS exposure .29 .42 .14   .06   .07   .09 11. Child video game exposure .49 .21   .05  −.04  −.09 12. Child website exposure .30   .09   .03   .05 13. Child book exposure   .07   .05   .11 14. Proportion energy-dense foods, child preferred meal  −.02  −.00 15. Proportion energy-dense foods, child healthy meal .18 16. Food insecurity Measure . 1 . 2 . 3 . 4 . 5 . 6 . 7 . 8 . 9 . 10 . 11 . 12 . 13 . 14 . 15 . 16 . M 27.88  0.29 4.52 7.97 59.92  2.23  3.21 10.88  4.20  4.98  2.48  1.37  7.94  0.62  0.39  0.48 SD 7.68  0.46 1.17 4.81 28.50  2.08  1.69 12.15  7.10  7.60  5.30  3.71  7.22  0.22  0.25  0.50 1. Parent BMI .29 −.22 −.37 .20   .05   .00 .24   .07 .21   .10   .03   .10   .02 .16 .26 2. Child race/ethnicity −.42 −.52  .01 .23   .02 .40   .09 .24 .14 .12 .28  −.11 .25 .18 3. Parent education .67 −.20 −.28   .02 −.40  −.12 −.30 −.20 −.19 −.20   .05 −.30 −.35 4. Household income −.23 −.22   .08 −.38   .05 −.25 −.17 −.16 −.31   .02 −.23 −.46 5. Child BMI percentile   .12   .04 .14   .02 .22 .17   .08   .06 .13   .04 .15 6. Child obesogenic dietary intake   .02 .34 .15 .26 .32 .20   .11   .01   .08  −.02 7. Child fruit/vegetable intake −.18  −.03  −.05 −.16  −.12 .18   .03  −.02  −.10 8. Child commercial TV exposure .19 .41 .43 .36 .17  −.06 .23   .05 9. Child DVRed TV exposure .34 .17 .35   .06  −.01   .06   .00 10. Child DVD/VHS exposure .29 .42 .14   .06   .07   .09 11. Child video game exposure .49 .21   .05  −.04  −.09 12. Child website exposure .30   .09   .03   .05 13. Child book exposure   .07   .05   .11 14. Proportion energy-dense foods, child preferred meal  −.02  −.00 15. Proportion energy-dense foods, child healthy meal .18 16. Food insecurity Note: Child race/ethnicity coded Black = 1, non-Black = 0. Food insecurity coded insecure = 1, secure = 0. Means for dietary intake indicate number of times consumed per day. Coefficients significant at p < .05 are noted in bold. Open in new tab Table 2 Means, Standard Deviations, and Intercorrelations for Family Variables and Child Media Variables (Parent N = 296, Child N = 247) Measure . 1 . 2 . 3 . 4 . 5 . 6 . 7 . 8 . 9 . 10 . 11 . 12 . 13 . 14 . 15 . 16 . M 27.88  0.29 4.52 7.97 59.92  2.23  3.21 10.88  4.20  4.98  2.48  1.37  7.94  0.62  0.39  0.48 SD 7.68  0.46 1.17 4.81 28.50  2.08  1.69 12.15  7.10  7.60  5.30  3.71  7.22  0.22  0.25  0.50 1. Parent BMI .29 −.22 −.37 .20   .05   .00 .24   .07 .21   .10   .03   .10   .02 .16 .26 2. Child race/ethnicity −.42 −.52  .01 .23   .02 .40   .09 .24 .14 .12 .28  −.11 .25 .18 3. Parent education .67 −.20 −.28   .02 −.40  −.12 −.30 −.20 −.19 −.20   .05 −.30 −.35 4. Household income −.23 −.22   .08 −.38   .05 −.25 −.17 −.16 −.31   .02 −.23 −.46 5. Child BMI percentile   .12   .04 .14   .02 .22 .17   .08   .06 .13   .04 .15 6. Child obesogenic dietary intake   .02 .34 .15 .26 .32 .20   .11   .01   .08  −.02 7. Child fruit/vegetable intake −.18  −.03  −.05 −.16  −.12 .18   .03  −.02  −.10 8. Child commercial TV exposure .19 .41 .43 .36 .17  −.06 .23   .05 9. Child DVRed TV exposure .34 .17 .35   .06  −.01   .06   .00 10. Child DVD/VHS exposure .29 .42 .14   .06   .07   .09 11. Child video game exposure .49 .21   .05  −.04  −.09 12. Child website exposure .30   .09   .03   .05 13. Child book exposure   .07   .05   .11 14. Proportion energy-dense foods, child preferred meal  −.02  −.00 15. Proportion energy-dense foods, child healthy meal .18 16. Food insecurity Measure . 1 . 2 . 3 . 4 . 5 . 6 . 7 . 8 . 9 . 10 . 11 . 12 . 13 . 14 . 15 . 16 . M 27.88  0.29 4.52 7.97 59.92  2.23  3.21 10.88  4.20  4.98  2.48  1.37  7.94  0.62  0.39  0.48 SD 7.68  0.46 1.17 4.81 28.50  2.08  1.69 12.15  7.10  7.60  5.30  3.71  7.22  0.22  0.25  0.50 1. Parent BMI .29 −.22 −.37 .20   .05   .00 .24   .07 .21   .10   .03   .10   .02 .16 .26 2. Child race/ethnicity −.42 −.52  .01 .23   .02 .40   .09 .24 .14 .12 .28  −.11 .25 .18 3. Parent education .67 −.20 −.28   .02 −.40  −.12 −.30 −.20 −.19 −.20   .05 −.30 −.35 4. Household income −.23 −.22   .08 −.38   .05 −.25 −.17 −.16 −.31   .02 −.23 −.46 5. Child BMI percentile   .12   .04 .14   .02 .22 .17   .08   .06 .13   .04 .15 6. Child obesogenic dietary intake   .02 .34 .15 .26 .32 .20   .11   .01   .08  −.02 7. Child fruit/vegetable intake −.18  −.03  −.05 −.16  −.12 .18   .03  −.02  −.10 8. Child commercial TV exposure .19 .41 .43 .36 .17  −.06 .23   .05 9. Child DVRed TV exposure .34 .17 .35   .06  −.01   .06   .00 10. Child DVD/VHS exposure .29 .42 .14   .06   .07   .09 11. Child video game exposure .49 .21   .05  −.04  −.09 12. Child website exposure .30   .09   .03   .05 13. Child book exposure   .07   .05   .11 14. Proportion energy-dense foods, child preferred meal  −.02  −.00 15. Proportion energy-dense foods, child healthy meal .18 16. Food insecurity Note: Child race/ethnicity coded Black = 1, non-Black = 0. Food insecurity coded insecure = 1, secure = 0. Means for dietary intake indicate number of times consumed per day. Coefficients significant at p < .05 are noted in bold. Open in new tab Hypotheses and research questions Parent and child media exposure were positively correlated, from r = .23 for website hours to r = .72 for commercial TV hours. Parents reported media exposure for both themselves and their children, so these correlations could be inflated by measurement or could be legitimately high given probable overlap in parent–child viewing in daily family life. In spite of this overlap, child media predictors were not controlled when testing parent media predictors and vice versa because our goal was not to test the predictive power of independent parent and child media use but the gross estimated media diet of each, including overlapping exposure. Given the potential for inflated media exposure correlations due to measurement, any differences observed for parent versus child media exposure should be interpreted with caution. Hypothesis 1a–b We predicted that for food-secure parents, both parent and child commercial TV exposure would predict increased child intake of obesogenic foods (H1a) and decreased child intake of nonobesogenic foods (H1b). Multiple regression analyses with all media variables entered along with key demographic controls (see Table 3) demonstrated clear support for H1a. Both parent and child commercial TV exposure predicted increased child intake of obesogenic foods in food-secure families. H1b, in contrast, was supported only for child commercial TV exposure. Parent commercial TV exposure did not significantly predict decreased fruit and vegetable intake. Table 3 Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Child Obesogenic (OB) and Nonobesogenic (Non-OB) Dietary Intake as Reported by Parents, for Food-Secure Families Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . OB . Non-OB . OB . Non-OB . β . β . β . β . Parent BMI −.02 −.04 −.00  .00 Race/ethnicity   .21*   .29*   .35**  .16 Parent education  .00  .12  .04  .10 Household income  .07  .07  .07  .02 Child BMI percentile  .03  .01  .08  .06 Commercial TV    .43*** −.17    .37***  −.30* DVR'd noncommercial TV  .05  .06   .18*  .06 DVD/VHS −.16 −.09  −.21** −.07 Video games  .15  .04  .14 −.11 Websites −.17 −.10  .10  .01 Books  .01    .27* −.02   .33** Total R2    .32***  .14    .49***   .19* n 114 114 117 117 Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . OB . Non-OB . OB . Non-OB . β . β . β . β . Parent BMI −.02 −.04 −.00  .00 Race/ethnicity   .21*   .29*   .35**  .16 Parent education  .00  .12  .04  .10 Household income  .07  .07  .07  .02 Child BMI percentile  .03  .01  .08  .06 Commercial TV    .43*** −.17    .37***  −.30* DVR'd noncommercial TV  .05  .06   .18*  .06 DVD/VHS −.16 −.09  −.21** −.07 Video games  .15  .04  .14 −.11 Websites −.17 −.10  .10  .01 Books  .01    .27* −.02   .33** Total R2    .32***  .14    .49***   .19* n 114 114 117 117 Note: Analyses using parent media predictors used parent race/ethnicity, and analyses using child media predictors used child race/ethnicity; other predictor variables are identical across analyses. Race/ethnicity coded Black = 1, non-Black = 0. * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001. Open in new tab Table 3 Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Child Obesogenic (OB) and Nonobesogenic (Non-OB) Dietary Intake as Reported by Parents, for Food-Secure Families Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . OB . Non-OB . OB . Non-OB . β . β . β . β . Parent BMI −.02 −.04 −.00  .00 Race/ethnicity   .21*   .29*   .35**  .16 Parent education  .00  .12  .04  .10 Household income  .07  .07  .07  .02 Child BMI percentile  .03  .01  .08  .06 Commercial TV    .43*** −.17    .37***  −.30* DVR'd noncommercial TV  .05  .06   .18*  .06 DVD/VHS −.16 −.09  −.21** −.07 Video games  .15  .04  .14 −.11 Websites −.17 −.10  .10  .01 Books  .01    .27* −.02   .33** Total R2    .32***  .14    .49***   .19* n 114 114 117 117 Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . OB . Non-OB . OB . Non-OB . β . β . β . β . Parent BMI −.02 −.04 −.00  .00 Race/ethnicity   .21*   .29*   .35**  .16 Parent education  .00  .12  .04  .10 Household income  .07  .07  .07  .02 Child BMI percentile  .03  .01  .08  .06 Commercial TV    .43*** −.17    .37***  −.30* DVR'd noncommercial TV  .05  .06   .18*  .06 DVD/VHS −.16 −.09  −.21** −.07 Video games  .15  .04  .14 −.11 Websites −.17 −.10  .10  .01 Books  .01    .27* −.02   .33** Total R2    .32***  .14    .49***   .19* n 114 114 117 117 Note: Analyses using parent media predictors used parent race/ethnicity, and analyses using child media predictors used child race/ethnicity; other predictor variables are identical across analyses. Race/ethnicity coded Black = 1, non-Black = 0. * p < .05. ** p < .01. *** p < .001. Open in new tab Research question 1a– These research questions asked whether the relationships between commercial TV viewing and dietary intake predicted for food-secure families would be observed for food-insecure families. The answer was no on both counts. As shown in Table 4, there were no significant relationships between any of the media variables and child dietary intake for the food-insecure families. Table 4 Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Child Obesogenic (OB) and Nonobesogenic (Non-OB) Dietary Intake as Reported by Parents, for Food-Insecure Families Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . OB . Non-OB . OB . Non-OB . β . β . β . β . Parent BMI  −.05   .08  −.05   .15 Race/ethnicity  −.08  −.05   .00  −.04 Parent education  −.15  −.15  −.17  −.17 Household income  −.03   .10  −.05   .03 Child BMI percentile   .02   .11   .02   .13 Commercial TV  −.09  −.01  −.10  −.12 DVR'd noncommercial TV  −.02  −.05  −.03   .04 DVD/VHS   .21  −.02  −.03  −.23 Video games   .11  −.06   .23  −.13 Websites   .14   .05   .07   .28 Books  −.06   .04  −.02   .02 Total R2   .13   .04   .10   .13 n 86 85 85 84 Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . OB . Non-OB . OB . Non-OB . β . β . β . β . Parent BMI  −.05   .08  −.05   .15 Race/ethnicity  −.08  −.05   .00  −.04 Parent education  −.15  −.15  −.17  −.17 Household income  −.03   .10  −.05   .03 Child BMI percentile   .02   .11   .02   .13 Commercial TV  −.09  −.01  −.10  −.12 DVR'd noncommercial TV  −.02  −.05  −.03   .04 DVD/VHS   .21  −.02  −.03  −.23 Video games   .11  −.06   .23  −.13 Websites   .14   .05   .07   .28 Books  −.06   .04  −.02   .02 Total R2   .13   .04   .10   .13 n 86 85 85 84 Note: Analyses using parent media predictors used parent race/ethnicity, and analyses using child media predictors used child race/ethnicity; other predictor variables are identical across analyses. Race/ethnicity coded Black = 1, non-Black = 0. Open in new tab Table 4 Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Child Obesogenic (OB) and Nonobesogenic (Non-OB) Dietary Intake as Reported by Parents, for Food-Insecure Families Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . OB . Non-OB . OB . Non-OB . β . β . β . β . Parent BMI  −.05   .08  −.05   .15 Race/ethnicity  −.08  −.05   .00  −.04 Parent education  −.15  −.15  −.17  −.17 Household income  −.03   .10  −.05   .03 Child BMI percentile   .02   .11   .02   .13 Commercial TV  −.09  −.01  −.10  −.12 DVR'd noncommercial TV  −.02  −.05  −.03   .04 DVD/VHS   .21  −.02  −.03  −.23 Video games   .11  −.06   .23  −.13 Websites   .14   .05   .07   .28 Books  −.06   .04  −.02   .02 Total R2   .13   .04   .10   .13 n 86 85 85 84 Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . OB . Non-OB . OB . Non-OB . β . β . β . β . Parent BMI  −.05   .08  −.05   .15 Race/ethnicity  −.08  −.05   .00  −.04 Parent education  −.15  −.15  −.17  −.17 Household income  −.03   .10  −.05   .03 Child BMI percentile   .02   .11   .02   .13 Commercial TV  −.09  −.01  −.10  −.12 DVR'd noncommercial TV  −.02  −.05  −.03   .04 DVD/VHS   .21  −.02  −.03  −.23 Video games   .11  −.06   .23  −.13 Websites   .14   .05   .07   .28 Books  −.06   .04  −.02   .02 Total R2   .13   .04   .10   .13 n 86 85 85 84 Note: Analyses using parent media predictors used parent race/ethnicity, and analyses using child media predictors used child race/ethnicity; other predictor variables are identical across analyses. Race/ethnicity coded Black = 1, non-Black = 0. Open in new tab Research question 2 To answer the question of whether the relationships between commercial TV viewing and child dietary intake differed significantly for food-secure and food-insecure families (RQ2), we conducted multiple regression analyses similar to those above but with the entire sample and an additional step featuring the interaction between commercial TV viewing and food insecurity. Parent (β = −.16, ΔR2 = .02, p < .05) and child (β = −.22, ΔR2 = .04, p < .01) commercial TV exposure both interacted significantly with food insecurity in predicting child obesogenic dietary intake. The slopes reported for commercial TV and obesogenic dietary intake in Table 3 (food-secure families) were therefore significantly stronger than their counterparts in Table 4 (food-insecure families). The analogous analyses for nonobesogenic dietary intake did not produce significant interaction coefficients for either parent commercial TV viewing (β = .08) or child commercial TV viewing (β = .03). Thus the slopes reported for commercial TV and nonobesogenic dietary intake in Table 3 (food-secure families) were not significantly different from their counterparts in Table 4 (food-insecure families). Hypothesis 2 This hypothesis predicted that child and parent commercial TV viewing would predict a greater proportion of energy-dense to total foods in children's pretend healthy meals. The multiple regression analyses testing this hypothesis featured the same demographic variables as the analyses for dietary intake, plus four additional controls: family food insecurity, child obesogenic dietary intake, child nonobesogenic dietary intake, and the proportion of energy-dense to total foods in children's pretend preferred meals. This controlled the potential influence of family food insecurity and children's daily eating habits and preferences on the composition of their pretend healthy meals. As summarized in Table 5, H2 was supported for child commercial TV viewing only. More child commercial TV viewing predicted a higher proportion of energy-dense to total foods in children's pretend healthy meals. There was no relationship for parent commercial TV viewing, although the coefficient was positive. Table 5 Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Proportion of Energy-Dense to Total Foods in Children's Pretend Healthy Meals Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . β . β . Parent BMI   .06   .07 Race/ethnicity    .20*   .12 Parent education  −.09  −.12 Household income   .06   .05 Child BMI percentile  −.04  −.02 Food insecurity   .18*   .10 Dietary intake (OB)  −.09  −.02 Dietary intake (fruit/veg)  −.06  −.04 ED foods, preferred meal  −.00   .01 Commercial TV   .09    .21* DVR'd noncommercial TV   .09   .08 DVD/VHS   .04  −.02 Video games  −.09  −.17 Websites  −.13   .01 Books  −.07  −.04 Total R2   .15*    .14* n 195 197 Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . β . β . Parent BMI   .06   .07 Race/ethnicity    .20*   .12 Parent education  −.09  −.12 Household income   .06   .05 Child BMI percentile  −.04  −.02 Food insecurity   .18*   .10 Dietary intake (OB)  −.09  −.02 Dietary intake (fruit/veg)  −.06  −.04 ED foods, preferred meal  −.00   .01 Commercial TV   .09    .21* DVR'd noncommercial TV   .09   .08 DVD/VHS   .04  −.02 Video games  −.09  −.17 Websites  −.13   .01 Books  −.07  −.04 Total R2   .15*    .14* n 195 197 Note: Analysis using parent media predictors used parent race/ethnicity, and analysis using child media predictors used child race/ethnicity; other predictor variables are identical across analyses. Race/ethnicity coded Black = 1, non-Black = 0. Food insecurity coded 0 = food-secure, 1 = food-insecure. * p < .05. Open in new tab Table 5 Multiple Regression Analyses Predicting Proportion of Energy-Dense to Total Foods in Children's Pretend Healthy Meals Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . β . β . Parent BMI   .06   .07 Race/ethnicity    .20*   .12 Parent education  −.09  −.12 Household income   .06   .05 Child BMI percentile  −.04  −.02 Food insecurity   .18*   .10 Dietary intake (OB)  −.09  −.02 Dietary intake (fruit/veg)  −.06  −.04 ED foods, preferred meal  −.00   .01 Commercial TV   .09    .21* DVR'd noncommercial TV   .09   .08 DVD/VHS   .04  −.02 Video games  −.09  −.17 Websites  −.13   .01 Books  −.07  −.04 Total R2   .15*    .14* n 195 197 Predictor . With Parent Media Predictors . With Child Media Predictors . β . β . Parent BMI   .06   .07 Race/ethnicity    .20*   .12 Parent education  −.09  −.12 Household income   .06   .05 Child BMI percentile  −.04  −.02 Food insecurity   .18*   .10 Dietary intake (OB)  −.09  −.02 Dietary intake (fruit/veg)  −.06  −.04 ED foods, preferred meal  −.00   .01 Commercial TV   .09    .21* DVR'd noncommercial TV   .09   .08 DVD/VHS   .04  −.02 Video games  −.09  −.17 Websites  −.13   .01 Books  −.07  −.04 Total R2   .15*    .14* n 195 197 Note: Analysis using parent media predictors used parent race/ethnicity, and analysis using child media predictors used child race/ethnicity; other predictor variables are identical across analyses. Race/ethnicity coded Black = 1, non-Black = 0. Food insecurity coded 0 = food-secure, 1 = food-insecure. * p < .05. Open in new tab Discussion Both child and parent commercial TV exposure predicted increased child intake of obesogenic foods such as sugared beverages, French fries, fast food, candy/sweets, and salty snacks, but only for food-secure families. This finding is consistent with the argument that food insecurity constrains the influence of external factors such as food advertising by imposing limits on families' ability to satisfy hedonic hunger with gratuitous food expenditures. Although people who receive supplemental nutritional assistance may spend their allotment on obesogenic fast foods and snack foods (USDA, 2013b) as well as fruits and vegetables, the overall dollar limit on the allotment (as well as limits on the monthly budgets of lower-income families who do not receive assistance) places a natural cap on food spending overall and demands careful planning regarding grocery expenditures. Food-insecure families, whose periodic hunger may render them equally if not more vulnerable than food-secure families to marketing-induced appetitive priming, are generally less able than food-secure families to fulfill their cravings with spontaneous shopping or food expenditures beyond those planned for any given pay period. It is interesting that food insecurity operated in this manner given that income was controlled in the regression analyses. Regardless of income, it seems that the emotional state of food insecurity, which appears based on the screening items to be an amalgam of worry and powerlessness, may be the driving factor behind its moderating role. The fact that commercial TV viewing and obesogenic dietary intake were correlated for food-secure but not food-insecure families raises the possibility that food-related modeling and priming processes are realized as action primarily among those who feel they can afford to take such action. Our findings also raise the possibility that food insecurity may temporarily protect children in food-insecure homes from marketing effects on excess dietary intake, even when appetitive priming occurs. Children cannot eat what their parents cannot buy. Food insecurity is exceptionally stressful (Bickel et al., 2000) so it would be unwise to argue that it is advantageous; still, the constraints it imposes on food purchasing may provide children limited protection against obesity during the preschool years. Findings for child fruit and vegetable intake were less consistent, with support for the predicted negative correlation emerging for child commercial TV viewing in food-secure families only. Perhaps persuasive marketing and media-induced appetitive priming processes occur primarily for obesogenic food, which is heavily advertised, and not fruits and vegetables, which are not (Gantz et al., 2007) and for which purchase and consumption are more strongly influenced by other factors. The fact that child commercial TV viewing predicted a more energy-dense pretend healthy meal for all children regardless of family food insecurity raises the possibility that family spending limits do not constrain the effects of direct exposure to marketing on children's developing meal schemas. Food-insecure children's perceptions of healthy meals as containing more energy-dense foods could influence their independent food choices when and if the economic constraints of food insecurity are lifted (Baskale & Bahar, 2011; Kandiah & Jones, 2002). If these children continue to develop in a state of food insecurity, qualifying for reduced-price school lunches increases the likelihood that they will eat school lunch as opposed to a home-packed lunch. Panel research has linked children's consumption of school lunches with increased overweight and obesity over time (Schanzenbach, 2009; but see Kimbro & Rigby, 2010, for the contextual influence of local factors such as food pricing). Children who enter school with the perception that energy-dense foods play a prominent role in healthy meals may end up selecting more of these foods when given the option in the school lunch line. These possibilities are beyond verification with the current data set, but they merit investigation in longitudinal research. At this point our interpretation of the mechanisms by which food insecurity moderates the potential effects of household commercial TV exposure on child dietary intake is speculative. At the very least, our data indicate that it would be wise to continue distinguishing food-secure from food-insecure families when exploring the mechanisms by which food marketing influences child dietary intake in the home. The purpose of including other media variables in this study was to compare a marketing-saturated medium (commercial TV) with media featuring little or no food marketing. The failure of most of the other media variables to predict child dietary intake and healthy-meal schemas may be due to the lack of food advertising in those media or to the possibility that relatively low exposure scores for the non-TV screen media (see Tables 1 & 2) reduced variance. Parent and child TV viewing are typically correlated because children and parents often watch together (Barradas et al., 2007), but the low correlations (r = .19 for children and r = .31 for parents) between commercial TV and DVR'd TV suggest that some households in our sample watched mostly commercial TV whereas others watched mostly DVR'd TV. Commercial TV households may differ from other households on a number of factors linking media use and obesogenic food consumption through various leisure-time preferences. The positive correlations for child and parent book-reading and child fruit and vegetable intake in food-secure households are consistent with this possibility. What makes one household a book-reading, fruit-and-vegetable-eating dominion and another a commercial-TV-viewing, fast-food-eating dominion? Identifying how media and dietary variables cluster together should be a central goal of future research. Although this study reported some compelling findings, it has some important limitations. First, parent reports of media exposure and dietary intake are more susceptible to error than observational data, so supplemental research involving in-home observation of family media and dietary habits seems necessary. The magnitude of the observed relationships was also relatively modest, and the survey design precludes conclusions about causality. Certainly, commercial media exposure is but one of many factors influencing families' food-buying decisions and cannot be expected to account for a large portion of the variance in family dietary intake. At the same time, even a small influence is important because TV habits are much more easily changed than factors such as family income or the community retail food environment. Just as nutrient-dense foods are not void of energy, energy-dense foods are not void of nutrients; therefore the distinction between energy-dense and nutrient-dense foods for the meal schema task was somewhat arbitrary, as a diet high in energy-dense foods can be quite nutritious. However, in terms of nutritional “bang for the buck,” we maintain that low-to-moderate-energy, micronutrient-dense foods such as whole-grain spaghetti, carrots, grapes, and milk are preferable to energy-dense, low-to-moderate-micronutrient foods such a doughnuts, potato salad, Jello, and fruit punch. This is because more vital macronutrients and micronutrients can be delivered by the first set of foods without excessive energy. Also, independent of energy intake, adequate intake of micronutrients delays degenerative aging and is more healthful than inadequate intake (Ames, 2006). The Placemat Protocol yields representations of meals as children imagine them but does not offer information on what children consume. The average pretend healthy meal for our child sample featured about eight items. Research on children's food plating preferences points to an average of seven different items and six different colors (compared to three items and three colors for adults; Zampollo, Kniffin, Wansink, & Shimuzu, 2011), so a childhood fondness for variety has been empirically documented and is not unique to our sample. Food placed on a plate is not necessarily food eaten, but increased variety and larger portion sizes both encourage greater intake among children and adults (Wansink, 2006). If larger pretend meals predict the composition of larger real meals, then children who view more commercial TV may not only compose more energy-dense meals even when trying to eat healthily, but end up consuming more calories too. Lastly, just as adults do not always eat what they believe is healthiest, children's perceptions of healthy eating will not necessarily guide their food choices. However, as nutritional curricula have led to more healthful independent food choices in children as young as 6 (Baskale & Bahar, 2011; Kandiah & Jones, 2002), interventions aimed at altering children's healthy-meal schemas to include more accurate perceptions of healthy eating should increase the frequency with which children choose nutrient-dense foods over energy-dense foods when they begin making dietary decisions outside the scope of parental supervision. This project did not involve collecting healthy-meal schema data for parents, so the correspondence between parents' and children's schemas is unknown. Likewise, without data recording foods available in the home, we are unable to document whether parent commercial TV exposure was directly correlated with family food buying behavior. Whatever the case, the relationships between family media exposure, family dietary habits, and parent–child discussions about food and health are undoubtedly complex and vary enormously within and between food-secure and food-insecure families. The data summarized here underscore the importance of measuring food insecurity in future attempts to map media and marketing effects on early childhood obesity and overweight, along with the need for more observational research to document these dynamics and their contributions to children's developing meal schemas in the early-life context of the family home. A better understanding of how the processes of media-produced modeling and appetitive priming function within specific family constraints, financial or otherwise, will be necessary to produce feasible, realistic, and individually tailored strategies to help families meet their children's nutritional needs while educating children to make the most healthful dietary choices possible when they begin assembling real meals on their own. Acknowledgments The STRONG Kids Team is a transdisciplinary collaborative at the University of Illinois including founding members Kelly K. Bost, Brent A. McBride, Sharon M. Donovan, Diana S. Grigsby-Toussaint, Janet M. Liechty, Angela Wiley, Margarita Teran-Garcia, and Barbara Fiese. The development of the Placemat Protocol was funded by the Illinois Council for Food and Agriculture Research, the Illinois Department of Human Services, and the USDA. Thanks to Sarah Brennan, Jennifer Lynne Buechel, Evan Carpenter, Rachel Hamden, Ana Hauser, Jessica Kaltz, Dugan Kemple, Jane Kim, Julia Lipowski, Jennifer Park, Elisse Rodriguez, Anna Spigelman, Nicole Tung, and Hannah Weiner for data collection. Notes 1 " WIC refers to the U.S. Department of Agriculture's Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children. The program provides funding for “supplemental foods, health care referrals, and nutrition education for low-income pregnant, breastfeeding, and nonbreastfeeding postpartum women, and to infants and children up to age five who are found to be at nutritional risk.” (http://www.fns.usda.gov/wic/women-infants-and-children-wic) 2 " The vegetable food models included peas, carrots, and green beans (nutrient-dense) and French fries, potato salad, and pumpkin pie (energy-dense). Fruits included bananas, apples, and grapes (nutrient-dense), and Jello, gummy fruit snacks, and chocolate-covered raisins (energy-dense). Grains included oatmeal, whole-grain bread, and whole-grain spaghetti (nutrient-dense), and brownies, doughnuts, and tortilla chips (energy-dense). Dairy included string cheese, cheddar cheese, and cottage cheese (nutrient-dense), and cheesecake, ice cream, and grilled cheese on white bread (energy-dense). Meats/beans included sliced turkey, grilled chicken, and beans (nutrient-dense), and fried chicken nuggets, hot dogs, and fried eggs (energy-dense). Beverages included milk, orange juice, and water (nutrient-dense), and fruit punch, chocolate milk, and soda pop (energy-dense). Using portion size data stamped on each food model and nutrition data available through the USDA National Nutrient Database for Standard Reference (http://ndb.nal.usda.gov/), we were able to verify that the energy-dense foods in each category collectively had significantly more estimated Kcal than the nutrient-dense foods in the same category, p < .001. References Ames , B. N. ( 2006 ). Low micronutrient intake may accelerate the degenerative diseases of aging through allocation of scarce micronutrients by triage . Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 103 ( 47 ), 17589 – 17594 . doi:10.1073/pnas.0608757103. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Bandura , A. ( 2002 ). Social cognitive theory of mass communication. In J. Bryant, & D. Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 121 – 154 ). Mahwah, NJ : Erlbaum . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Barradas , D. T. , Fulton , J. E., Blanck , H. M., & Huhman , M. ( 2007 ). Parental influences on youth television viewing . Journal of Pediatrics , 151 , 369 – 373 . doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2007.04.069. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Baskale , H. , & Bahar , Z. ( 2011 ). Outcomes of nutrition knowledge and healthy food choices in 5- to 6-year-old children who received a nutrition intervention based on Piaget's theory . Journal for Specialists in Pediatric Nursing , 16 , 263 – 279 . doi:10.1111/j.1744-6155.2011.00300.x. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Bickel , G. , Nord , M., Price , C., Hamilton , W., & Cook , J. ( 2000 ). Guide to measuring household food security. Retrieved from http://www.fns.usda.gov/fsec/FILES/FSGuide.pdf Centers for Disease Control & Prevention . ( 2007 ). Childhood obesity. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/nccdphp/dnpa/obesity/childhood/ Centers for Disease Control & Prevention . ( 2010 ). Measuring children's height and weight accurately at home. Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/healthyweight/assessing/bmi/childrens_bmi/measuring_children.html#Height Crespo , C. J. , Smit , E., Troiano , R. P., Bartlett , S. J., Macera , C. A., & Andersen , R. E. ( 2001 ). Television watching, energy intake, and obesity in U.S. children . JAMA Pediatrics , 155 ( 3 ), 360 – 365 . doi:10.1001/archpedi.155.3.360. OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Dinour , L. M. , Bergen , D., & Yeh , M. ( 2007 ). The food insecurity-obesity paradox: A review of the literature and the role food stamps may play . Journal of the American Dietetic Association , 107 , 1952 – 1961 . doi:10.1016/j.jada.2007.08.006. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Gantz , W. , Schwartz , N. C., Angelini , J. R., & Rideout , V. ( 2007 ). Food for thought: Television food advertising to children in the United States . Washington, DC : Kaiser Family Foundation . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Gearhardt , A. N. , & Brownell , K. D. ( 2013 ). The importance of understanding the impact of children's food marketing on the brain . Journal of Pediatrics , 62 ( 4 ), 672 – 673 . doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2012.11.068. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Harris , J. L. , Bargh , J. A., & Brownell , K. D. ( 2009 ). Priming effects of television food advertising on eating behavior . Health Psychology , 28 ( 4 ), 404 – 413 . doi:10.1037/a0014399. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Harrison , K. ( 2005 ). Is ‘fat free’ good for me? A panel study of television viewing and children's nutritional knowledge and reasoning . Health Communication , 17 ( 2 ), 117 – 132 . doi:10.1207/s15327027hc1702_1. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Harrison , K. , Bost , K. K., McBride , B. A., Donovan , S. M., Grigsby-Toussaint , D. S., Kim , J., … Jacobsohn , G. C. ( 2011 ). Toward a developmental conceptualization of contributors to overweight and obesity in childhood: The Six-Cs Model . Child Development Perspectives , 5 , 50 – 58 . doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00150.x. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Harrison , K. , & Liechty , J. ( 2012 ). Preschoolers' media exposure and dietary habits: The primacy of television and the limits of parental mediation . Journal of Children and Media , 6 ( 1 ), 18 – 36 . doi:10.1080/17482798.2011.633402. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Harrison , K. , & Marske , A. L. ( 2005 ). Nutritional content of foods advertised during the television programs children watch most . American Journal of Public Health , 95 ( 9 ), 1568 – 1574 . doi:10.2105/AJPH.2004.048058. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Harrison , K. , & Peralta , M. ( 2013 , June). The placemat protocol: Parent TV viewing predicts energy-dense food choices in preschoolers' pretend healthy meals. Paper presented at the 63rd Annual Conference of the International Communication Association, London, United Kingdom. Hingle , M. , & Kunkel , D. ( 2012 ). Childhood obesity and the media . Pediatric Clinics of North America , 59 ( 3 ), 677 – 692 . doi:10.1016/j.pcl.2012.03.021. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Holub , S. C. , & Musher-Eizenman , D. R. ( 2010 ). Examining preschoolers' nutrition knowledge using a meal creation and food group classification task: Age and gender differences . Early Child Development and Care , 180 , 787 – 798 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Institute of Medicine ( 2006 ). Food marketing to children and youth: Threat or opportunity? Washington, DC : Author . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Jordan , A. B. , & Robinson , T. N. ( 2008 ). Children, television viewing, and weight status: Summary and recommendations from an expert panel meeting . The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science , 615 , 119 – 132 . doi:10.1177/0002716207308681. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Kandiah , J. , & Jones , C. ( 2002 ). Nutrition knowledge and food choices of elementary school children . Early Child Development and Care , 172 ( 3 ), 269 – 273 . doi:10.1080/03004430212123. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Kim , J. , & Dev , D. ( 2011 ). Child care nutrition environment: Is it related to children's fruit and vegetable intake? FASEB Journal , 25 , 232.3 . OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Kimbro , R. T. , & Rigby , E. ( 2010 ). Federal food policy and childhood obesity: A solution or part of the problem? Health Affairs , 29 ( 3 ), 411 – 418 . doi:10.1377/hlthaff.2009.0731. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Lakshman , R. R. , Sharp , S. J., Ong , K. K., & Forouhi , N. G. ( 2010 ). A novel school-based intervention to improve nutrition knowledge in children: Cluster randomized controlled trial . BMC Public Health , 10 , 123 . doi:10.1186/1471-2458-10-123. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Lowe , M. R. , & Butryn , M. L. ( 2007 ). Hedonic hunger: A new dimension of appetite . Physiology and Behavior , 91 , 432 – 439 . doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.04.006. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat McCarthy , A. , Hughes , R., Tilling , K., Davies , D., Smith , G. D., & Ben-Shlomo , Y. ( 2007 ). Birth weight; postnatal, infant, and childhood growth; and obesity in young adulthood: Evidence from the Barry Caerphilly Growth Study . American Journal of Clinical Nutrition , 86 , 907 – 913 . Google Scholar PubMed OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat McNeal , J. ( 1998 ). Tapping the three kids' markets . American Demographics , 20 , 37 – 41 . OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Miller , S. A. , Taveras , E. M., Rifas-Shiman , S. L., & Gillman , M. W. ( 2008 ). Association between television viewing and poor diet quality in young children . International Journal of Pediatric Obesity , 3 ( 3 ), 168 – 176 . doi:10.1080/17477160801915935. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat National Center for Health Statistics ( 2000a ). 2 to 20 years: Boys stature-for-age and weight-for age percentiles . Atlanta, GA : Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/growthcharts/data/set1clinical/cj41c021.pdf. Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC National Center for Health Statistics ( 2000b ). 2 to 20 years: Girls stature-for-age and weight-for age percentiles . Atlanta, GA : Centers for Disease Control and Prevention . Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/growthcharts/data/set1clinical/cj41c022.pdf. Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Nguyen , S. P. ( 2007 ). An apple a day keeps the doctor away: Children's evaluative categories of food . Appetite , 48 , 114 – 118 . doi:10.1016/j.appet.2006.06.001. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Nguyen , S. P. , Gordon , C. L., & McCullough , M. B. ( 2011 ). Not as easy as pie: Disentangling the theoretical and applied components of children's health knowledge . Appetite , 56 , 265 – 268 . doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.01.008. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Oates , C. , Blades , M., & Gunter , B. ( 2002 ). Children and television advertising: When do they understand persuasive intent? Journal of Consumer Behaviour , 1 , 238 – 245 . doi:10.1002/cb.69. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Ogden , C. L. , Carroll , M. D., Kit , B. K., & Flegal , K. M. ( 2014 ). Prevalence of childhood and adult obesity in the United States, 2011–2012 . JAMA , 311 ( 8 ), 806 – 814 . doi:10.1001/jama.2014.732. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Roberts , D. F. , & Foehr , U. G. ( 2004 ). Kids and media in America . Cambridge, England : Cambridge University Press . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Scholar Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Rolland-Cachera , M. F. , Deheeger , M., Maillot , M., & Bellisle , F. ( 2006 ). Early adiposity rebound: Causes and consequences for obesity in children and adults . International Journal of Obesity , 30 , S11 – S17 . doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0803514. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Schanzenbach , D. W. ( 2009 ). Do school lunches contribute to childhood obesity? Journal of Human Resources , 44 ( 3 ), 684 – 709 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Singleton , J. C. , Achterberg , C. L., & Shannon , B. M. ( 1992 ). Role of food and nutrition in the health perceptions of young children . Journal of the American Dietetic Association , 92 , 67 – 70 . doi:10.1177/1049732309334104. Google Scholar PubMed OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Skinner , J. , Carruth , B. R., Moran , J., Houck , K., Schmidhammer , J., Reed , A., … Ott , D. ( 1998 ). Toddlers' food preferences: Concordance with family members' preferences . Journal of Nutrition Education , 30 , 17 – 22 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Sonneville , K. R. , Rifas-Shiman , S. L., Kleinman , K. P., Gortmaker , S. L., Gillman , M. W., & Taveras , E. M. ( 2012 ). Associations of obesogenic behaviors in mothers and obese children participating in a randomized trial . Obesity , 20 , 1449 – 1454 . doi:10.1038/oby.2012.43. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Towensend , M. S. , Peerson , J., Love , B., Achterberg , C., & Murphy , S. P. ( 2001 ). Food insecurity is positively related to overweight in women . Journal of Nutrition , 131 , 1738 – 1745 . Google Scholar PubMed OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat United States Department of Agriculture . ( 2012 ). Health and nutrition information for preschoolers. Retrieved from www.choosemyplate.gov/preschoolers.html United States Department of Agriculture . ( 2013a ). Supplemental nutritional assistance program: Eligibility. Retrieved from http://www.fns.usda.gov/snap/applicant_recipients/eligibility.htm United States Department of Agriculture . ( 2013b ). Supplemental nutritional assistance program: Eligible food items. Retrieved from http://www.fns.usda.gov/snap/retailers/eligible.htm United States Department of Education ( 2008 ). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study, Birth Cohort (ECLS-B): Longitudinal 8-month-preschool restricted-use data file (NCES-2008-024) . Washington, DC : National Center for Education Statistics, Institute of Educational Sciences, U. S. Department of Education . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC United States Department of Health and Human Services . ( 2013 ). 2013 poverty guidelines. Retrieved from http://aspe.hhs.gov/poverty/13poverty.cfm#thresholds Wansink , B. ( 2006 ). Mindless eating: Why we eat more than we think . New York, NY : Bantam-Dell . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Wethington , E. , & Johnson-Askew , W. L. ( 2009 ). Contributions of the life course perspective to research on food decision-making . Annals of Behavioral Medicine , 38 ( Suppl. 1 ), S74 – S80 . doi:10.1007/s12160-009-9123-6. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Zampollo , F. , Kniffin , K. M., Wansink , B., & Shimuzu , M. ( 2011 ). Food plating preferences of children: The importance of presentation on desire for diversity . Acta Paediatrica , 101 , 61 – 66 . doi:10.1111/j.1651-2227.2011.02409.x. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat © 2015 International Communication Association TI - Parent and Child Media Exposure, Preschooler Dietary Intake, and Preschooler Healthy-Meal Schemas in the Context of Food Insecurity JF - Journal of Communication DO - 10.1111/jcom.12153 DA - 2015-06-01 UR - https://www.deepdyve.com/lp/oxford-university-press/parent-and-child-media-exposure-preschooler-dietary-intake-and-xLCpt0xFnx SP - 443 VL - 65 IS - 3 DP - DeepDyve ER -