TY - JOUR AU1 - Galán,, Julia AU2 - Felip,, Francisco AU3 - García-García,, Carlos AU4 - Contero,, Manuel AB - Abstract This research aimed to analyse the influence of presentation means in assessing different household product characteristics and to study the influence of physical contact with the product on that assessment. To this end, the presentation of an armchair in four different means was prepared: two offering the chance to touch the product (real setting and virtual reality with passive haptics) and two not offering the physical interaction possibility (virtual reality and 3D interactive image on a screen). The product was assessed by 128 volunteers (74 men, 54 women) on a semantic scale with 12 bipolar pairs. The results revealed that the presentation means did not influence the overall product assessment, but affected the assessment of 3 of 12 features (weight, size, and aesthetics), where coming into physical contact with the product impacted the assessment of these features. Finally, similar assessments of the product were obtained in both means of visual-only presentation. Graphical Abstract Open in new tabDownload slide Graphical Abstract Open in new tabDownload slide virtual reality, passive haptics, product presentation, product evaluation, semantic differential Highlights The presentation means impacted the assessment of 3 of 12 characteristics. The presentation means had no influence on the overall product assessment. Physical contact with the product impacts the assessment of some characteristics. Visual-only presentation means led to similar product assessments. 1. Introduction Presentations of physical products in real settings (conventional shops, showrooms) currently co-exist with several image-based presentation telematic formats (e-commerce). In recent years, online product presentations have become more frequent owing to the ever-increasing presence of e-commerce (Jiang & Benbasat, 2007; Yoo & Kim, 2014). As new applications emerge, visual product presentation formats adapt to offer more information to potential buyers. Indeed, many shops have gone from using an online catalogue with static images to an online catalogue with interactive images, and recently to using virtual reality (VR) to show customers their products. Several studies have concluded that different factors influence product assessments and later purchase decisions. Unal (2017) defends the importance of acquiring previous product knowledge for customers to go ahead with or go back on these decisions; Jalil, Fikry, and Zainuddin (2016) see the setting surrounding the product as a factor that can determine how it is assessed, and Chen (2018) defends product aesthetics as a conditioning factor. Other researchers (Algharabat, Alalwan, Rana, & Dwivedi, 2017; Wu et al., 2016) have verified that the quality of the images employed to present a product may make its understanding and assessment easy or difficult. Flavián, Gurrea, and Orús (2009) also add that the size of these images and movement could also be factors that contribute to purchase decision-making by helping consumers better understand the extent of the presented product's usability. The increased calculation power of home computers and the prices of VR devices going down have favoured some businesses including this technology into their online catalogues. This presentation format allows coming into contact with a product more immersively and interactively than screens to experiment with the product's 3D volumetrics, which approaches real volumetrics more. This provides users with more information and improves their experience (Grewal, Noble, Roggeveen, & Nordfalt, 2020; Ozok & Komlodi, 2009; Verhagen, Vonkeman, Feldberg, & Verhagen, 2014). This can also improve consumers’ product knowledge and favour their purchase intention (Jiang & Benbasat, 2004; Suh & Lee, 2005). All this evidences the importance that image currently has on product presentation, and how it influences the way a product's characteristics and functions are perceived. In merely visual product presentation means, it is important to truthfully transmit the product's real characteristics so that consumers are suitably informed before they make a purchase decision (Saraswati, 2018). As part of the new product design process, we see how apart from physical prototypes, virtual prototypes are also often used in their first development stages because they are cheaper and more versatile (Cecil & Kanchanapiboon, 2007). In this case, the virtual prototype must allow users to realistically experiment with the functions and characteristics that the real product will have. So essentially, this representation must be capable of transmitting reliable information. Some studies defend that VR is a suitable means in which to assess products in their different development stages (Bordegoni & Ferrise, 2013; Violante, Marcolin, Vezzetti, Nonis, & Moos, 2019; Ye, Badiyani, Raja, & Schlege, 2007). They even propose using immersive spaces to obtain a realistic context that enables the product to be assessed under conditions that come as close to the real scenario used by users/consumers as possible (Delarue & Lageat, 2019), or to analyse how the product presentation context modulates the way its features are perceived (Naderi, Naderi, & Balakrishnan, 2020). Nonetheless, different factors can affect the way that products are perceived in these settings, such as the employed technology or users feeling its presence, which can also affect how the simulated space size and distances between the various objects forming them are perceived (Ebrahimi, 2017; Ebrahimi, Babu, Pagano, & Joerg, 2016; Lok, Naik, Whitton, & Brooks, 2003; Willemsen, Gooch, Thompson, & Creem-Regehr, 2008), which may affect product assessments. Other studies also demonstrate the validity of interactive 3D images presented on screens as a resource for assessing products. Some works have explored the advantages of manipulating 3D images with mobile devices to examine the presented objects’ volumetrics by several techniques (Bergé, Dubois, & Raynal, 2015), while others have used such images to assess certain affective qualities of vehicle interiors (Park, Park, Kim, Choe, & Jung, 2014) to help decision-making during the design process. Nonetheless, completely visual presentation means do not allow products to be touched, so it is impossible to completely and reliably perceive some product characteristics, such as texture, the material's temperature to the touch, or its comfort when used or handled. Therefore, lack of tactile possibilities in visual presentation means can prevent accessing part of the product information, which could alter how some product characteristics are perceived. This could influence the product assessment during either the design process or the presentation process used to sell it (Steinmann, Kilian, & Brylla, 2014). In order to avoid this, passive haptics (PH), active haptics (AH), and pseudo-haptics can be added to the VR experience. In a VR setting, PH, defined as the use of physical elements capable of providing information to users through their shape (Lindeman, Sibert, & Hahn, 1999), can provide users with new information that supplements entirely visual information by creating a much more immersive experience. This means that a VR setting with passive haptics (VRPH), in which the position of physical objects is synchronized with virtual objects, may improve the immersion sensation in a spatial setting (Azmandian, Hancock, Benko, Ofek, & Wilson, 2016; Insko, 2001). Moreover, AH is capable of transmitting to users the tactile sensation through a mechanical device with no physical object around. Several research works have employed wearable haptic interfaces, especially finger- and hand-related ones (Hinchet, Vechev, Shea, & Hilliges, 2018; Minamizawa, Fukamachi, Kajimoto, Kawakami, & Tachi, 2007; Minamizawa, Kamuro, Fukamachi, Kawakami, & Tachi, 2008; Pacchierotti et al., 2017), and demonstrated, on the one hand, their capacity to transmit a wide range of tactile sensations while handling virtual objects using systems that generate vibrations, pin arrays that deform skin to simulate specific shapes, and mechanisms capable of applying forces in various spatial directions (Prattichizzo, Chinello, Pacchierotti, & Malvezzi, 2013), and, on the other hand, their capacity to be simply transported to be used in different contexts. Likewise, former studies (Biocca, Inoue, Polinsky, Lee, & Tang, 2002) and other recent ones into pseudo-haptic techniques (Collins & Kapralos, 2019; Musashi, 2019) have shown that it is possible to provide haptic information via visual and auditory information, which allows the user's experience in the virtual world to improve. When assessing a product and determining the purchase experience, haptics and visual explorations have a notable influence (Luo, Shen, & Liu, 2019; Schifferstein & Cleiren, 2005). Visual presentations help consumers to form a view about a product, influence their purchase decision (Elder & Krishna, 2012; Krishna, 2012), and contribute to create mental simulations about their use form (Elder & Krishna, 2012; Schlosser, 2003), which favour product-related cognitive activities appearing that could impact the product assessment process (Barsalou 2008). Likewise, the information transmitted by touch is relevant for forming opinions, helps to better perceive products’ true quality (Krishna & Schwarz, 2014; Peck & Childers, 2003; Schwarz, 2012), and contributes to purchase decision-making (Keng, Liao, & Yang, 2012; Peck & Wiggins, 2006). Sometimes, certain visual characteristics (e.g. cold–warm colours) can influence how some physical characteristics (light–heavy, big–small) are perceived, which demonstrates that both are related to the mental representation of a product's features (Löffler, Arlt, Toriizuka, Tscharn, & Hurtienne, 2016; Löffler, Tscharn, & Hurtienne, 2018). Using PH in VR environments has reported many benefits in recent decades. From the very beginning, Hoffman et al. (1996), Hoffman (1998), and Insko (2001) demonstrated that employing physical objects in virtual settings allowed the simulation's level of realism to improve. This has helped advance in knowledge in different fields. For instance in the medical field, PH is a proven resource that helps to improve treatment efficacy for some phobias in VR settings (Carlin, Hoffman, & Weghorst, 1997; Tardif, Therrien, & Bouchard, 2019). In the marketing field, research has been conducted into the influence of being able to touch a product on purchase decision-making (Zenner et al., 2020). In the product design field, using PH helps to assess the usability of products in VR settings (Falcao & Soares, 2014), improve the efficiency of certain tasks that require a physical interaction (Carvalheiro, Nóbrega, da Silva, & Rodrigues, 2016), and develop HMI dashboards applied in simulation booths that can reduce learning times (Joyce & Robinson, 2017; Lassagne, Kemeny, Posselt, & Merienne, 2018). Recent studies also confirm that an active haptic exploring process, in which users explore the surface of a passive object with their own hands, helps to better recognize the surface properties of these objects and is, therefore, an interesting method to assess household products in VR (Velázquez et al., 2019). Thus, if the tactile side completes a user's opinion of a product, it is feasible to understand that presentation means can influence the way a product is understood and assessed depending on whether it allows physical contact with the product or not. Nowadays, several product presentation means exist that may interest salespersons to know how a product can be perceived in each means in order to select the means that better favours their interests. Similarly, during a product's design process, designers might be interested in selecting one presentation means or another so that users can more reliably assess certain product characteristics to help to readdress some design decisions. Given the interest shown in consumers assessing products in e-retailing, and in users assessing a product during the design process of new products, this study intended to find out how a product's characteristics are assessed in four different means that are presently employed in conventional shops, online shops, showrooms, and product design studies: two with only a visual interaction (an interactive 3D image viewed on a screen and a simulated VR immersive setting) and two with a visual–tactile interaction (a real setting with a product or physical prototype and a VR setting that allows the product to be touched). For this purpose, we built four different testing environments in four rooms, each one for presenting the same product in a different means. We carried out the experiment enrolling 128 volunteer participants who were divided into four subgroups, where each group had to evaluate the product presented in one means. We built a semantic differential scale with 12 bipolar pairs in order to assess different parameters of the product in a seven-interval scale. Finally, to study the results we carried out a statistical analysis and conducted Kruskal–Wallis, Shapiro–Wilk, and Dunn–Bonferroni tests. 2. Research Aim and Hypotheses Our research aim was, on the one hand, to analyse the influence of presentation means in assessing different household product characteristics and, on the other hand, to study the influence of physical contact with the product on that assessment. To this end, a same product was presented in four different means normally used today: two offered the chance to touch the product—real setting (R) and virtual reality with passive haptics (VRPH); two presented the product, but did not offer any physical interaction—virtual reality (VR) and 3D interactive image on a screen (S). This study put forward three initial hypotheses: H1: The means employed to present a household product influences users’ assessments. H2: The presence or absence of physical contact with a household product during its presentation influences its assessment. H3: Similar assessments are made when a household product with no physical contact is presented, regardless of the means. 3. Materials and Methods 3.1 Case study To test the above hypotheses, an experiment was designed in which a same product was presented in four different means. Each user had access to one of the four presentations, and interacted and studied the object according to the characteristics of the means it was presented in. The product chosen for this study was an armchair because it is a normal piece of household furniture known by all users. In order to place the armchair in a more realistic scene, some decorative objects were added. Having selected the product to be assessed and the four presentation means, scenes were prepared in four rooms: Room 1: real setting (R). The real armchair was placed along with the other decorative pieces. This experiment allowed users to approach the product, touch it, and to sit on it. Nonetheless, they could not interact with any other element placed in this room apart from standing or kneeling on the rug. Room 2: virtual reality (VR). The armchair and the rest of the scene were represented as VR. Using a VR headset, users could walk around the scene and look at the armchair from any viewing point, move their heads, and kneel down. However, they could neither touch any other element, nor stand or kneel on the rug, because these elements were not physically found in this room. Room 3: virtual reality with passive haptics (VRPH). In this room, the scene was exactly the same as in Room 2, but this time two physical elements were added: the armchair and the rug, which occupied the same places in their equivalent virtual experiments. The participants could touch the armchair, sit on it, look at it from any perspective, and could kneel or stand on the rug. Room 4: 3D interactive image on a screen (S). The same scene was shown as an interactive 3D image on a computer screen. When users moved the mouse, they could make the camera revolve from a static point to look at the scene from any angle. By using keys W (move forward) and S (move backward), they could move around the scene and around the armchair, and move closer to examine the armchair's details. The scenes shown in Rooms 2, 3, and 4 reproduced the same conditions of lighting, sizes, and the relative position of the elements as those that appeared in the scene in Room 1 (Fig. 2). 3.2. Semantic scale To assess the product in different means, a semantic differential scale was prepared of several bipolar pairs that acted as product descriptors. This way to collect data about a product's different parameters is quite common and has been previously used in several research works (Hsiao, Chiu, & Chen, 2008; Mondragón, Company, & Vergara, 2005). This study employed the semantic scale used by Felip, Galán, García-García, and Mulet (2019) because the product to be assessed in both cases was the same: an armchair. This semantic scale was built using the most normally employed adjectives to describe an armchair. In order to more objectively know these adjectives, three different sources were selected: 12 commercial websites offering habitat products, 70 habitual users of this product, and 10 professional designers. This was done because designers and users normally describe the product differently (Crozier, 1994), and it allowed a wide spectrum of adjectives to be obtained. The adjectives that described armchairs on websites were collected, as were the adjectives that both designers and users employ to describe the 15 different armchair typologies, which were displayed using images in which each armchair was presented on a white background. By assuming that the adjectives of the three samples were equally relevant, and bearing in mind that samples were not equal, the amount of adjectives in each sample was multiplied by a correction factor so that the three quantities of adjectives were comparable to one another. Then, we selected the most repeated adjectives in each sample, which were classified according to the four pleasure categories that products may offer, defined by Tiger (1992) as: Physio-pleasure (deriving from sensorial organs); Socio-pleasure (deriving from relationships with others); Psycho-pleasure (related to cognitive and emotional reactions); and Ideo-pleasure (related to values). We decided all this to obtain a sample of adjectives that was as representative of all four categories as possible, which has also been indicated by previous works (Achiche et al., 2014). In fields like marketing, this can be useful for studying how a means can impact the adjectives in a given category. However, in the present research work, which studies the product presentation means’ influence when widely assessing a product, it would seem more appropriate to contemplate the attributes in all the categories. Of these adjectives, only those mostly repeated in each category were selected to form bipolar pairs (12 in all, three for each pleasure category). We chose to use only 12 bipolar pairs to avoid the product assessment process becoming tedious for the participants, and also because this quantity seemed enough to obtain quite a complete product assessment. In fact, other studies have shown that a large quantity of bipolar pairs is not necessary to obtain relevant information in a product assessment (Achiche et al., 2014; Perez Mata et al., 2017). To evaluate each bipolar pair, a seven-interval scale was used because it allowed the participants to assess the product reliably and quite easily, as former studies have demonstrated (Al-Hindawe, 1996) (Fig. 1). Figure 1: Open in new tabDownload slide List of the bipolar pairs used to assess the armchair on a seven-interval scale. Figure 1: Open in new tabDownload slide List of the bipolar pairs used to assess the armchair on a seven-interval scale. 3.3. Stimulus To run the experiment, four different scenes were prepared (viewed in Rooms 1 to 4). All the scenes displayed the same product to be assessed, which was presented with a series of neutral furnishing elements to help to contextualize the object inside its setting. The product to be assessed was the dark grey Vedbo armchair from the 2020 Ikea catalogue. The other elements were also obtained from the Ikea catalogue: a beige rug (Adum), a white side table (Lack), a white plant pot (Papaja), and two white frames from the Ribba collection. To display the scene, a room was created in neutral tones to avoid distracting the participants: medium-grey walls and floor, and a light grey ceiling. Cenital lighting allowed all the product's details and texture to be suitably observed so they could be assessed, and was fitted more or less centrally to the room. The Room 1 scene was prepared by employing all the real furnishing elements. To prepare scenes in Rooms 2, 3, and 4, all the furnishing elements were modelled, as were the floor, walls, and ceiling. To ensure that the visual aspect was as similar as possible to the real setting, textures scanned from the real objects were applied. The virtual setting displayed in Rooms 2, 3, and 4 was prepared with Unity 2017.4.1. To view the scenes in Rooms 2 and 3, HTC Vive 0PJT100 headsets were employed because recent studies have demonstrated that they provide a sufficiently high degree of distance perception accuracy (Hornsey, Hibbard, & Scarfe, 2020), along with HTC Vive 2PR8100 Base Station position sensors. To view the Room 4 scene, an HP 250 G6 Notebook PC laptop was used with screen size 15.6 inches, 1920 × 1080 resolution, and a Genuine Alienware MODMUO USB mouse. 3.4 Sampling To take part in our experiment, 128 volunteers offered, of whom 74 were men and 54 were women. Their age range went from 18 to 28 years, and their mean age was 20.25 years. All the participants were studying the Degree in Industrial Design and Product Development Engineering at the Universitat Jaume I of Castellón in Spain. 3.5 Experiment protocol The experiment took place in the morning on two days of the same week. The sample subjects were randomly divided into four groups so that each group was formed by a similar number of subjects to view each presentation means: group 1 (32 subjects) in Room 1 (R); group 2 (31) in Room 2 (VR); group 3 (32) in Room 3 (VRPH); and group 4 (33) in Room 4 (S). Figure 2 illustrates the images inside all four rooms. Figure 2: Open in new tabDownload slide The participants in the different scene rooms: R (Room 1); VR (Room 2); VRPH (Room 3); S (Room 4). Figure 2: Open in new tabDownload slide The participants in the different scene rooms: R (Room 1); VR (Room 2); VRPH (Room 3); S (Room 4). The protocol was written and it helped the researchers to follow the sequence of steps during the experiment, and to address the participants in the same way: STAGE 1. Welcome room (2 min). Step 1. When the participants arrived at reception, they were registered, and asked to read and sign the consent form to perform the experiment. Then, they were accompanied to the room where the experiment would take place. STAGE 2. Scene rooms (5 min). Step 2. When entering Rooms 2 (VR) and 3 (VRPH), the researchers placed and fitted headsets on the participants’ heads. A panel separated the small entrance area from the bigger area in which users would be presented the scene, so they could not see anything beforehand. The users directly accessed Rooms 1 (R) and 4 (S) without having to walk around a panel. Step 3. When the participants wore headsets (Rooms 2 and 3), or had just entered (Rooms 1 and 4), a research staff member informed them that they were about to view a scene in which an armchair would be presented, which they had to later assess using a questionnaire. In each room, they were told about all the various procedures followed to view the product: in Rooms 1, 2, and 3, the participants could move around the armchair, move closer to it, move away from it, or kneel down to observe it from different viewing points. In Rooms 1 and 3, they could also touch and sit on the armchair. In Room 4, the participants were explained how they could move around the 3D setting: by moving the mouse, the users could see any part of the scene from the position they were in, and could move forward and backward using keys W and S to move the camera around the armchair, and to look closer at its details. An instruction sheet was left next to the laptop. Step 4. For 2 min, each participant could freely view the product according to the conditions in each scene. In Rooms 2 and 3, the research staff members silently held the headset wire so that the participants would not get tangled in it. Step 5. With the research staff members’ help, the participants in Rooms 2 and 3 removed the headset behind the panel. Then, the participants in all the rooms were given a printed questionnaire and were shown where they had to go to fill it in. STAGE 3. Survey room (5 min). Step 6. The users filled in the questionnaire in silence. The research staff members helped the participants with any doubts they had. The intention of the first group of questions was to collect data about the participants’ age, gender, and if they had possible viewing problems that could have conditioned their visual experience using VR headsets. The purpose of the second group of questions was to know the participants’ assessments of the presented product. To do so, each participant had to assess the armchair according to the 12 bipolar pairs indicated in Fig. 1. The sentence read: 'Rate the armchair you just saw according to whether you think it is closer or further away from the following adjectives (write an X in the appropriate checkbox)' for each semantic pair. The participants were asked if they had felt confident when making their assessment using one of two options: 'Yes' or 'No' ('Did you feel completely confident when you made the assessment?'). Then, they were asked to give an overall score as to how much they liked the armchair on a 5-point Likert scale, where 1 was the lowest score ('I do not like it at all') and 5 was the highest score ('I like it very much'). A blank space was left in which the participants could write any comment about the experiment that they wished to communicate to the researchers ('Would you like to add any comments about the experiment?'). Step 7. Finally, questionnaires were collected and each participant was thanked for collaborating and was accompanied to the exit. 4. Results In order to confirm or not the initial hypotheses, an inferential statistical analysis was carried out using the data obtained from the questionnaires conducted about the experiments run in the various means (R, VRPH, VR, and S). Two data types were collected: to assess the different product characteristics (using semantic pairs) and to globally assess the product (the 'I like it' question). The descriptive statistics and box plots for each semantic scale are shown in Table 1 and Fig. 3, respectively, while the overall assessment and its box plot are presented in Table 2 and Fig. 4, respectively. Figure 3: Open in new tabDownload slide Box plots for the semantic scales (*indicates significant differences in the Kruskal–Wallis test; **indicates significant differences in pair-wise comparisons using the Dunn–Bonferroni test). Figure 3: Open in new tabDownload slide Box plots for the semantic scales (*indicates significant differences in the Kruskal–Wallis test; **indicates significant differences in pair-wise comparisons using the Dunn–Bonferroni test). Figure 4: Open in new tabDownload slide Box plot and histogram for the overall assessment. Figure 4: Open in new tabDownload slide Box plot and histogram for the overall assessment. Table 1: Descriptive statistics for the semantic scales. . . . Conditions . . Semantic scales . . R (N = 32) . VRPH (N = 32) . VR (N = 31) . S (N = 33) . Physio Comfortable–Uncomfortable Mean −1.87 −1.75 −1.97 −1.73 Median −2.00 −2.00 −2.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. .75 1.27 .66 .72 Heavy–Light Mean .56 −1.09 −.32 −.27 Median 1.00 −1.00 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. 1.50 1.25 1.38 1.57 Large-sized–Small-sized Mean −1.87 −1.47 −.77 −.67 Median −2.00 −2.00 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. .94 1.05 .96 1.45 Psycho Simple–Complex Mean −1.34 −.87 −1.03 −1.27 Median −1.50 −1.00 −1.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. 1.04 1.04 1.43 1.44 Useless–Practical Mean 1.75 1.56 1.74 1.66 Median 2.00 2.00 2.00 2.00 Std. Dev. .95 .87 .93 .92 Disproportionate–Well-proportionate Mean .81 1.53 1.68 1.63 Median 1.00 2.00 2.00 2.00 Std. Dev. 1.53 1.22 1.05 1.17 Socio Classic–Modern Mean .97 .87 .93 .88 Median 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 Std. Dev. 1.36 1.10 1.46 1.71 Ugly–Nice Mean 2.21 1.66 1.84 1.46 Median 2.00 2.00 2.00 1.00 Std. Dev. .71 .90 .86 .94 Original–Common Mean −.50 −.59 −.45 −.03 Median −1.00 −1.00 −1.00 .00 Std. Dev. 1.32 1.32 1.41 1.66 Ideo Tasteful–Tasteless Mean −1.28 −1.37 −1.29 −1.54 Median −2.00 −1.00 −1.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. 1.14 .75 .78 .83 Boring–Fun Mean .25 .22 .29 −1.15 Median .00 .00 .00 .00 Std. Dev. .62 .97 .94 1.09 Timeless–Temporary Mean −.72 −.53 −.67 −.54 Median −1.00 −.50 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. 1.25 1.29 1.30 1.54 . . . Conditions . . Semantic scales . . R (N = 32) . VRPH (N = 32) . VR (N = 31) . S (N = 33) . Physio Comfortable–Uncomfortable Mean −1.87 −1.75 −1.97 −1.73 Median −2.00 −2.00 −2.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. .75 1.27 .66 .72 Heavy–Light Mean .56 −1.09 −.32 −.27 Median 1.00 −1.00 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. 1.50 1.25 1.38 1.57 Large-sized–Small-sized Mean −1.87 −1.47 −.77 −.67 Median −2.00 −2.00 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. .94 1.05 .96 1.45 Psycho Simple–Complex Mean −1.34 −.87 −1.03 −1.27 Median −1.50 −1.00 −1.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. 1.04 1.04 1.43 1.44 Useless–Practical Mean 1.75 1.56 1.74 1.66 Median 2.00 2.00 2.00 2.00 Std. Dev. .95 .87 .93 .92 Disproportionate–Well-proportionate Mean .81 1.53 1.68 1.63 Median 1.00 2.00 2.00 2.00 Std. Dev. 1.53 1.22 1.05 1.17 Socio Classic–Modern Mean .97 .87 .93 .88 Median 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 Std. Dev. 1.36 1.10 1.46 1.71 Ugly–Nice Mean 2.21 1.66 1.84 1.46 Median 2.00 2.00 2.00 1.00 Std. Dev. .71 .90 .86 .94 Original–Common Mean −.50 −.59 −.45 −.03 Median −1.00 −1.00 −1.00 .00 Std. Dev. 1.32 1.32 1.41 1.66 Ideo Tasteful–Tasteless Mean −1.28 −1.37 −1.29 −1.54 Median −2.00 −1.00 −1.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. 1.14 .75 .78 .83 Boring–Fun Mean .25 .22 .29 −1.15 Median .00 .00 .00 .00 Std. Dev. .62 .97 .94 1.09 Timeless–Temporary Mean −.72 −.53 −.67 −.54 Median −1.00 −.50 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. 1.25 1.29 1.30 1.54 Open in new tab Table 1: Descriptive statistics for the semantic scales. . . . Conditions . . Semantic scales . . R (N = 32) . VRPH (N = 32) . VR (N = 31) . S (N = 33) . Physio Comfortable–Uncomfortable Mean −1.87 −1.75 −1.97 −1.73 Median −2.00 −2.00 −2.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. .75 1.27 .66 .72 Heavy–Light Mean .56 −1.09 −.32 −.27 Median 1.00 −1.00 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. 1.50 1.25 1.38 1.57 Large-sized–Small-sized Mean −1.87 −1.47 −.77 −.67 Median −2.00 −2.00 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. .94 1.05 .96 1.45 Psycho Simple–Complex Mean −1.34 −.87 −1.03 −1.27 Median −1.50 −1.00 −1.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. 1.04 1.04 1.43 1.44 Useless–Practical Mean 1.75 1.56 1.74 1.66 Median 2.00 2.00 2.00 2.00 Std. Dev. .95 .87 .93 .92 Disproportionate–Well-proportionate Mean .81 1.53 1.68 1.63 Median 1.00 2.00 2.00 2.00 Std. Dev. 1.53 1.22 1.05 1.17 Socio Classic–Modern Mean .97 .87 .93 .88 Median 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 Std. Dev. 1.36 1.10 1.46 1.71 Ugly–Nice Mean 2.21 1.66 1.84 1.46 Median 2.00 2.00 2.00 1.00 Std. Dev. .71 .90 .86 .94 Original–Common Mean −.50 −.59 −.45 −.03 Median −1.00 −1.00 −1.00 .00 Std. Dev. 1.32 1.32 1.41 1.66 Ideo Tasteful–Tasteless Mean −1.28 −1.37 −1.29 −1.54 Median −2.00 −1.00 −1.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. 1.14 .75 .78 .83 Boring–Fun Mean .25 .22 .29 −1.15 Median .00 .00 .00 .00 Std. Dev. .62 .97 .94 1.09 Timeless–Temporary Mean −.72 −.53 −.67 −.54 Median −1.00 −.50 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. 1.25 1.29 1.30 1.54 . . . Conditions . . Semantic scales . . R (N = 32) . VRPH (N = 32) . VR (N = 31) . S (N = 33) . Physio Comfortable–Uncomfortable Mean −1.87 −1.75 −1.97 −1.73 Median −2.00 −2.00 −2.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. .75 1.27 .66 .72 Heavy–Light Mean .56 −1.09 −.32 −.27 Median 1.00 −1.00 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. 1.50 1.25 1.38 1.57 Large-sized–Small-sized Mean −1.87 −1.47 −.77 −.67 Median −2.00 −2.00 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. .94 1.05 .96 1.45 Psycho Simple–Complex Mean −1.34 −.87 −1.03 −1.27 Median −1.50 −1.00 −1.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. 1.04 1.04 1.43 1.44 Useless–Practical Mean 1.75 1.56 1.74 1.66 Median 2.00 2.00 2.00 2.00 Std. Dev. .95 .87 .93 .92 Disproportionate–Well-proportionate Mean .81 1.53 1.68 1.63 Median 1.00 2.00 2.00 2.00 Std. Dev. 1.53 1.22 1.05 1.17 Socio Classic–Modern Mean .97 .87 .93 .88 Median 1.00 1.00 1.00 1.00 Std. Dev. 1.36 1.10 1.46 1.71 Ugly–Nice Mean 2.21 1.66 1.84 1.46 Median 2.00 2.00 2.00 1.00 Std. Dev. .71 .90 .86 .94 Original–Common Mean −.50 −.59 −.45 −.03 Median −1.00 −1.00 −1.00 .00 Std. Dev. 1.32 1.32 1.41 1.66 Ideo Tasteful–Tasteless Mean −1.28 −1.37 −1.29 −1.54 Median −2.00 −1.00 −1.00 −2.00 Std. Dev. 1.14 .75 .78 .83 Boring–Fun Mean .25 .22 .29 −1.15 Median .00 .00 .00 .00 Std. Dev. .62 .97 .94 1.09 Timeless–Temporary Mean −.72 −.53 −.67 −.54 Median −1.00 −.50 −1.00 −1.00 Std. Dev. 1.25 1.29 1.30 1.54 Open in new tab Table 2: Descriptive statistics for the overall assessment. . . Conditions . Global evaluation . . R (N = 32) . VRPH (N = 32) . VR (N = 31) . S (N = 33) . I like it (1–5) Mean 4.19 4.12 4.10 3.97 Median 4.00 4.00 4.00 4.00 Std. deviation .53 .49 .30 .59 . . Conditions . Global evaluation . . R (N = 32) . VRPH (N = 32) . VR (N = 31) . S (N = 33) . I like it (1–5) Mean 4.19 4.12 4.10 3.97 Median 4.00 4.00 4.00 4.00 Std. deviation .53 .49 .30 .59 Open in new tab Table 2: Descriptive statistics for the overall assessment. . . Conditions . Global evaluation . . R (N = 32) . VRPH (N = 32) . VR (N = 31) . S (N = 33) . I like it (1–5) Mean 4.19 4.12 4.10 3.97 Median 4.00 4.00 4.00 4.00 Std. deviation .53 .49 .30 .59 . . Conditions . Global evaluation . . R (N = 32) . VRPH (N = 32) . VR (N = 31) . S (N = 33) . I like it (1–5) Mean 4.19 4.12 4.10 3.97 Median 4.00 4.00 4.00 4.00 Std. deviation .53 .49 .30 .59 Open in new tab Having taken the descriptive statistics from all the semantic pairs and the overall product assessment, comparing the results obtained in the different means was necessary to determine whether a statistically significant difference existed. For this purpose, a study was conducted about samples’ normality and whether there were normal distributions or not, by means of the Shapiro–Wilk test, with a α = .05. As samples’ normality was lacking, a Kruskal–Wallis test was used to establish whether there were any statistically significant differences in the semantic scale scores among the four experimental conditions (Table 3). The null hypothesis in the Kruskal–Wallis tests determined that the mean ranges of the scores made on the semantic scales for the four experimental conditions were the same. Table 3: Ranks and Kruskal–Wallis tests. . Semantic scales . Condition . N . Mean rank . Kruskal–Wallis test . Physio Uncomfortable–Comfortable R 32 63.67 VRPH 31 60.50 Χ2(3) = 2.101 VR 32 62.16 p = .552 S 33 71.33 Total 128 Heavy–Light R 32 84.03 VRPH 31 64.76 Χ2(3) = 18.266 VR 32 45.27 p < .001 S 33 63.97 Total 128 Large-sized–Small-sized R 32 43.77 VRPH 31 80.50 Χ2(3) = 22.525 VR 32 56.80 p < .001 S 33 77.05 Total 128 Psycho Complex–Simple R 32 59.03 VRPH 31 65.81 Χ2(3) = 3.936 VR 32 74.34 p = .268 S 33 59.03 Total 128 Practical–Useless R 32 68.11 VRPH 31 67.05 Χ2(3) = 1.248 VR 32 59.28 p= .074 S 33 63.67 Total 128 Disproportionate–Well-proportioned R 32 50.27 VRPH 31 69.81 Χ2(3) = 6.936 VR 32 67.17 p= .074 S 33 70.73 Total 128 Socio Modern–Classic R 32 64.86 VRPH 31 65.81 Χ2(3) = .261 VR 32 61.73 p= .967 S 33 65.61 Total 128 Ugly–Nice R 32 81.67 VRPH 31 66.31 Χ2(3) = 14.006 VR 32 59.67 p = .003 S 33 50.83 Total 128 Original–Common R 32 62.84 VRPH 31 61.90 Χ2(3) = 2.380 VR 32 60.22 p = .497 S 33 72.70 Total 128 Ideo Tasteful–Tasteless R 32 62.83 VRPH 31 69.29 Χ2(3) = 2.220 VR 32 68.23 p < .528 S 33 58.00 Total 128 Boring–Fun R 32 69.06 VRPH 31 70.13 Χ2(3) = 4.023 VR 32 64.72 p = .259 S 33 54.58 Total 128 Timeless–Temporary R 32 62.30 VRPH 31 64.03 Χ2(3) = .280 VR 32 67.03 p = .964 S 33 64.62 Total 128 . Semantic scales . Condition . N . Mean rank . Kruskal–Wallis test . Physio Uncomfortable–Comfortable R 32 63.67 VRPH 31 60.50 Χ2(3) = 2.101 VR 32 62.16 p = .552 S 33 71.33 Total 128 Heavy–Light R 32 84.03 VRPH 31 64.76 Χ2(3) = 18.266 VR 32 45.27 p < .001 S 33 63.97 Total 128 Large-sized–Small-sized R 32 43.77 VRPH 31 80.50 Χ2(3) = 22.525 VR 32 56.80 p < .001 S 33 77.05 Total 128 Psycho Complex–Simple R 32 59.03 VRPH 31 65.81 Χ2(3) = 3.936 VR 32 74.34 p = .268 S 33 59.03 Total 128 Practical–Useless R 32 68.11 VRPH 31 67.05 Χ2(3) = 1.248 VR 32 59.28 p= .074 S 33 63.67 Total 128 Disproportionate–Well-proportioned R 32 50.27 VRPH 31 69.81 Χ2(3) = 6.936 VR 32 67.17 p= .074 S 33 70.73 Total 128 Socio Modern–Classic R 32 64.86 VRPH 31 65.81 Χ2(3) = .261 VR 32 61.73 p= .967 S 33 65.61 Total 128 Ugly–Nice R 32 81.67 VRPH 31 66.31 Χ2(3) = 14.006 VR 32 59.67 p = .003 S 33 50.83 Total 128 Original–Common R 32 62.84 VRPH 31 61.90 Χ2(3) = 2.380 VR 32 60.22 p = .497 S 33 72.70 Total 128 Ideo Tasteful–Tasteless R 32 62.83 VRPH 31 69.29 Χ2(3) = 2.220 VR 32 68.23 p < .528 S 33 58.00 Total 128 Boring–Fun R 32 69.06 VRPH 31 70.13 Χ2(3) = 4.023 VR 32 64.72 p = .259 S 33 54.58 Total 128 Timeless–Temporary R 32 62.30 VRPH 31 64.03 Χ2(3) = .280 VR 32 67.03 p = .964 S 33 64.62 Total 128 Note: Greyed rows identify scales where there are statistically significant differences. Open in new tab Table 3: Ranks and Kruskal–Wallis tests. . Semantic scales . Condition . N . Mean rank . Kruskal–Wallis test . Physio Uncomfortable–Comfortable R 32 63.67 VRPH 31 60.50 Χ2(3) = 2.101 VR 32 62.16 p = .552 S 33 71.33 Total 128 Heavy–Light R 32 84.03 VRPH 31 64.76 Χ2(3) = 18.266 VR 32 45.27 p < .001 S 33 63.97 Total 128 Large-sized–Small-sized R 32 43.77 VRPH 31 80.50 Χ2(3) = 22.525 VR 32 56.80 p < .001 S 33 77.05 Total 128 Psycho Complex–Simple R 32 59.03 VRPH 31 65.81 Χ2(3) = 3.936 VR 32 74.34 p = .268 S 33 59.03 Total 128 Practical–Useless R 32 68.11 VRPH 31 67.05 Χ2(3) = 1.248 VR 32 59.28 p= .074 S 33 63.67 Total 128 Disproportionate–Well-proportioned R 32 50.27 VRPH 31 69.81 Χ2(3) = 6.936 VR 32 67.17 p= .074 S 33 70.73 Total 128 Socio Modern–Classic R 32 64.86 VRPH 31 65.81 Χ2(3) = .261 VR 32 61.73 p= .967 S 33 65.61 Total 128 Ugly–Nice R 32 81.67 VRPH 31 66.31 Χ2(3) = 14.006 VR 32 59.67 p = .003 S 33 50.83 Total 128 Original–Common R 32 62.84 VRPH 31 61.90 Χ2(3) = 2.380 VR 32 60.22 p = .497 S 33 72.70 Total 128 Ideo Tasteful–Tasteless R 32 62.83 VRPH 31 69.29 Χ2(3) = 2.220 VR 32 68.23 p < .528 S 33 58.00 Total 128 Boring–Fun R 32 69.06 VRPH 31 70.13 Χ2(3) = 4.023 VR 32 64.72 p = .259 S 33 54.58 Total 128 Timeless–Temporary R 32 62.30 VRPH 31 64.03 Χ2(3) = .280 VR 32 67.03 p = .964 S 33 64.62 Total 128 . Semantic scales . Condition . N . Mean rank . Kruskal–Wallis test . Physio Uncomfortable–Comfortable R 32 63.67 VRPH 31 60.50 Χ2(3) = 2.101 VR 32 62.16 p = .552 S 33 71.33 Total 128 Heavy–Light R 32 84.03 VRPH 31 64.76 Χ2(3) = 18.266 VR 32 45.27 p < .001 S 33 63.97 Total 128 Large-sized–Small-sized R 32 43.77 VRPH 31 80.50 Χ2(3) = 22.525 VR 32 56.80 p < .001 S 33 77.05 Total 128 Psycho Complex–Simple R 32 59.03 VRPH 31 65.81 Χ2(3) = 3.936 VR 32 74.34 p = .268 S 33 59.03 Total 128 Practical–Useless R 32 68.11 VRPH 31 67.05 Χ2(3) = 1.248 VR 32 59.28 p= .074 S 33 63.67 Total 128 Disproportionate–Well-proportioned R 32 50.27 VRPH 31 69.81 Χ2(3) = 6.936 VR 32 67.17 p= .074 S 33 70.73 Total 128 Socio Modern–Classic R 32 64.86 VRPH 31 65.81 Χ2(3) = .261 VR 32 61.73 p= .967 S 33 65.61 Total 128 Ugly–Nice R 32 81.67 VRPH 31 66.31 Χ2(3) = 14.006 VR 32 59.67 p = .003 S 33 50.83 Total 128 Original–Common R 32 62.84 VRPH 31 61.90 Χ2(3) = 2.380 VR 32 60.22 p = .497 S 33 72.70 Total 128 Ideo Tasteful–Tasteless R 32 62.83 VRPH 31 69.29 Χ2(3) = 2.220 VR 32 68.23 p < .528 S 33 58.00 Total 128 Boring–Fun R 32 69.06 VRPH 31 70.13 Χ2(3) = 4.023 VR 32 64.72 p = .259 S 33 54.58 Total 128 Timeless–Temporary R 32 62.30 VRPH 31 64.03 Χ2(3) = .280 VR 32 67.03 p = .964 S 33 64.62 Total 128 Note: Greyed rows identify scales where there are statistically significant differences. Open in new tab It was first necessary to check four assumptions: The dependent variable must be measured ordinally or continuously. In our case, the semantic scale scores were measured from −3 to 3. The independent variable must consist in two independent categorical groups or more. We had four independent groups (R, VRPH, VR, S). There is no relation among either the observations made in each group or the groups themselves. The distributions among each group must have a similar form and variability. The Kruskal–Wallis test results are found in Table 3 and revealed that the null hypothesis was confirmed (.05 level of significance) on all the semantic scales, except for scales 'Light–Heavy', 'Large-sized–Small-sized', and 'Ugly–Nice', which are grey shaded. In order to continue the Kruskal–Wallis test, a pair-wise comparison was made in SPSS, v. 22, to determine which pairs of interaction conditions (R, VRPH, VR, S) significantly differed from one another using the Dunn–Bonferroni tests. The adjusted levels of significance are shown in Table 4, and were calculated by multiplying the unadjusted significance values by the number of comparisons by giving a value of 1 if the product was higher than 1. Table 4: Adjusted significance levels for pair-wise comparisons by Dunn–Bonferroni tests. Comparison . Heavy–Light . Large-sized–Small-sized . Ugly–Nice . R–VRPH <.001 .869 .061 R–S .155 .001 .002 R–VR .211 <.001 .451 VRPH–S .227 .135 1.000 VRPH–VR .198 .051 .061 S–VR 1.000 1.000 .426 Comparison . Heavy–Light . Large-sized–Small-sized . Ugly–Nice . R–VRPH <.001 .869 .061 R–S .155 .001 .002 R–VR .211 <.001 .451 VRPH–S .227 .135 1.000 VRPH–VR .198 .051 .061 S–VR 1.000 1.000 .426 Note: Greyed columns identify comparisons of conditions where there are statistically significant differences (p < .05). Open in new tab Table 4: Adjusted significance levels for pair-wise comparisons by Dunn–Bonferroni tests. Comparison . Heavy–Light . Large-sized–Small-sized . Ugly–Nice . R–VRPH <.001 .869 .061 R–S .155 .001 .002 R–VR .211 <.001 .451 VRPH–S .227 .135 1.000 VRPH–VR .198 .051 .061 S–VR 1.000 1.000 .426 Comparison . Heavy–Light . Large-sized–Small-sized . Ugly–Nice . R–VRPH <.001 .869 .061 R–S .155 .001 .002 R–VR .211 <.001 .451 VRPH–S .227 .135 1.000 VRPH–VR .198 .051 .061 S–VR 1.000 1.000 .426 Note: Greyed columns identify comparisons of conditions where there are statistically significant differences (p < .05). Open in new tab The overall assessment made by the 'I like it' question showed no statistically significant difference among conditions, and a Kruskal–Wallis test was applied (Χ2(3) = 2.785, p = .426) because of the sample's lack of data normality. 5. Discussion The Kruskal–Wallis test demonstrated that there were statistically significant differences among the assessments made for three product characteristics: differences in weight (light–heavy), size (large-sized–small-sized), and aesthetics (ugly–nice). So, we state that the means employed to present the product influenced the assessment made of it, at least for some of its characteristics. Therefore, H1 was met. This result coincides with other research works. For instance, Artacho-Ramírez, Diego-Mas, and Alcaide-Marzal (2008) compared the assessment made of a real product in four different presentation formats (photography, static infographic image, 3D navigable model, and 3D navigable stereographic model). They also concluded that the presentation means influenced the assessment made of some product characteristics. Our study also demonstrated that significant differences appeared between some presentation means pairs: R–VRPH for heavy–light; R–S and R–VR for large-sized–small-sized; and R–S for ugly–nice. So, the results for the R–VRPH pair indicated that the product was scored more closely to the 'Heavy' adjective when presented in VRPH (−1.09) but, conversely, it was scored more closely to ‘Light’ when presented in R (.56). This result was unexpected because the product data collected from both means were similar (physical contact and being able to view the product on a real scale). Therefore, we would have thought that the score given to this semantic pair should have been similar. Nonetheless, other studies have warned that colours may affect how an object's weight is perceived (Löffler et al., 2016, 2018). This might explain the results obtained in this case, although the colour used to represent the product in VRPH was similar to the real armchair's colour. Perhaps minor tone details could have altered assessments. Likewise with the R–S pair, the results showed that the product obtained a closer score to the ‘Large-sized’ adjective when presented in R, and was scored more closely to the ‘Small-sized’ adjective when viewed in S. This result was expected because the screen employed in S was small and, therefore, objects would have appeared smaller than they actually were, although the armchair scale compared with the other elements defining the scene was exactly the same in all the settings. A similar result was obtained for the R–VR pair because the product looked bigger in R than in VR. So, when we examined the scores given to the large-sized–small-sized semantic pair in all four means (R −1.87; VRPH −1.47; VR −.77; S −.67), we found that the least immersive the presentation setting was, the smaller this product was perceived (closer to 0 on the 7-interval scale). These results can be explained according to former studies, which defend that the immersion sensation in a means, or presence, can affect how the size of a represented space is perceived and, therefore, how objects appear in it (Heineken & Schulte, 2007). Similarly, the results for the R–S pair indicated that the product was scored more closely to the adjective ‘Nice’ when displayed in R (2.21), but more closely to ‘Ugly’ when presented in S (1.46). Former studies have indicated that the novelty of the 3D presentation format plays a key role in forming purchase intention (Edwards & Gangadharbatla, 2001), perhaps because it is more appealing. So, although the present research did not ask the participants about their purchase intentions, we expected the product to be assessed as 'nicer' if presented in digital formats, which are more novel than conventional presentation formats. This result indicates that the armchair was perceived as being more appealing when presented in a real setting. Nevertheless, the second best assessment was made when the product was displayed in VR (1.84), which made us think that the novel presentation format could influence assessments after all. The present work demonstrates that the means employed to present a household product affects users’ assessments about some of its characteristics, but the Kruskal–Wallis test indicated that the overall assessment was not affected. This means that the product does not necessarily have to be generally liked more when presented in one format or another. Although these results were not statistically significant, the assessments made by the 'I like it' question reveal that the more real the product presentation is, the more it is generally liked, albeit with a slight difference (R: 4.19; VRPH: 4.12; VR: 4.10; S: 3.97). These results also allowed us to see that the possibility of touching the product (in R and VRPH) could be related to it obtaining a slightly better overall assessment because some studies have shown that haptic qualities provide more product information (Krishna & Schwarz, 2014; Schwarz, 2012) and can improve its acceptance, which would favour purchase intentions (Jiang & Benbasat, 2004; Suh & Lee, 2005). Our results also corroborate that having the chance or not to come into physical contact with a presented household product influences its assessment, and statistically significant differences were found for the scores given for the semantic pairs large-sized–small-sized (for R–S and R–VR) and ugly–nice (for R and S). Thus, H2 was met. Finally, H3 contemplated that similar assessments would be made of a product presented without physical contact, regardless of the means employed. As no statistically significant differences were found in the assessments made between the two means that did not allow the product to be touched (VR and S), H3 was met. Moreover, presenting a product in either VR or S did not affect the assessments made of its characteristics. Although no statistically significant differences appeared between VR and S for the seven semantic pairs with positive and negative connotations, namely comfortable–uncomfortable, useless–practical, disproportionate–well-proportionate, ugly–nice, original–common, tasteful–tasteless, and boring–fun, we observed that more positive connotations of some of these pairs were assessed in VR, and more negative connotations were assessed in S. The armchair obtained the highest score for 'comfortable' when presented by VR, and its lowest score was when presented by S. Likewise, the product was perceived as being more practical when presented by VR, and it was nicer and more original than in S. Finally, the products’ overall score was higher for ‘fun’ when presented in VR, and was higher for ‘boring’ when displayed in S. An explanation as to why a product is assessed more positively when presented in VR than in S is beyond the present research objective, but recent studies have demonstrated that attitudes to a product can be affected by a means’ level of interaction and the amount of feedback it offers. Park, Choi, Kim, and Kwon (2019) measured attitudes to a product presented in three non-tangible formats (still pictures, motion pictures, virtual graphs) and collected data using questionnaires. They concluded that an attitude to a product presented in virtual graphs (real image-based 3D contents with interactive functions) proved more positive than towards the same product displayed by the other two formats, which were less interactive. In our research, we employed different means, but the product was observed more intuitively and interactively in VR than in the less intuitive S, where it was necessary to use a mouse and keyboard to move closer and away, or to change the viewing point. This difference in the experiment's interactivity and feedback in both means could, therefore, explain the more positive attitude to the product displayed through VR. 6. Conclusions This work studied the influence that a presentation means and coming into physical contact have when assessing 12 characteristics of a household product. This was performed by a sample of 128 people divided into four subgroups. Each subgroup assessed the same armchair presented in a different means: two means offered the possibility of touching the product (R and VRPH), while the other two presented the product with no physical interaction involved (VR and S). This study first demonstrated that the semantic pairs selected and grouped in Tiger categories allowed us to accurately study the influence that a presentation means can have on the assessment of a presented product. So, it proved to be an efficient method to describe this influence on different characteristics. Our results also revealed that when globally assessing a product, the presentation means had no significant influence, but was significant when assessing some product characteristics. Product characteristics (weight, size, and aesthetics) were influenced by the presentation means, and would be perceived differently depending on the means. Our data also showed that including physical contact or not with the product while it was displayed would influence its assessment. Therefore, the means is important because it can make a product look bigger, smaller, nicer, or uglier. A product is perceived as being bigger in those means that allow physical contact with the product, and looks smaller in those means that do not; it was perceived as lighter in R, heavier by VRPH, nicer in R, and uglier by S. Conversely, presenting a product by VR or S was not significant because both these means obtained similar assessments to one another (barely statistically significant). So, they could be employed indiscriminately to either present products telematically or assess virtual prototypes. These conclusions must be taken into account when selecting one means or another to assess different design alternatives in the project phase because the scores given to some important characteristics (physical and aesthetic) can be affected, which can impact subsequent design decisions. Likewise, presenting a product to be commercialized in one means or another can affect how some of its characteristics are assessed, which impacts purchase decisions. Perceiving distorted information about a product while it is being presented via virtual means can also affect users’ purchase satisfaction when they are delivered the real product as some product features would differ from those they expected. Therefore, according to this study, if the real product was delivered, it would be perceived as being lighter and bigger than expected than if it was presented by any of the virtual means (VRPH, VR, S). Moreover, our results reveal that the product obtained the best product assessment for the adjective 'nice' when it is presented in a real setting; that is, when users see and touch the real product. So, if the product was presented via another means, user satisfaction with this aspect would be better than expected when they received the product. The present study focused on assessing the perception of a furnishing product whose habitual use implies a marked tactile–physical interaction. This experiment obtained significant results only for the semantic pairs in the Physio and Socio categories. Nonetheless, assessing a single product (armchair) can imply a limitation for this study, so it would be desirable for future research to assess other similar products in all four means, such as chairs, chaise longue, or couches, to better confirm the results for this product family. In parallel, it would be interesting for future studies to verify what results this methodology would offer to assess other household products related more to the other Psycho or Ideo categories. Considering the sample size limitations, this work can be considered a pilot study for following up research. So, it would be interesting to check whether these results would be the same with an older sample for which the technology bias might be more pronounced and could have an unexpected effect on the assessments made in some means. Finally, it might be worth recruiting subjects who have no industrial design training, which was the degree that the subjects of the present work were studying. ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS The authors wish to thank designers Francisco Cueto, Paula Bañuelos, Carlos Marín, and Pau Fenollosa for their collaboration. Funding This work was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation (grant number PID2019-106426RB-C32); the Spanish Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness(MINECO) (grant number TIN2016-75866-C3-1-R); and the Universitat Jaume I (grant number UJI-B2019-39). Conflict of Interest Statement None declared. References Achiche S. , Maier A., Milanonva K., Vadean A. ( 2014 ). Visual Product Evaluation: Using the Semantic Differential to Investigate the Influence of Basic Geometry on User Perception . In Proceedings of the ASME 2014 International Mechanical Engineering Congress and Exposition. Volume 11: Systems, Design, and Complexity . (pp. 1 – 10 .). Montreal : ASME , https://doi.org/10.1115/IMECE2014-40443 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Al-Hindawe J. ( 1996 ). Considerations when constructing a semantic differential scale . La Trobe Papers in Linguistics , 9 ( 7 ), 1 – 9 . Google Scholar OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Algharabat R. , Alalwan A. A., Rana N. P., Dwivedi Y. K. ( 2017 ). Three-dimensional product presentation quality antecedents and their consequences for online retailers: The moderating role of virtual product experience . Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services , 36 , 203 – 217 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Artacho-Ramírez M. A. , Diego-Mas J. A., Alcaide-Marzal J. ( 2008 ). Influence of the mode of graphical representation on the perception of product aesthetic and emotional features: An exploratory study . International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics , 38 ( 5 ), 942 – 952 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Azmandian M. , Hancock M., Benko H., Ofek E., Wilson A. D. ( 2016 ). Haptic retargeting: Dynamic repurposing of passive haptics for enhanced virtual reality experiences . In Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems . (pp. 1968 – 1979 .). San Jose, CA : Association for Computing Machinery . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Barsalou L. W. ( 2008 ). Grounded cognition . Annual Review of Psychology , 59 ( 1 ), 617 – 645 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Bergé L-P. , Dubois E., Raynal M. ( 2015 ). Design and evaluation of an “Around the SmartPhone” technique for 3D manipulations on distant display . In Proceedings of the 3rd ACM Symposium on Spatial User Interaction . (pp. 69 – 78 .). New York, NY : Association for Computing Machinery . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Biocca F. , Inoue Y., Polinsky H., Lee A., Tang A. ( 2002 ). Visual cues and virtual touch: Role of visual stimuli and intersensory integration in cross-modal haptic illusions and the sense of presence . In Gouveia F. (Ed.), Proceedings of Presence 2002 . Porto : Fernando Pessoa University . Retrieved from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi = 10.1.1.118.6253&rep = rep1&type = pdf . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Bordegoni M. , Ferrise F. ( 2013 ). Designing interaction with consumer products in a multisensory virtual reality environment . Virtual and Physical Prototyping , 8 ( 1 ), 51 – 64 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Carlin A. S. , Hoffman H. G., Weghorst S. 1997 . Virtual reality and tactile augmentation in the treatment of spider phobia: A case report . Behaviour Research and Therapy , 35 ( 2 ), 153 – 158 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Carvalheiro C. , Nóbrega R., da Silva H., Rodrigues R. ( 2016 ). User redirection and direct haptics in virtual environments . In Proceedings of the 24th ACM International Conference on Multimedia . (pp. 1146 – 1155 .). New York, NY : Association for Computing Machinery . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Cecil J. , Kanchanapiboon A. ( 2007 ). Virtual engineering approaches in product and process design . The International Journal of Advanced Manufacturing Technology , 31 ( 9 ), 846 – 856 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Chen Y. , ( 2018 ). Neurological effect of the aesthetics of product design on the decision-making process of consumers . NeuroQuantology , 16 ( 6 ), 501 – 506 .. https://doi.org/10.14704/nq.2018.16.6.1675. Google Scholar OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Collins K. , Kapralos B. ( 2019 ) Pseudo-haptics: Leveraging cross-modal perception in virtual environments . The Senses and Society , 14 ( 3 ), 313 – 329 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Crozier W. R. ( 1994 ). Manufactured pleasures: Psychological response to design . New York : Manchester University Press . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Delarue J. , Lageat T. ( 2019 ). Conducting contextualized and real-life product tests: Benefits and experimental challenges . In Context . (pp. 457 – 473 .). Duxford : Woodhead Publishing . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Ebrahimi E. ( 2017 ). Investigating embodied interaction in near-field perception-action re-calibration on performance in immersive virtual environments . Ph.D. Thesis . Clemson University, South Carolina . Retrieved from https://tigerprints.clemson.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=3075&context=all_dissertations . Google Scholar Ebrahimi E. , Babu S. V., Pagano C. C., Joerg S. ( 2016 ). Towards a comparative evaluation of visually guided physical reach motions during 3D interactions in real and virtual environments . In Thomas B. H., Lindeman R., Marchal M. (Eds.), 2016 IEEE symposium on 3D user interfaces (3DUI) . (pp. 237 – 238 .). Greenville, SC : IEEE Computer Society . https://doi.org/10.1109/3DUI.2016.7460058 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Edwards S. E. , Gangadharbatla H. ( 2001 ). The novelty of 3D product presentations online . Journal of Interactive Advertising , 2 ( 1 ), 10 – 18 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Elder R. S. , Krishna A. ( 2012 ). The “Visual Depiction Effect” in advertising: Facilitating embodied mental simulation through product orientation . Journal of Consumer Research , 38 ( 6 ), 988 – 1003 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Falcao C. S. , Soares M. M. ( 2014 ). Applications of haptic devices & virtual reality in consumer products usability evaluation . In Soares M., Rebelo F. (Eds.), Advances in ergonomics in design, usability & special populations: Part I . (pp. 377 – 383 .). Louisville : AHFE Conference . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Felip F. , Galán J., García-García C., Mulet E. ( 2019 ). Influence of presentation means on industrial product evaluations with potential users: A first study by comparing tangible virtual reality and presenting a product in a real setting . Virtual Reality , 24 , 439 – 451 ., https://doi.org/10.1007/s10055-019-00406-9. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Flavián C. , Gurrea R., Orús C. ( 2009 ). The impact of online product presentation on consumers' perceptions: An experimental analysis . International Journal of E-Services and Mobile Applications , 1 ( 3 ), 17 – 37 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Grewal D. , Noble S. M., Roggeveen A. L., Nordfalt J. ( 2020 ). The future of in-store technology . Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science , 48 ( 1 ), 96 – 113 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Heineken E. , Schulte F. P. ( 2007 ). Seeing size and feeling weight: The size–weight illusion in natural and virtual reality . Human Factors , 49 ( 1 ), 136 – 144 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Hinchet R. , Vechev V., Shea H., Hilliges O. ( 2018 ). DextrES: Wearable haptic feedback for grasping in VR via a thin form-factor electrostatic brake . In Proceedings of the 31st Annual ACM Symposium on User Interface Software and Technology (UIST '18) (pp. 901 – 912 .). New York, NY : Association for Computing Machinery . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Hoffman H. G , Groen J., Rousseau S., Hollander A., Winn W., Wells M., Furness T. ( 1996 ). Tactile augmentation: Enhancing presence in inclusive VR with tactile feedback from real objects . Paper presented at Meeting of the American Psychological Sciences , San Francisco . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Hoffman H. G. ( 1998 ). Physically touching virtual objects using tactile augmentation enhances the realism of virtual environments . In Proceedings of the IEEE 1998 Virtual Reality Annual International Symposium (pp. 59 – 63 .). https://doi.org/10.1109/VRAIS.1998.658423 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Hornsey R. L. , Hibbard P. B., Scarfe P. ( 2020 ). Size and shape constancy in consumer virtual reality . Behavior Research Methods , 52 , 1587 – 1598 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Hsiao S. W. , Chiu F. Y., Chen C. S. ( 2008 ). Applying aesthetics measurement to product design . International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics , 38 ( 11–12 ), 910 – 920 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Insko B. E. ( 2001 ). Passive haptics significantly enhances virtual environments . Ph.D. Thesis . University of North Carolina . Retrieved from http://www.cs.unc.edu/techreports/01-017.pdf . Google Scholar Jalil N. A. A. , Fikry A., Zainuddin A. ( 2016 ). The impact of store atmospherics, perceived value, and customer satisfaction on behavioural intention . Procedia Economics and Finance , 37 , 538 – 544 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Jiang Z. , Benbasat I. ( 2004 ). Virtual product experience: Effects of visual and functional control of products on perceived diagnosticity and flow in electronic shopping . Journal of Management Information Systems , 21 ( 3 ), 111 – 147 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Jiang Z. , Benbasat I. ( 2007 ). Investigating the influence of the functional mechanisms of online product presentations . Information Systems Research , 18 ( 4 ), 454 – 470 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Joyce R. D. , Robinson S. K. ( 2017 ). Passive haptics to enhance virtual reality simulations . Paper presented at AIAA Modeling and Simulation Technologies Conference , Grapevine, Texas . Retrieved from https://arc.aiaa.org/doi/abs/10.2514/6.2017-1313 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Keng C. J. , Liao T. H., Yang Y. I. ( 2012 ). The effects of sequential combinations of virtual experience, direct experience, and indirect experience: The moderating roles of need for touch and product involvement . Electronic Commerce Research , 12 ( 2 ), 177 – 199 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Krishna A. ( 2012 ). An integrative review of sensory marketing: Engaging the senses to affect perception, judgment and behavior . Journal of Consumer Psychology , 22 ( 3 ), 332 – 351 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Krishna A. , Schwarz N. ( 2014 ). Sensory marketing, embodiment, and grounded cognition . Journal of Consumer Psychology , 24 ( 2 ), 159 – 168 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Lassagne A. , Kemeny A., Posselt J., Merienne F. ( 2018 ). Performance evaluation of passive haptic feedback for tactile HMI design in CAVEs . IEEE Transactions on Haptics , 11 ( 1 ), 119 – 127 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Lindeman R. W. , Sibert J. L., Hahn J. K. ( 1999 ). Hand-held windows: Towards effective 2D interaction in immersive virtual environments . In Proceedings of the IEEE Virtual Reality . (pp. 205 – 212 .). Houston, TX : IEEE . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Lok A. , Naik S., Whitton M., Brooks F. P. ( 2003 ). Effects of handling real objects and self-avatar fidelity on cognitive task performance and sense of presence in virtual environments . Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments , 12 ( 6 ), 615 – 628 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Luo A. , Shen Y., Liu Y. ( 2019 ). Look and feel: The importance of sensory feedback in virtual product experience . Paper presented at Fortieth International Conference on Information Systems , Munich, Germany . Retrieved from https://aisel.aisnet.org/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article = 1064&context = icis2019 . Google Scholar Löffler D. , Arlt L., Toriizuka T., Tscharn R., Hurtienne J. ( 2016 ). Substituting color for haptic attributes in conceptual metaphors for tangible interaction design . In Proceedings of the TEI ’16: Tenth International Conference on Tangible, Embedded, and Embodied Interaction (TEI ’16) (pp. 118 – 125 .). New York, NY : ACM . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Löffler D. , Tscharn R., Hurtienne J. ( 2018 ). Multimodal effects of color and haptics on intuitive interaction with tangible user interfaces . In Proceedings of the Twelfth International Conference on Tangible, Embedded, and Embodied Interaction (TEI ’18) . (pp. 647 – 655 .). New York, NY : ACM . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Minamizawa K. , Fukamachi S., Kajimoto H., Kawakami N., Tachi S. ( 2007 ). Gravity grabber: Wearable haptic display to present virtual mass sensation . In Proceedings of the ACM SIGGRAPH 2007 Emerging Technologies (SIGGRAPH '07) . (pp. 8 – es .). New York, NY : Association for Computing Machinery . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Minamizawa K. , Kamuro S., Fukamachi S., Kawakami N., Tachi S. ( 2008 ). GhostGlove: Haptic existence of the virtual world . In Proceedings of the ACM SIGGRAPH 2008 posters (SIGGRAPH '08) ( Article 134 ). New York, NY : Association for Computing Machinery . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Mondragón S. , Company P., Vergara M. ( 2005 ). Semantic differential applied to the evaluation of machine tool design . International Journal of Industrial Ergonomics , 35 ( 11 ), 1021 – 1029 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Musashi N. ( 2019 ). Pseudo-haptic feedback by cross-modal perception of auditory stimuli . Impact , 10 , 36 – 38 . Google Scholar OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Naderi E. , Naderi I., Balakrishnan B. ( 2020 ). Product design matters, but is it enough? Consumers’ responses to product design and environment congruence . Journal of Product & Brand Management , 29 , 939 – 954 .. https://doi.org/10.1108/JPBM-08-2018-1975 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Ozok A. , Komlodi A. ( 2009 ). Better in 3D? An empirical investigation of user satisfaction and preferences concerning two-dimensional and three-dimensional product representations in business-to-consumer e-commerce . International Journal of Human–Computer Interaction , 25 ( 4 ), 243 – 281 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Pacchierotti C. , Sinclair S., Solazzi M., Frisoli A., Hayward V., Prattichizzo D. ( 2017 ). Wearable haptic systems for the fingertip and the hand: Taxonomy, review, and perspectives . IEEE Transactions on Haptics , 10 ( 4 ), 580 – 600 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Park J. , Choi J., Kim H., Kwon H. ( 2019 ). The influence of media type and length of time delay on user attitude: Effects of product-focused virtual reality . Computers in Human Behavior , 101 , 466 – 473 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Park K. , Park J., Kim S., Choe J., Jung E. S. ( 2014 ). Affective evaluation of interior design of commercial cars using 3D images . Journal of The Ergonomics Society of Korea , 33 , 515 – 532 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Peck J. , Childers T. L. ( 2003 ). To have and to hold: The influence of haptic information on product judgments . Journal of Marketing , 67 ( 2 ), 35 – 48 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Peck J. , Wiggins J. ( 2006 ). It just feels good: Customers’ affective response to touch and its influence on persuasión . Journal of Marketing , 70 ( 4 ), 56 – 69 . Google Scholar OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Perez Mata M. , Ahmed-Kristensen S., Brockhoff P.B ., Yanagisawa H. ( 2017 ). Investigating the influence of product perception and geometric features . Research in Engineering Design , 28 , 357 – 379 ..https://doi.org/10.1007/s0016 3-016-0244-1. Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Prattichizzo D. , Chinello F., Pacchierotti C., Malvezzi M. ( 2013 ). Towards wearability in fingertip haptics: A 3-DoF wearable device for cutaneous force feedback . IEEE Transactions on Haptics , 6 ( 4 ), 506 – 516 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Saraswati T. G. ( 2018 ). Driving factors of consumer to purchase furniture online on IKEA Indonesia website . Jurnal Sekretaris & Administrasi Bisnis , 2 ( 1 ), 19 – 28 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Schifferstein H. N. , Cleiren M. P. ( 2005 ). Capturing product experiences: A split-modality approach . Acta Psychologica , 118 ( 3 ), 293 – 318 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Schlosser A. E. ( 2003 ). Experiencing products in the virtual world: The role of goal and imagery in influencing attitudes versus purchase intentions , Journal of Consumer Research , 30 ( 2 ), 184 – 198 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Schwarz N. ( 2012 ). Feelings-as-information theory . In Handbook of theories of social psychology . (pp. 289 – 308 .). Thousand Oaks, CA : SAGE . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Steinmann S. , Kilian T., Brylla D. ( 2014 ). Experiencing products virtually: The role of vividness and interactivity in influencing mental imagery and user reactions . Paper presented at 35th International Conference on Information Systems , Auckland, New Zealand . Retrieved from https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/1b38/43fbe5d8551e81527421df8579afda164848.pdf . Google Scholar Suh K. S. , Lee Y. E. ( 2005 ). The effects of virtual reality on consumer learning: An empirical investigation . Management Information Systems Quarterly , 29 , 673 – 697 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Tardif N. , Therrien C.É, Bouchard S. ( 2019 ). Re-examining psychological mechanisms underlying virtual reality-based exposure for spider phobia . Cyberpsychology, Behavior and Social Networking , 22 ( 1 ), 39 – 45 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Tiger L. ( 1992 ). The pursuit of pleasure . Boston : Little, Brown & Company . Google Scholar Google Preview OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat COPAC Unal S. ( 2017 ). The mediating role of product familiarity in consumer animosity . Journal of Accounting and Marketing , 6 ( 4 ), 1 – 11 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Velázquez R. , Pissaloux E., Del-Valle-Soto C., Arai M., Valdivia L. J., Del Puerto-Flores J. A., Gutiérrez C. A. ( 2019 ). Performance evaluation of active and passive haptic feedback in shape perception . In Proceedings of the IEEE 39th Central America and Panama Convention CONCAPAN XXXIX (pp. 1 – 6 .). Guatemala City : IEEE . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Verhagen T. , Vonkeman C., Feldberg F., Verhagen P. ( 2014 ). Present it like it is here: Creating local presence to improve online product experiences . Computers in Human Behavior , 39 , 270 – 280 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Violante M. G. , Marcolin F., Vezzetti E., Nonis F., Moos S. ( 2019 ). Emotional design and virtual reality in product lifecycle management (PLM) . In Sustainable design and manufacturing 2019. KES-SDM 2019. Smart innovation, systems and technologies . (pp. 177 – 187 .). Singapore : Springer . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS Google Preview WorldCat COPAC Willemsen P. , Gooch A. A., Thompson W. B., Creem-Regehr S. H. ( 2008 ). Effects of stereo viewing conditions on distance perception in virtual environments . Presence: Teleoperators & Virtual Environments , 17 ( 1 ), 91 – 101 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Wu K. , Vassileva J., Zhao Y., Noorian Z., Waldner W., Adaji I. ( 2016 ). Complexity or simplicity? Designing product pictures for advertising in online marketplaces . Journal of Retailing and Consumer Services , 28 , 17 – 27 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Ye J. , Badiyani S., Raja V., Schlege T. ( 2007 ). Applications of virtual reality in product design evaluation . Human–Computer Interaction. HCI Applications and Services , 4553 , 1190 – 1199 . Google Scholar OpenURL Placeholder Text WorldCat Yoo J. , Kim M. ( 2014 ). The effects of online product presentation on consumer responses: A mental imagery perspective . Journal of Business Research , 67 ( 11 ), 2464 – 2472 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat Zenner A. , Kosmalla F., Ehrlich J., Hell P., Kahl G., Murlowski C., Marco Speicher M., Daiber F., Heinrich D., Krüger A. ( 2020 ). A virtual reality couch configurator leveraging passive haptic feedback . Paper presented at 2020 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems , Honolulu, USA . Retrieved from https://dl.acm.org/doi/pdf/10.1145/3334480.3382953 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS © The Author(s) 2020. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the Society for Computational Design and Engineering. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. TI - The influence of haptics when assessing household products presented in different means: a comparative study in real setting, flat display, and virtual reality environments with and without passive haptics JF - Journal of Computational Design and Engineering DO - 10.1093/jcde/qwaa081 DA - 2021-01-25 UR - https://www.deepdyve.com/lp/oxford-university-press/the-influence-of-haptics-when-assessing-household-products-presented-cTxGEOc0Ur SP - 330 EP - 342 VL - 8 IS - 1 DP - DeepDyve ER -