TY - JOUR AU1 - Pan, Xiaoyang AU2 - Soh, Kim Geok AU3 - Jaafar, Wan Marzuki Wan AU4 - Soh, Kim Lam AU5 - Deng, Nuannuan AU6 - Cao, Shudian AU7 - Li, Mingtian AU8 - Liu, Huange AB - Introduction Mental fatigue (MF), or cognitive fatigue, arises from prolonged mental effort during demanding tasks [1]. It manifests as feeling mentally drained, lacking energy, exhaustion, and reduced motivation to focus [2–5]. MF is identified through neurophysiological, behavioral, or subjective indicators [6]. Athletes report symptoms impacting their physical, technical, tactical, psychological, and psychomotor performance [7,8]. Research indicates athletes see MF as detrimental to training and competition [9]. Athletes experience MF across all sports, particularly pre-season and in-season [10]. Recognizing MF’s impact on sports is crucial, and practitioners should manage symptoms properly [11]. Golf is a sport enjoyed globally by recreational and professional players [12]. Recently, there has been increasing scientific interest in how golfers manage MF [13]. Competitive golf requires exceptional hand-eye coordination, significant cognitive demands, advanced motor skills, biomechanical precision, and extended periods of play [14–18]. Research indicates that critical shot-making decisions, extensive walking, numerous high-intensity swings, and putting can induce MF, potentially impairing performance [19]. From the cognitive perspective, golf requires continuous effort, strategic thinking, planning, and adaptation to course conditions [20]. Prolonged engagement in these tasks may lead to cognitive fatigue, affecting decision-making [21]. The extended duration of golf rounds [22], involving continuous shot decisions and swings, exposes players to sustained mental strain, potentially leading to MF [23]. Complex decision-making in golf, such as choosing clubs, shot types, and putting techniques, can make golfers vulnerable to MF, impairing optimal decision-making and resulting in suboptimal performance [24]. The biomechanical complexity of swings and putts is susceptible to MF, influencing coordination and technical performance [25]. In summary, MF is important in training and competition [13]. The need for this systematic review lies in the fact that there is a lack of systematic reviews of the interactions between MF and golf performance. Although studies have been conducted to point out the potential effects of MF on golf sport performance, a systematic review can comprehensively summarize and analyze the existing findings to better understand the current state of research and findings in this area. By identifying gaps in existing research and research gaps, systematic reviews can provide guidance for future research and identify questions and directions that need to be further explored. Methods This article adhered to the PRISMA (2020) guidelines [26]. This title has already been registered on the International Platform of Registered Systematic Review and Meta-analysis Protocols, and the registration number is INPLASY202410111. Electronic databases such as PubMed, Web of Science, SPORTDiscus, Scopes, and China National Knowledge Infrastructure (CNKI) were systematically searched until January 2, 2024. Boolean operators AND and OR were applied with the specified keywords, as shown in Table 1 below. Harmonized search terms and search string are used in all databases (S1 Table). Download: PPT PowerPoint slide PNG larger image TIFF original image Table 1. Search string. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310403.t001 Population, intervention, comparison, outcome, and study design (PICOS) criteria were included (Table 2). Studies had to meet the following conditions: (1) Participants had to be healthy individuals engaged in golf; (2) Any type of intervention was considered; (3) Comparison group includes any different intervention or no intervention; (4) Outcomes included levels of MF or any form of golf performance, encompassing physical, technical, cognitive, and tactical aspects; and (5) The study designs needed to include randomized controlled trials (RCT), non-randomized controlled trials (nRCT), and non-randomized non-controlled trials (nCT). Download: PPT PowerPoint slide PNG larger image TIFF original image Table 2. PICOS criteria for inclusion criteria. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310403.t002 The results (titles and abstracts) of studies obtained using the search technique and those obtained from other sources to identify articles that may satisfy the inclusion criteria mentioned above were independently screened. The identification, screening, and inclusion phases of selecting references for analysis are summed up in Fig 1. Automation tools are not used in the process. Additionally, this review strictly adhered to the PRISMA Checklist requirements to ensure transparency and systematic procedures during the screening process (S1 File). Download: PPT PowerPoint slide PNG larger image TIFF original image Fig 1. PRISMA flow diagram. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310403.g001 The methodology quality was assessed using "QualSyst" [27], which includes 14 items (Table 3). Each item was scored based on the degree to which specific criteria were met: yes = 2, partial = 1, no = 0. Items that did not apply to the study design were marked as "NA" and excluded from the total score calculation. A score of ≥75% indicated strong quality, 55–75% indicated moderate quality, and a ≤55% indicated weak quality. The detailed ratings are shown in Table 3. Download: PPT PowerPoint slide PNG larger image TIFF original image Table 3. Assessing the quality of studies. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310403.t003 Results Fig 1 Flowchart depicting the study selection process from search to inclusion in the review. Studies that met the inclusion criteria but were excluded are labeled in the flow. 725 articles were screened in the review and 10 articles were finally included (S2 Table). The quality evaluation results showed that five articles were of strong quality, five were of moderate quality, and none were of weak quality (Table 3). Table 4 analyses the characteristics of these ten articles. Based on the country or region of the author’s university, five articles were from Europe (three from the United Kingdom, one each from Greece and Ireland)[19,31–34], two articles were from North America (both from the United States) [12,28], and three articles were from Asia (one each from Japan, South Korea, and Hong Kong, China)[29,30,35]. Download: PPT PowerPoint slide PNG larger image TIFF original image Table 4. Characteristics of the 10 articles. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310403.t004 Participant characteristics Out of ten articles, four studies focused on male golfers [12,19,28,31], one study focused on a female golfer [30], and three studies investigated mixed-gender populations [19,32,34]. Additionally, two studies did not specify the gender of the golfers [33,35]. Furthermore, MF effects varied across age groups: five studies involved participants aged 18–22 [28,29,33–35], three studies included golfers aged 22–30 [19,30,32], and two studies focused on golfers aged 30–50 [12,31]. The literature includes four studies involving beginner golfers [29,30,33,34], three studies focused on sub-professional golfers [12,19,31], and three studies on well-trained golfers [28,32,35]. MF-inducing interventions This systematic review included ten studies covering various MF interventions, including measures to induce MF and measures to recover from MF. As measures to induce MF, Shin et al. [30] required participants to transcribe a neutral text at their fastest speed participants in the experimental group had to omit all letters "e" and "t", inducing MF by violating habitual writing practices. Doan et al. [28] used a continuous 36-hole official competition and Runswick et al. [33] used a pressure competition scenario, simulating actual competition pressure to induce MF. Measures to recover from MF mainly included nutritional supplements and psychological strategies, such as Stevenson et al. [19] and Mumford et al.’s [12] caffeine-containing drinks and supplements, as well as Nagashima et al.’s [35] carbohydrate supplement gummies, aimed to alleviate MF, increase alertness, and reduce fatigue to improve performance. Psychological strategies aimed to improve performance by adjusting the athletes’ psychological state, such as Lam et al. [29] preparation and execution following a successful putt and Galanis et al. [34] self-talk strategy. The MF intervention scenarios included a laboratory scenario, a simulated match scenario, and an actual match scenario. Three studies focused on match scenarios [12,28,35], one study investigated simulated match scenarios [19], and six studies investigated laboratory scenarios that focused on technique/skill, specifically putting performance [29,30,31,32,33,34] The short-duration studies are all from the laboratory, while the long durations are all based on competition scenarios. For example, six studies examined activities lasting 1–30 minutes [29,30,31,32,33,34], three studies investigated activities lasting 120–240 minutes [12,19,35] and one study explored activities lasting over 240 minutes [28]. Because of the different research scenarios, the tools for detecting MF are categorized in two ways. Five out of ten studies detected MF through subjective measures of Visual Analogue Scales [19,28,35] and Questionnaire [30,34]. The other five studies identified the effects of MF through Cognitive Performance [12,29,31,32,33]. Effects of MF on golf performance Doan et al. [28] demonstrated in their study that physical fatigue and MF significantly increased as the number of holes played in a 36-hole golf tournament progressed, leading to a decline in overall golf scores. Shin et al. [30] supported this observation in a laboratory setting, showing that MF significantly impacts putting performance. Campbell et al. [31] and Carnegie et al. [32] explored the potential moderating effects of different skill levels of golfers on intervention outcomes. While their study designs varied, their results indicated potential differences between high and low-level golfers in response to MF and performance. Galanis et al. [34] found that self-talk strategies significantly reduced the occurrence of MF and markedly improved golf putting performance. This suggests that cognitive interventions can enhance athletes’ focus and performance levels. On the other hand, Mumford et al. [12] highlighted the benefits of caffeine supplementation in enhancing alertness, confidence, and attention during golf competition, while reducing the experience of MF. Nagashima et al. [35] further supported this finding, showing that continuous ingestion of carbohydrate supplements lowered perceived fatigue levels and increased attention and relaxation. Stevenson et al. [19] confirmed that using carbohydrate-electrolyte formulations containing caffeine significantly improved golf putting accuracy. Participant characteristics Out of ten articles, four studies focused on male golfers [12,19,28,31], one study focused on a female golfer [30], and three studies investigated mixed-gender populations [19,32,34]. Additionally, two studies did not specify the gender of the golfers [33,35]. Furthermore, MF effects varied across age groups: five studies involved participants aged 18–22 [28,29,33–35], three studies included golfers aged 22–30 [19,30,32], and two studies focused on golfers aged 30–50 [12,31]. The literature includes four studies involving beginner golfers [29,30,33,34], three studies focused on sub-professional golfers [12,19,31], and three studies on well-trained golfers [28,32,35]. MF-inducing interventions This systematic review included ten studies covering various MF interventions, including measures to induce MF and measures to recover from MF. As measures to induce MF, Shin et al. [30] required participants to transcribe a neutral text at their fastest speed participants in the experimental group had to omit all letters "e" and "t", inducing MF by violating habitual writing practices. Doan et al. [28] used a continuous 36-hole official competition and Runswick et al. [33] used a pressure competition scenario, simulating actual competition pressure to induce MF. Measures to recover from MF mainly included nutritional supplements and psychological strategies, such as Stevenson et al. [19] and Mumford et al.’s [12] caffeine-containing drinks and supplements, as well as Nagashima et al.’s [35] carbohydrate supplement gummies, aimed to alleviate MF, increase alertness, and reduce fatigue to improve performance. Psychological strategies aimed to improve performance by adjusting the athletes’ psychological state, such as Lam et al. [29] preparation and execution following a successful putt and Galanis et al. [34] self-talk strategy. The MF intervention scenarios included a laboratory scenario, a simulated match scenario, and an actual match scenario. Three studies focused on match scenarios [12,28,35], one study investigated simulated match scenarios [19], and six studies investigated laboratory scenarios that focused on technique/skill, specifically putting performance [29,30,31,32,33,34] The short-duration studies are all from the laboratory, while the long durations are all based on competition scenarios. For example, six studies examined activities lasting 1–30 minutes [29,30,31,32,33,34], three studies investigated activities lasting 120–240 minutes [12,19,35] and one study explored activities lasting over 240 minutes [28]. Because of the different research scenarios, the tools for detecting MF are categorized in two ways. Five out of ten studies detected MF through subjective measures of Visual Analogue Scales [19,28,35] and Questionnaire [30,34]. The other five studies identified the effects of MF through Cognitive Performance [12,29,31,32,33]. Effects of MF on golf performance Doan et al. [28] demonstrated in their study that physical fatigue and MF significantly increased as the number of holes played in a 36-hole golf tournament progressed, leading to a decline in overall golf scores. Shin et al. [30] supported this observation in a laboratory setting, showing that MF significantly impacts putting performance. Campbell et al. [31] and Carnegie et al. [32] explored the potential moderating effects of different skill levels of golfers on intervention outcomes. While their study designs varied, their results indicated potential differences between high and low-level golfers in response to MF and performance. Galanis et al. [34] found that self-talk strategies significantly reduced the occurrence of MF and markedly improved golf putting performance. This suggests that cognitive interventions can enhance athletes’ focus and performance levels. On the other hand, Mumford et al. [12] highlighted the benefits of caffeine supplementation in enhancing alertness, confidence, and attention during golf competition, while reducing the experience of MF. Nagashima et al. [35] further supported this finding, showing that continuous ingestion of carbohydrate supplements lowered perceived fatigue levels and increased attention and relaxation. Stevenson et al. [19] confirmed that using carbohydrate-electrolyte formulations containing caffeine significantly improved golf putting accuracy. Discussion Participant characteristics Only two studies focused on high-level athletes [12,28], and even four studies had participants who were beginners who had never been exposed to golf [29,30,33,34]. Because high-level athletes and beginners may exhibit significant differences in their responses to MF, intervention effects, and golf performance, results from studies on beginners cannot be directly generalized to high-level athletes. This limitation may restrict the generalizability of study findings. Focusing excessively on beginners or individuals who have never been exposed to golf may lead to gaps and shortcomings in theoretical development, intervention design, and practical application. To comprehensively understand the role of MF in golf, future research should prioritize investigating high-level athletes and conducting comparative analyses across different levels of athletes. MF-inducing interventions Prolonged engagement in competitive golf, with its unique demands, may induce hormonal changes that impact immune function and influence overall performance on the course. Doan et al. [28] shed light on the potential consequences of extended golfing on hormonal balance, particularly the delicate interplay between testosterone and cortisol levels. Elevated testosterone levels, often associated with aggressive moods, might not align with the requirements for optimal golf performance. Interestingly, the study by Filaire et al. [36] found essential connections among cortisol levels, physical state anxiety, and cognitive state anxiety. Considering the complex relationship between MF and hormonal fluctuations [28], understanding these physiological mechanisms remains a research gap, which will be a feasible direction for future research, such as how changes in hormone levels affect specific golf performance and how interventions can optimize these effects. According to Doan et al. [28], during a continuous 36-hole golf competition, golfers experienced an increase in MF with each successive hole, with significant increases in MF observed between holes 25–30 and 31–36. A possible reason for this is that a golf competition lasting over 5 hours requires athletes to engage in complex strategic thinking and decision-making. For instance, selecting the right club, deciding on shot strategies, and adjusting swing angles all demand high levels of cognitive load. Prolonged high cognitive load can lead to MF [37]. Furthermore, unlike sports such as basketball, soccer, and volleyball, which are played on standardized fields, golf courses are designed in vast natural environments [16]. Golfers must operate like snipers, facing challenges from nature and constantly adjusting their play based on factors such as the slope of the fairway, grass length, temperature, humidity, wind speed, and direction. This interesting phenomenon suggests that the competitive golf task in an outdoor setting can itself be a trigger for MF. Effects of MF on golf performance This review demonstrates that MF directly affects golf performance, including the overall score for 18 holes, iron club accuracy, drive distance, and especially impacts putting performance. Among the ten articles included in this review, seven studies only involved testing of golf putting [19,29–34]. As a result, the findings are limited and one-sided. The other three studies focused on 18-hole scores [12,28,25], with two of these studies examining driving performance [12,35], and only one study involving iron club tests [35]. These studies were conducted during outdoor 18-hole competitions, making it impossible to rule out various confounding factors of the outdoor environment. For example, Doan et al. [28] found that both physical fatigue and MF were significantly affected by the number of holes played and the duration of time, indicating that as a golf competition progresses, both physical fatigue and MF gradually increase, leading to a decline in golf performance. However, due to the difficulty in clearly distinguishing between physical fatigue and MF in this context, it is challenging to demonstrate that MF is the direct cause of the decline in golf performance. Based on the study designs included in this review, an effective method to avoid confounding factors is to induce MF in a controlled practice range or laboratory setting using specific tasks or tests and then accurately assess golf performance through golf-specific tests. Unfortunately, the results of this review show that the only golf-specific skill tested in such a controlled environment is putting, which is only 1 of 14 clubs in a professional golfer’s bag. This represents a significant research gap, and future studies must test more clubs and techniques. On the other hand, various studies suggest that the beneficial effects of caffeine can enhance cognitive function in tasks with high mental and physical demands, indicating that its effects are not limited to physical performance enhancement [12,38]. Stevenson et al. [19] found that dietary interventions, such as consuming sports drinks containing carbohydrates and caffeine, can positively impact mental function levels during golf activities, especially in putting. Ataka et al. [39] supported Stevenson et al.’s [19] findings, pointing out that caffeine intake can improve work performance by stimulating the central nervous system without increasing fatigue. For example, Judelson et al. [40] suggested that 100-mg caffeine significantly decreased lethargy/fatigue and increased vigor. Lorenzo Calvo et al. [41] indicated that coffee might improve attention. This highlights the potential impact of nutrition on mental function during golf competitions, warranting further exploration of nutritional interventions in the future. Participant characteristics Only two studies focused on high-level athletes [12,28], and even four studies had participants who were beginners who had never been exposed to golf [29,30,33,34]. Because high-level athletes and beginners may exhibit significant differences in their responses to MF, intervention effects, and golf performance, results from studies on beginners cannot be directly generalized to high-level athletes. This limitation may restrict the generalizability of study findings. Focusing excessively on beginners or individuals who have never been exposed to golf may lead to gaps and shortcomings in theoretical development, intervention design, and practical application. To comprehensively understand the role of MF in golf, future research should prioritize investigating high-level athletes and conducting comparative analyses across different levels of athletes. MF-inducing interventions Prolonged engagement in competitive golf, with its unique demands, may induce hormonal changes that impact immune function and influence overall performance on the course. Doan et al. [28] shed light on the potential consequences of extended golfing on hormonal balance, particularly the delicate interplay between testosterone and cortisol levels. Elevated testosterone levels, often associated with aggressive moods, might not align with the requirements for optimal golf performance. Interestingly, the study by Filaire et al. [36] found essential connections among cortisol levels, physical state anxiety, and cognitive state anxiety. Considering the complex relationship between MF and hormonal fluctuations [28], understanding these physiological mechanisms remains a research gap, which will be a feasible direction for future research, such as how changes in hormone levels affect specific golf performance and how interventions can optimize these effects. According to Doan et al. [28], during a continuous 36-hole golf competition, golfers experienced an increase in MF with each successive hole, with significant increases in MF observed between holes 25–30 and 31–36. A possible reason for this is that a golf competition lasting over 5 hours requires athletes to engage in complex strategic thinking and decision-making. For instance, selecting the right club, deciding on shot strategies, and adjusting swing angles all demand high levels of cognitive load. Prolonged high cognitive load can lead to MF [37]. Furthermore, unlike sports such as basketball, soccer, and volleyball, which are played on standardized fields, golf courses are designed in vast natural environments [16]. Golfers must operate like snipers, facing challenges from nature and constantly adjusting their play based on factors such as the slope of the fairway, grass length, temperature, humidity, wind speed, and direction. This interesting phenomenon suggests that the competitive golf task in an outdoor setting can itself be a trigger for MF. Effects of MF on golf performance This review demonstrates that MF directly affects golf performance, including the overall score for 18 holes, iron club accuracy, drive distance, and especially impacts putting performance. Among the ten articles included in this review, seven studies only involved testing of golf putting [19,29–34]. As a result, the findings are limited and one-sided. The other three studies focused on 18-hole scores [12,28,25], with two of these studies examining driving performance [12,35], and only one study involving iron club tests [35]. These studies were conducted during outdoor 18-hole competitions, making it impossible to rule out various confounding factors of the outdoor environment. For example, Doan et al. [28] found that both physical fatigue and MF were significantly affected by the number of holes played and the duration of time, indicating that as a golf competition progresses, both physical fatigue and MF gradually increase, leading to a decline in golf performance. However, due to the difficulty in clearly distinguishing between physical fatigue and MF in this context, it is challenging to demonstrate that MF is the direct cause of the decline in golf performance. Based on the study designs included in this review, an effective method to avoid confounding factors is to induce MF in a controlled practice range or laboratory setting using specific tasks or tests and then accurately assess golf performance through golf-specific tests. Unfortunately, the results of this review show that the only golf-specific skill tested in such a controlled environment is putting, which is only 1 of 14 clubs in a professional golfer’s bag. This represents a significant research gap, and future studies must test more clubs and techniques. On the other hand, various studies suggest that the beneficial effects of caffeine can enhance cognitive function in tasks with high mental and physical demands, indicating that its effects are not limited to physical performance enhancement [12,38]. Stevenson et al. [19] found that dietary interventions, such as consuming sports drinks containing carbohydrates and caffeine, can positively impact mental function levels during golf activities, especially in putting. Ataka et al. [39] supported Stevenson et al.’s [19] findings, pointing out that caffeine intake can improve work performance by stimulating the central nervous system without increasing fatigue. For example, Judelson et al. [40] suggested that 100-mg caffeine significantly decreased lethargy/fatigue and increased vigor. Lorenzo Calvo et al. [41] indicated that coffee might improve attention. This highlights the potential impact of nutrition on mental function during golf competitions, warranting further exploration of nutritional interventions in the future. Conclusions In conclusion, investigating MF within the context of golf based on findings from ten different studies offers valuable insights into its diverse effects on golf performance. The intensity of MF experienced during golf activities appears to be influenced by factors such as extended game duration, extensive walking distances, and environmental conditions. Hormonal dynamics, particularly the intricate interaction between testosterone and cortisol levels, contribute significantly to the nuanced relationship between MF and golf performance. Interventions such as dietary adjustments, caffeine supplementation, and stress management techniques are identified as potential strategies to modulate MF levels during a golf round. Regarding the impact of MF on golf performance, sustained participation in competitive golf can induce physical and MF, impacting both energy reserves and cognitive sharpness. Caffeine supplementation is viable to alleviate perceived fatigue, sustain energy levels, and enhance overall golf performance. While initial instances of MF, as indicated by ego depletion and anxiety, may initially impede performance, repeated exposure and recovery of self-control resources can mitigate these effects over time. Skilled golfers often exhibit lower cognitive load during golf tasks, resulting in more efficient and consistent performance outcomes. Limitations Only published articles were included in this review. Therefore, the results of the study may be affected by publication bias. In addition, this study excluded qualitative research, which may have limited the understanding of golfers’ perceptions and experiences of MF. Three of the ten articles selected were conducted during an 18-hole tournament, an outdoor bustling environment which may have interfered with the findings. Finally, the lack of articles on golf-specific skill tests other than putting limits the overall understanding of the knowledge of the effects of MF on golf performance. Supporting information S1 Table. Detailed search strategy. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310403.s001 (PDF) S2 Table. Numbered table of all studies. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310403.s002 (PDF) S1 File. PRISMA checklist. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0310403.s003 (DOCX) Acknowledgments The authors would like to sincerely thank all the authors for their contribution to the completion of this study. TI - Mental fatigue in golf: A systematic review JF - PLoS ONE DO - 10.1371/journal.pone.0310403 DA - 2025-02-20 UR - https://www.deepdyve.com/lp/public-library-of-science-plos-journal/mental-fatigue-in-golf-a-systematic-review-L2acu00ZFj SP - e0310403 VL - 20 IS - 2 DP - DeepDyve ER -