TY - JOUR AU - Hancox, Robert, J AB - Abstract Background The rising prevalence of atopic diseases implies a strong influence of environmental determinants. Epidemiological studies have identified several early life exposures that appear to influence the risk of developing atopic sensitization, but the combined influence of these exposures is unknown. We sought to estimate the proportion of atopy that could be attributed to common childhood exposures associated with atopic sensitization in adolescence and young adulthood. Methods Atopic sensitization was measured by skin-prick tests for common aeroallergens in a population-based New Zealand birth cohort at ages 13 and 32 years. The independent effects of previously identified risk and protective factors for atopic sensitization were assessed using multiple logistic regression. Population attributable fractions were calculated for atopic sensitization in childhood and adulthood. Results Tobacco smoke exposure, dog and cat ownership, nail-biting and thumb-sucking, attending pre-school day care, and household crowding were associated with a lower risk of atopic sensitization whereas breastfeeding was associated with a higher risk. Population attributable fractions for combined effects of these environmental factors suggest that they may account for 58% of atopic sensitization at age 13 and 49% at age 32 years. Conclusions A substantial proportion of atopic sensitization appears to be attributable to common childhood environmental and lifestyle factors, and the influence of these exposures persists into adulthood. The absolute risks attributable to these exposures will be different in other cohorts and we cannot assume that these associations are necessarily causal. Nevertheless, the findings suggest that identifiable childhood environmental factors contribute substantially to atopic sensitization. Atopic hypersensitivity, breastfeeding, crowding, child day care, environmental tobacco smoke, nail-biting, pets, thumb-sucking Key Messages The majority of atopic sensitization in adolescence may be attributable to the combined influence of common identifiable childhood exposures. The influence of these childhood exposures on atopic sensitization persists into adulthood. Many of the exposures that appear to increase the risk for atopy have other health and social benefits, and their avoidance may be undesirable. Introduction Over recent decades the prevalence of atopy and its related diseases has increased substantially in Western countries.1,2 Whereas the development of atopy is likely to involve interactions between genetic and environmental factors, the rapid increase in the prevalence of atopy implies a strong influence of environmental determinants.3,4 Numerous environmental exposures in early childhood have been associated with atopic sensitization. Previous reports from the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development study found that breastfeeding was associated with an increased risk of atopic sensitization,5 whereas exposure to both cats and dogs,6 tobacco smoke,7 and thumb-sucking and nail-biting in childhood appear to be protective against atopic sensitization.8 The study did not find a clear association between atopy and birth order,9 but the risk of atopy has been found to be inversely associated with household crowding at the age of 3 years.6,8 Some of these observations are controversial, as the apparent protective effect of smoking and the increased risk of atopy with breastfeeding contradict widely held beliefs, although each of these observations has been supported by other studies.10–12 Breastfeeding in particular has been promoted as a protective factor against atopy in childhood,13 yet several studies have failed to confirm these protective effects.14–16 Similarly, the literature is divided on whether exposure to tobacco smoke influences the risk of developing atopic sensitization.10,17–20 Furthermore, despite numerous studies identifying associations with other environmental exposures such as ownership of pets,21–23 household crowding,24,25 and day care attendance,25–27 the overall impact of these exposures remains uncertain. The issue is made more complicated by changes in the pattern of atopic sensitization with age and sex: boys are more likely to develop atopic diseases in early childhood compared with girls,28 whereas adult women report more atopy than men.29 Genetic risk factors undoubtedly play a large role: a family history of atopy is considered the strongest risk factor for atopy development, although this may be explained by shared environmental exposures as well as genes.12 Most studies investigating the association between environmental predictors and atopy have focused on individual exposures, and it is unclear to what extent multiple environmental exposures influence the risk of atopy. Despite the known inverse association between the number of siblings and risk for atopy, Upchurch et al. found that only 3% of the increase in the prevalence of atopy over recent decades could be attributed to decreasing family size.30 To our knowledge there are no published studies assessing the combined effect of multiple risk factors on atopic sensitization in childhood and adulthood. To address this, we explored the combined effect of common childhood environmental exposures previously found to predict the risk for atopic sensitization in the Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development study and other research. Our aim was to estimate what proportion of atopic sensitization could be attributed to these exposures in a population-based birth cohort at ages 13 and 32 years. Methods The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development study is a population-based birth cohort study of 1037 participants born in Queen Mary Maternity Hospital, Dunedin, New Zealand, in 1972–73. Children still living in the greater Dunedin area were invited to the first follow-up at age 3 years. The 1037 children (91% of those eligible; 52% male) who attended this assessment formed the cohort for further follow-up assessments, which have been done at ages 5, 7, 9, 11, 13, 15, 18, 21, 26, 32 and 38 years. The cohort represents the full range of socioeconomic status in the South Island of New Zealand, and the study participants are mostly of New Zealand European ethnicity, reflecting the ethnic mix of the South Island at that time. For each assessment, study members are invited back to the research unit. The study has a high rate of retention: 95% of the surviving cohort participated in the latest assessment at age 38. A full description of the study is reported elsewhere.31 The Otago Ethics Committee approved the study, and written informed consent was obtained at each assessment. Atopic sensitization Atopic sensitization was first measured at age 13 years when 724 of 1031 (70%) study members consented to skin prick tests for 11 common inhaled allergens: house dust mite (Dermatophagoides pteronyssinus; Bencard, Brentford, UK), grass, cat, dog, horse, kapok, wool, Aspergillus fumigatus, Alternaria, Penicillium and Cladosporium (Hollister-Stier, Washington, USA). Skin prick testing was performed in 946 of 1015 (93%) of the surviving study members at age 32 years. We used the same allergens at age 32 as at age 13, with the addition of cockroach, but due to necessity allergens were obtained from a different manufacturer (ALK, supplied by Allergy Canada, Thornhill, ON, Canada). In keeping with previous reports from this cohort, a positive response to skin prick testing was defined by a wheal diameter at least 2 mm greater than the negative saline control. Atopic sensitization was defined as having at least one positive response to any allergen.6 Environmental exposures Environmental risk and protective factors were assessed at interviews throughout the follow-up period. At age 3 years, the child’s mother was asked about the initiation and duration of breastfeeding and in the majority of cases this was verified from prospective visiting nurse records.5 Participants were considered to have been breastfed if they had been breastfed until at least 4 weeks of age. An index of exposure to other children/household crowding was derived from the number of children living in the house at participant age 3 years, divided by the reported number of rooms (excluding kitchens and bathrooms). Households were regarded as uncrowded if there were more than two rooms per child.6 Day care attendance at age 3 years was reported by the parent attending with the child. Retrospective information regarding cat and dog ownership during childhood was obtained at age 9 years. Only ownership of both a dog and a cat has been found to be associated with a lower risk of atopy in this cohort, so only children with both pets were regarded as exposed.6 Parental smoking history was obtained from a parent at the assessments at ages 7, 9 and 11 years. At age 13 years, the study members themselves were asked whether their parents smoked. Participants were regarded as being exposed to parental smoking if either parent smoked at any of these ages.7 Children were considered to be thumb-suckers or nail-biters if their parents reported that they frequently bit their nails or sucked their thumbs at any of the assessments at ages 5, 7, 9 or 11 years.8 A history of asthma and hay fever in the child’s natural mother and father was obtained from the parents at the age 7 years assessment and from the study members themselves at age 18 years. A parental history of atopy was considered positive if either parent had a history of hay fever or asthma.7 Childhood socioeconomic status was recorded at each assessment between birth and age 13 using a 6-point scale (1 = high, 6 = low) based on the occupational status of either parent as previously described.32 A mean score between 1 and 3 across these assessments was considered a high socioeconomic status. Statistical analysis Associations between each childhood exposures and atopic sensitization were first confirmed in unadjusted logistic regression models using a positive skin prick test to any allergen at age 13 or 32 years as the primary outcome. Protective environment factors were reverse-coded so that they appeared as risk factors. Subsequently, all exposures were included in adjusted logistic regression analyses. Population attributable fractions associated with the exposures were calculated from these multivariable models. Sex and parental history of atopy were included in these adjusted models, but were not included in the estimates of population attributable fractions because they were not deemed to be modifiable environmental exposures. Population attributable fractions (also known as population attributable risks) estimate the proportion of disease in the population attributable to the exposure, based on the relative risk of developing the disease among the exposed compared with the unexposed and the proportion of the population exposed to the risk factor. They provide an estimate of the fraction of disease that would be prevented if the risk factor could be eliminated from the population. Population attributable fractions can be derived from logistic regression using maximum likelihood estimation.33 However, a known problem with population attributable fractions from multivariable models is that the individual risks can be overestimated and the sum of these can exceed the overall attributable risk (and even exceed 100%), because most individuals are exposed to more than one risk and the estimated attributable fraction is dependent on the sequence of removing each risk factor from the model.34,35 This problem can be overcome by sequentially removing each of the risk factors from the logistic regression model in every possible order and averaging the attributable fraction obtained for each factor.35,36 We used the averaging procedure described by Rückinger et al. to do this.36 This procedure does not calculate confidence intervals directly, so these were generated by bootstrapping 1000 samples and reporting the resulting bias-corrected estimates.37 Analyses were performed using Stata versions 13 or 14 (StataCorp, College Station, TX, USA) Results At age 13 years, more boys had positive skin prick tests than girls. At age 32 years, there was no longer evidence of a sex difference in atopic sensitization. There were no sex differences observed for exposure to any of the environmental risk factors (Table 1). Only 70% of participants consented to skin prick testing at age 13 years, whereas 93% of the living cohort received skin prick testing at age 32 years. Those who did not undergo the test at age 13 years had a similar prevalence of atopic sensitization at age 32 years compared with those who did (56% vs 60%, P = 0.22); 33% (126/377) of those with negative tests at age 13 had positive tests at age 32. Only 6% (18/310) of those with positive tests at age 13 had negative tests at age 32. Table 1 Prevalence of environmental exposures and outcomes n Female (%) Male (%) P Atopic sensitization at 13 years 724 132/349 (38) 196/375 (52) <0.001 Atopic sensitization at 32 years 946 271/467 (58) 294/479 (61) 0.294 Parental history of atopy 954 201/460 (44) 219/494 (44) 0.843 Breastfed 1035 248/501 (50) 254/534 (48) 0.534 Parental smoking 972 299/468 (64) 318/504 (63) 0.797 Cat and dog ownership 815 158/387 (41) 174/428 (41) 0.960 Household crowding 1037 202/502 (40) 229/535 (43) 0.402 Thumb-sucking or nailbiting at 5–11 years 1013 164/492 (33) 153/521 (29) 0.174 Day care attendance 1027 88/497 (18) 85/530 (16) 0.475 n Female (%) Male (%) P Atopic sensitization at 13 years 724 132/349 (38) 196/375 (52) <0.001 Atopic sensitization at 32 years 946 271/467 (58) 294/479 (61) 0.294 Parental history of atopy 954 201/460 (44) 219/494 (44) 0.843 Breastfed 1035 248/501 (50) 254/534 (48) 0.534 Parental smoking 972 299/468 (64) 318/504 (63) 0.797 Cat and dog ownership 815 158/387 (41) 174/428 (41) 0.960 Household crowding 1037 202/502 (40) 229/535 (43) 0.402 Thumb-sucking or nailbiting at 5–11 years 1013 164/492 (33) 153/521 (29) 0.174 Day care attendance 1027 88/497 (18) 85/530 (16) 0.475 P values <0.05 are highlighted in bold. Table 1 Prevalence of environmental exposures and outcomes n Female (%) Male (%) P Atopic sensitization at 13 years 724 132/349 (38) 196/375 (52) <0.001 Atopic sensitization at 32 years 946 271/467 (58) 294/479 (61) 0.294 Parental history of atopy 954 201/460 (44) 219/494 (44) 0.843 Breastfed 1035 248/501 (50) 254/534 (48) 0.534 Parental smoking 972 299/468 (64) 318/504 (63) 0.797 Cat and dog ownership 815 158/387 (41) 174/428 (41) 0.960 Household crowding 1037 202/502 (40) 229/535 (43) 0.402 Thumb-sucking or nailbiting at 5–11 years 1013 164/492 (33) 153/521 (29) 0.174 Day care attendance 1027 88/497 (18) 85/530 (16) 0.475 n Female (%) Male (%) P Atopic sensitization at 13 years 724 132/349 (38) 196/375 (52) <0.001 Atopic sensitization at 32 years 946 271/467 (58) 294/479 (61) 0.294 Parental history of atopy 954 201/460 (44) 219/494 (44) 0.843 Breastfed 1035 248/501 (50) 254/534 (48) 0.534 Parental smoking 972 299/468 (64) 318/504 (63) 0.797 Cat and dog ownership 815 158/387 (41) 174/428 (41) 0.960 Household crowding 1037 202/502 (40) 229/535 (43) 0.402 Thumb-sucking or nailbiting at 5–11 years 1013 164/492 (33) 153/521 (29) 0.174 Day care attendance 1027 88/497 (18) 85/530 (16) 0.475 P values <0.05 are highlighted in bold. As expected, all previously identified environmental exposures analysed were predictors of atopic sensitization in our cohort at both age 13 and age 32 years. Not attending day care showed a tendency to an increased risk of atopic sensitization at age 13, but was not a predictor at age 32 years (Table 2). Table 2 Unadjusted associations between childhood exposures and atopic sensitization at ages 13 and 32 years Age 13 Age 32 n OR 95% CI P n OR 95% CI P Male sex 724 1.80 1.33, 2.42 < 0.001 946 1.15 0.89, 1.49 0.294 Parental history of atopy 712 1.69 1.25, 2.28 0.001 893 1.79 1.36, 2.35 < 0.001 Breastfed for 4 weeks of longer 722 1.87 1.39, 2.52 < 0.001 944 1.44 1.11, 1.87 0.006 Parents were non-smokers at ages 7 to 13 724 1.60 1.18, 2.17 0.002 903 1.42 1.01, 1.87 0.013 No household crowding at age 3 724 1.56 1.16, 2.10 0.004 946 1.49 1.14, 1.94 0.003 Didn’t have a cat and a dog before age 9 688 1.84 1.35, 2.51 < 0.001 767 1.56 1.16, 2.10 0.003 Didn’t attend day care at age 3 718 1.45 0.96, 2.18 0.079 937 1.13 0.79, 1.61 0.508 Did not suck thumb or bite nails between ages 5 to 11 years 724 1.52 1.11, 2.10 0.010 935 1.64 1.24, 2.17 0.001 Age 13 Age 32 n OR 95% CI P n OR 95% CI P Male sex 724 1.80 1.33, 2.42 < 0.001 946 1.15 0.89, 1.49 0.294 Parental history of atopy 712 1.69 1.25, 2.28 0.001 893 1.79 1.36, 2.35 < 0.001 Breastfed for 4 weeks of longer 722 1.87 1.39, 2.52 < 0.001 944 1.44 1.11, 1.87 0.006 Parents were non-smokers at ages 7 to 13 724 1.60 1.18, 2.17 0.002 903 1.42 1.01, 1.87 0.013 No household crowding at age 3 724 1.56 1.16, 2.10 0.004 946 1.49 1.14, 1.94 0.003 Didn’t have a cat and a dog before age 9 688 1.84 1.35, 2.51 < 0.001 767 1.56 1.16, 2.10 0.003 Didn’t attend day care at age 3 718 1.45 0.96, 2.18 0.079 937 1.13 0.79, 1.61 0.508 Did not suck thumb or bite nails between ages 5 to 11 years 724 1.52 1.11, 2.10 0.010 935 1.64 1.24, 2.17 0.001 Table 2 Unadjusted associations between childhood exposures and atopic sensitization at ages 13 and 32 years Age 13 Age 32 n OR 95% CI P n OR 95% CI P Male sex 724 1.80 1.33, 2.42 < 0.001 946 1.15 0.89, 1.49 0.294 Parental history of atopy 712 1.69 1.25, 2.28 0.001 893 1.79 1.36, 2.35 < 0.001 Breastfed for 4 weeks of longer 722 1.87 1.39, 2.52 < 0.001 944 1.44 1.11, 1.87 0.006 Parents were non-smokers at ages 7 to 13 724 1.60 1.18, 2.17 0.002 903 1.42 1.01, 1.87 0.013 No household crowding at age 3 724 1.56 1.16, 2.10 0.004 946 1.49 1.14, 1.94 0.003 Didn’t have a cat and a dog before age 9 688 1.84 1.35, 2.51 < 0.001 767 1.56 1.16, 2.10 0.003 Didn’t attend day care at age 3 718 1.45 0.96, 2.18 0.079 937 1.13 0.79, 1.61 0.508 Did not suck thumb or bite nails between ages 5 to 11 years 724 1.52 1.11, 2.10 0.010 935 1.64 1.24, 2.17 0.001 Age 13 Age 32 n OR 95% CI P n OR 95% CI P Male sex 724 1.80 1.33, 2.42 < 0.001 946 1.15 0.89, 1.49 0.294 Parental history of atopy 712 1.69 1.25, 2.28 0.001 893 1.79 1.36, 2.35 < 0.001 Breastfed for 4 weeks of longer 722 1.87 1.39, 2.52 < 0.001 944 1.44 1.11, 1.87 0.006 Parents were non-smokers at ages 7 to 13 724 1.60 1.18, 2.17 0.002 903 1.42 1.01, 1.87 0.013 No household crowding at age 3 724 1.56 1.16, 2.10 0.004 946 1.49 1.14, 1.94 0.003 Didn’t have a cat and a dog before age 9 688 1.84 1.35, 2.51 < 0.001 767 1.56 1.16, 2.10 0.003 Didn’t attend day care at age 3 718 1.45 0.96, 2.18 0.079 937 1.13 0.79, 1.61 0.508 Did not suck thumb or bite nails between ages 5 to 11 years 724 1.52 1.11, 2.10 0.010 935 1.64 1.24, 2.17 0.001 When modelled simultaneously, most of the environmental exposures remained associated with atopic sensitization. Day care attendance was not associated with atopic sensitization at either age in adjusted models (Table 3). Table 3 Adjusted analyses of the association between independent environmental exposures and atopic sensitization at 13 and 32 years Age 13 (n = 671) Age 32 (n = 743) OR 95% CI P OR 95% CI P Male sex 1.94 1.41, 2.68 <0.001 1.29 0.95, 1.76 0.098 Parental history of atopy 1.63 1.17, 2.26 0.003 1.53 1.12, 2.10 0.008 Breastfed for 4 weeks of longer 1.70 1.23, 2.35 0.001 1.53 1.12, 2.10 0.007 Parents were non-smokers at ages 7 to 13 1.38 0.99, 1.93 0.058 1.32 0.95, 1.83 0.095 No household crowding at age 3 1.43 1.03, 1.99 0.034 1.43 1.04, 1.96 0.025 Didn’t have a cat and a dog before age 9 1.63 1.16, 2.28 0.004 1.38 1.01, 1.89 0.044 Didn’t attend day care at age 3 1.50 0.96, 2.34 0.078 1.36 0.90, 2.05 0.141 Did not suck thumb or bite nails between ages 5 to 11 years 1.56 1.10, 2.20 0.012 1.65 1.19, 2.27 0.003 Age 13 (n = 671) Age 32 (n = 743) OR 95% CI P OR 95% CI P Male sex 1.94 1.41, 2.68 <0.001 1.29 0.95, 1.76 0.098 Parental history of atopy 1.63 1.17, 2.26 0.003 1.53 1.12, 2.10 0.008 Breastfed for 4 weeks of longer 1.70 1.23, 2.35 0.001 1.53 1.12, 2.10 0.007 Parents were non-smokers at ages 7 to 13 1.38 0.99, 1.93 0.058 1.32 0.95, 1.83 0.095 No household crowding at age 3 1.43 1.03, 1.99 0.034 1.43 1.04, 1.96 0.025 Didn’t have a cat and a dog before age 9 1.63 1.16, 2.28 0.004 1.38 1.01, 1.89 0.044 Didn’t attend day care at age 3 1.50 0.96, 2.34 0.078 1.36 0.90, 2.05 0.141 Did not suck thumb or bite nails between ages 5 to 11 years 1.56 1.10, 2.20 0.012 1.65 1.19, 2.27 0.003 P values <0.05 are highlighted in bold. Table 3 Adjusted analyses of the association between independent environmental exposures and atopic sensitization at 13 and 32 years Age 13 (n = 671) Age 32 (n = 743) OR 95% CI P OR 95% CI P Male sex 1.94 1.41, 2.68 <0.001 1.29 0.95, 1.76 0.098 Parental history of atopy 1.63 1.17, 2.26 0.003 1.53 1.12, 2.10 0.008 Breastfed for 4 weeks of longer 1.70 1.23, 2.35 0.001 1.53 1.12, 2.10 0.007 Parents were non-smokers at ages 7 to 13 1.38 0.99, 1.93 0.058 1.32 0.95, 1.83 0.095 No household crowding at age 3 1.43 1.03, 1.99 0.034 1.43 1.04, 1.96 0.025 Didn’t have a cat and a dog before age 9 1.63 1.16, 2.28 0.004 1.38 1.01, 1.89 0.044 Didn’t attend day care at age 3 1.50 0.96, 2.34 0.078 1.36 0.90, 2.05 0.141 Did not suck thumb or bite nails between ages 5 to 11 years 1.56 1.10, 2.20 0.012 1.65 1.19, 2.27 0.003 Age 13 (n = 671) Age 32 (n = 743) OR 95% CI P OR 95% CI P Male sex 1.94 1.41, 2.68 <0.001 1.29 0.95, 1.76 0.098 Parental history of atopy 1.63 1.17, 2.26 0.003 1.53 1.12, 2.10 0.008 Breastfed for 4 weeks of longer 1.70 1.23, 2.35 0.001 1.53 1.12, 2.10 0.007 Parents were non-smokers at ages 7 to 13 1.38 0.99, 1.93 0.058 1.32 0.95, 1.83 0.095 No household crowding at age 3 1.43 1.03, 1.99 0.034 1.43 1.04, 1.96 0.025 Didn’t have a cat and a dog before age 9 1.63 1.16, 2.28 0.004 1.38 1.01, 1.89 0.044 Didn’t attend day care at age 3 1.50 0.96, 2.34 0.078 1.36 0.90, 2.05 0.141 Did not suck thumb or bite nails between ages 5 to 11 years 1.56 1.10, 2.20 0.012 1.65 1.19, 2.27 0.003 P values <0.05 are highlighted in bold. The risk of developing atopic sensitization increased with the number of risk factors that children were exposed to. Only two participants were exposed to none of the risk factors. Children with one risk factor or fewer had a 25% risk of sensitization, whereas those with all six (breastfeeding, and absence of exposures to dogs and cats, parental smoking, household crowding, nail-biting or thumb-sucking, and day care attendance) had a 70% risk of sensitization (Figure 1). Similarly, the prevalence of atopic sensitization at age 32 increased with the number of childhood risk factors (Figure 2). Figure 1 Open in new tabDownload slide Association between atopic sensitization at age 13 years and the number of exposed environmental factors. Exposures assessed were: breastfeeding, no childhood exposure to tobacco smoke, no cat and dog ownership, no thumb-sucking or nail-biting, no day care attendance, and no household crowding. Non-parametric test of statistical significance across ordered groups P < 0.001. Figure 1 Open in new tabDownload slide Association between atopic sensitization at age 13 years and the number of exposed environmental factors. Exposures assessed were: breastfeeding, no childhood exposure to tobacco smoke, no cat and dog ownership, no thumb-sucking or nail-biting, no day care attendance, and no household crowding. Non-parametric test of statistical significance across ordered groups P < 0.001. Figure 2 Open in new tabDownload slide Association between atopic sensitization at age 32 years and the number of exposed environmental factors. Exposures assessed were: breastfeeding, no childhood exposure to tobacco smoke, no cat and dog ownership, no thumb-sucking or nail-biting, no day care attendance, and no household crowding. Non-parametric test of statistical significance across ordered groups P < 0.001. Figure 2 Open in new tabDownload slide Association between atopic sensitization at age 32 years and the number of exposed environmental factors. Exposures assessed were: breastfeeding, no childhood exposure to tobacco smoke, no cat and dog ownership, no thumb-sucking or nail-biting, no day care attendance, and no household crowding. Non-parametric test of statistical significance across ordered groups P < 0.001. Population attributable risk (PAF) analyses estimated that 58% of atopic sensitization in our cohort at age 13 years and 49% at age 32 years could be ‘attributed’ to the modifiable environmental risk factors in the multivariable models (Table 4). Whereas the focus here is on potentially modifiable factors, adding sex and parent report of atopy to the calculation of the PAFs increased these to 80% and 61%, respectively. Table 4 Population attributable fractions (PAFs) for early childhood exposures on atopic sensitization Age 13 (n = 671) Age 32 (n = 743) PAF 95% CI PAF 95% CI Breastfed 0.10 0.04, 0.17 0.08 0.02, 0.14 No parental smoking 0.05 0.00, 0.10 0.04 0.00, 0.09 No household crowding 0.08 0.01, 0.15 0.08 0.00, 0.15 No cats and dogs 0.11 0.04, 0.20 0.07 0.00, 0.14 No day care 0.12 0.00, 0.25 0.10 0.00, 0.22 No thumb-sucking/nail-biting 0.11 0.03, 0.20 0.13 0.04, 0.20 Total 0.58 0.43, 0.70 0.49 0.32, 0.64 Age 13 (n = 671) Age 32 (n = 743) PAF 95% CI PAF 95% CI Breastfed 0.10 0.04, 0.17 0.08 0.02, 0.14 No parental smoking 0.05 0.00, 0.10 0.04 0.00, 0.09 No household crowding 0.08 0.01, 0.15 0.08 0.00, 0.15 No cats and dogs 0.11 0.04, 0.20 0.07 0.00, 0.14 No day care 0.12 0.00, 0.25 0.10 0.00, 0.22 No thumb-sucking/nail-biting 0.11 0.03, 0.20 0.13 0.04, 0.20 Total 0.58 0.43, 0.70 0.49 0.32, 0.64 95% confidence intervals from 1000 bootstrapped samples (see text). Table 4 Population attributable fractions (PAFs) for early childhood exposures on atopic sensitization Age 13 (n = 671) Age 32 (n = 743) PAF 95% CI PAF 95% CI Breastfed 0.10 0.04, 0.17 0.08 0.02, 0.14 No parental smoking 0.05 0.00, 0.10 0.04 0.00, 0.09 No household crowding 0.08 0.01, 0.15 0.08 0.00, 0.15 No cats and dogs 0.11 0.04, 0.20 0.07 0.00, 0.14 No day care 0.12 0.00, 0.25 0.10 0.00, 0.22 No thumb-sucking/nail-biting 0.11 0.03, 0.20 0.13 0.04, 0.20 Total 0.58 0.43, 0.70 0.49 0.32, 0.64 Age 13 (n = 671) Age 32 (n = 743) PAF 95% CI PAF 95% CI Breastfed 0.10 0.04, 0.17 0.08 0.02, 0.14 No parental smoking 0.05 0.00, 0.10 0.04 0.00, 0.09 No household crowding 0.08 0.01, 0.15 0.08 0.00, 0.15 No cats and dogs 0.11 0.04, 0.20 0.07 0.00, 0.14 No day care 0.12 0.00, 0.25 0.10 0.00, 0.22 No thumb-sucking/nail-biting 0.11 0.03, 0.20 0.13 0.04, 0.20 Total 0.58 0.43, 0.70 0.49 0.32, 0.64 95% confidence intervals from 1000 bootstrapped samples (see text). Discussion This study suggests that a substantial proportion of atopic sensitization may be attributable to common identifiable childhood exposures, with 58% of the risk for atopic sensitization at age 13 years and 49% of atopic sensitization at age 32 years apparently explained by the combined influence of breastfeeding, and the absence of exposure to dogs and cats, parental smoking, household crowding, nail-biting or thumb-sucking, and day care attendance. To our knowledge this is the first prospective study to report the combined effect of multiple environmental factors on objective measures of atopic sensitization in childhood and adulthood. It must be acknowledged that the calculation of population attributable risks is based on the assumption that the risk factors are causal, rather than simply associated with the outcome through other mechanisms. The size of the attributable risks would also differ in other populations: not only because there are likely to be differences in the strength of association between these risk factors and the development of atopy, but also because the prevalences of the exposures will be different. Nevertheless, the overall findings of this study suggest that a large proportion of atopic sensitization can be explained by specific childhood exposures. As might be expected, the attributable risks due to early life exposures were higher in childhood than adulthood. A third of those with negative skin prick tests at age 13 years had positive tests at age 32. None of the early childhood exposures that we studied predicted the development of sensitization between ages 13 and 32 years (data not shown), but we have previously shown that adolescent and early adult exposures to dogs and cats and smoking influence the risk of atopy developing after age 13,6,7 and we would expect other adolescent or adult exposures to influence this as well. Despite this, half of adult atopic sensitization could still be attributed to these childhood exposures. If we included the additional influences of non-modifiable factors, namely a parental history of atopy and male sex, the attributable risks were 80% and 61% at ages 13 and 32 years, respectively. Perhaps surprisingly, the strength of association between a family history of atopy and atopic sensitization was of a similar magnitude to several of the individual environmental exposures (Tables 2 and 3). Although this may partly be due to the fact that the family history was based on self-reports of atopic disease rather than objective testing, this finding also indicates that genetic and other familial factors may be less important than the combined influence of the environment. Although we focused on potentially modifiable childhood exposures, altering these exposures would not necessarily be easy or desirable. There are many reasons to encourage breastfeeding and to avoid exposure to environmental tobacco smoke and household overcrowding, even if these were proven to increase the risk of atopic sensitization. Nail-biting and thumb-sucking may have other adverse effects and are widely regarded as undesirable habits,38,39 and cat and dog ownership is highly dependent on family and living circumstances. The public health implications of these findings are therefore unclear. Despite this, the extent of the relationship between environmental exposures and atopy remains poorly understood, and knowledge of the early life exposures that lead to these diseases is needed to enable early prevention. Many of the risk factors that we have studied are controversial: some of the environmental exposures that we found to be associated with atopy have also been reported to be protective factors (and vice versa). Hence, it may be that the direction as well as the strength of the associations between these exposures and atopic sensitization may be different in other populations. Several explanations have been proposed for the heterogeneity of results from different studies, including differences in measurement outcomes (different ages tested, dissimilar environmental exposures)13 and differences in the quality of the collected data (retrospective vs prospective collection, lack of standard definitions).19 There are also reported effects of genetic polymorphisms affecting the response to exposures such as breastfeeding, which we have not assessed in this analysis.40 Although higher socioeconomic status is also associated with atopic sensitization in this cohort in univariable analyses,32 we did not include it in our main analyses because we think that it is likely to be an indirect measure of other exposures rather than a direct exposure. When added to the multivariable analyses, a high childhood socioeconomic status did not predict sensitization at either age 13 [odds ratio (OR) = 1.13; 95% confidence interval (CI): 0.79, 1.62; P = 0.507] or 32 years (OR = 1.01; 95% CI: 0.72, 1.42; P = 0.958). Adding socioeconomic status also did not materially change any of the associations reported in Table 3. Strengths of this study include the prospective design, the objective measurement of atopic sensitization, and measures of exposure data over multiple assessments. The study has a very high cohort retention rate, although a lower proportion (70%) of the participants consented to skin prick testing at age 13 years. However, those who did not undergo skin prick test at age 13 years had a similar prevalence of atopic sensitization at age 32 years compared with those who did not. A limitation of this study is that the environmental exposures were self-reported rather than objectively measured, and not all were recorded at every age from birth to 13 years. There may also be other important environmental exposures that we have not recorded. For example, other household members may have smoked tobacco, resulting in childhood exposure, but we only assessed the parental status. However, these errors in exposure measurements would be most likely to bias to the null and may have led to underestimation of the attributable fractions. Although a strength of this study is that we used an objective measure of atopic sensitization, many of those with positive skin prick tests may not have symptoms of allergic disease. We also cannot eliminate the possibility that parents avoided certain exposures if their child was atopic or at risk for atopy. However, we have previously found that the prevalence of breastfeeding5 and pet ownership6 did not differ for children with a family history of atopy, making reverse causation an unlikely explanation for our findings. Smoking was less prevalent among parents with a history of atopy,7 however, and it is possible that this inflated the risk of sensitization associated with not being exposed to tobacco smoke. In conclusion, this study found that breastfeeding was associated with increased risk of atopic sensitization, whereas childhood exposure to tobacco smoke, cat and dog ownership, thumb-sucking and nail-biting, and household crowding, all appeared to protect against atopic sensitization. Within our cohort the combined effect of these environmental exposures (or lack thereof) may explain 58% of atopic sensitization at age 13 years. This effect persists into adult life and may explain 49% of atopic sensitization at age 32 years. Funding The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Research Unit was supported by the Health Research Council of New Zealand. H.S. was funded by a scholarship from the Otago Asthma Society. S.L. was funded by the Otago Medical Research Foundation-Kellier Charitable Trust Summer Scholarship. M.R.S. holds the AstraZeneca Chair in Respiratory Epidemiology, McMaster University. Acknowledgements We thank the study members, their friends and families for their continued support. We also thank Richie Poulton, the director of the study, Phil Silva, the study founder, and the unit staff. Conflict of interest: None of the authors has a conflict of interest with this manuscript. References 1 Law M , Morris JK , Wald N et al. Changes in atopy over a quarter of a century, based on cross sectional data at three time periods . BMJ 2005 ; 330: 1187 – 88 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 2 Asher MI , Montefort S , Björkstén B et al. Worldwide time trends in the prevalence of symptoms of asthma, allergic rhinoconjunctivitis, and eczema in childhood: ISAAC Phases One and Three repeat multicountry cross-sectional surveys . Lancet 2006 ; 368: 733 – 43 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 3 Eder W , Ege MJ , von Mutius E . The asthma epidemic . N Engl J Med 2006 ; 355: 2226 – 35 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 4 Subbarao P , Mandhane PJ , Sears MR . Asthma: epidemiology, etiology and risk factors . CMAJ 2009 ; 181 : E181 – 90 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 5 Sears MR , Greene JM , Willan AR et al. Long-term relation between breastfeeding and development of atopy and asthma in children and young adults: a longitudinal study . Lancet 2002 ; 360: 901 – 07 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 6 Mandhane PJ , Sears MR , Poulton R et al. Cats and dogs and the risk of atopy in childhood and adulthood . J Allergy Clin Immunol 2009 ; 124: 745 – 50 e4 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 7 Hancox RJ , Welch D , Poulton R et al. Cigarette smoking and allergic sensitization: a 32-year population-based cohort study . J Allergy Clin Immunol 2008 ; 121: 38 – 42 e3 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 8 Lynch SJ , Sears MR , Hancox RJ . Thumb-sucking, nail-biting, and atopic sensitization, asthma, and hay fever . Pediatrics 2016 ; 138: e20160443 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 9 Sears MR , Holdaway MD , Flannery EM et al. Parental and neonatal risk factors for atopy, airway hyperresponsiveness and asthma . Arch Dis Child 1996 ; 75: 392 – 98 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 10 Havstad SL , Johnson CC , Zoratti EM et al. Tobacco smoke exposure and allergic sensitization in children: A propensity score analysis . Respirology 2012 ; 17: 1068 – 72 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 11 Matheson MC , Erbas B , Balasuriya A et al. Breast-feeding and atopic disease: A cohort study from childhood to middle age . J Allergy Clin Immunol 2007 ; 120: 1051 – 57 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 12 Tariq SM , Matthews SM , Hakim EA et al. The prevalence of and risk factors for atopy in early childhood: A whole population birth cohort study . J Allergy Clin Immunol 1998 ; 101: 587 – 93 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 13 Kull I , Melen E , Alm J et al. Breast-feeding in relation to asthma, lung function, and sensitization in young schoolchildren . J Allergy Clin Immunol 2010 ; 125: 1013 – 19 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 14 Elliott L , Henderson J , Northstone K et al. Prospective study of breast-feeding in relation to wheeze, atopy, and bronchial hyperresponsiveness in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) . J Allergy Clin Immunol 2008 ; 122 : 49 – 54 , e1‐3 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 15 Kramer MS , Matush L , Vanilovich I et al. Effect of prolonged and exclusive breast feeding on risk of allergy and asthma: cluster randomised trial . BMJ 2007 ; 335: 815 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 16 Nagel G , Buchele G , Weinmayr G et al. Effect of breastfeeding on asthma, lung function and bronchial hyperreactivity in ISAAC Phase II . Eur Respir J 2009 ; 33: 993 – 1002 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 17 Ciaccio CE , DiDonna AC , Kennedy K et al. Association of tobacco smoke exposure and atopic sensitization . Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol 2013 ; 111: 387 – 90 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 18 Hansen K , Mangrio E , Lindstrom M et al. Early exposure to secondhand tobacco smoke and the development of allergic diseases in 4 year old children in Malmo, Sweden . BMC Pediatr 2010 ; 10: 61 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 19 Matheson MC , Allen KJ , Tang MLK . Understanding the evidence for and against the role of breastfeeding in allergy prevention . Clin Exp Allergy 2012 ; 42: 827 – 51 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 20 Strachan DP , Cook DG . Health effects of passive smoking. 5. Parental smoking and allergic sensitisation in children . Thorax 1998 ; 53: 117 – 23 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 21 Chen C-M , Tischer C , Schnappinger M et al. The role of cats and dogs in asthma and allergy – a systematic review . Int J Hyg Environ Health 2010 ; 213: 1 – 31 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 22 Lodge CJ , Lowe AJ , Gurrin LC et al. Pets at birth do not increase allergic disease in at-risk children . Clin Exp Allergy 2012 ; 42: 1377 – 85 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 23 Collin SM , Granell R , Westgarth C et al. Pet ownership is associated with increased risk of non-atopic asthma and reduced risk of atopy in childhood: findings from a UK birth cohort . Clin Exp Allergy 2015 ; 45: 200 – 10 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 24 Wickens K , Crane J , Pearce N et al. The magnitude of the effect of smaller family sizes on the increase in the prevalence of asthma and hay fever in the United Kingdom and New Zealand . J Allergy Clin Immunol 1999 ; 104: 554 – 58 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 25 Ball TM , Castro-Rodriguez JA , Griffith KA et al. Siblings, day care attendance, and the risk of asthma and wheezing during childhood . N Engl J Med 2000 ; 343: 538 – 43 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 26 Caudri D , Wijga A , Scholtens S et al. Early daycare is associated with an increase in airway symptoms in early childhood but is no protection against asthma or atopy at 8 years . Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2009 ; 180: 491 – 98 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 27 Cheng G , Smith AM , Levin L et al. Duration of day care attendance during infancy predicts asthma at the age of seven: the Cincinnati Childhood Allergy and Air Pollution Study . Clin Exp Allergy 2014 ; 44: 1274 – 81 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 28 Uekert SJ , Akan G , Evans MD et al. Sex-related differences in immune development and the expression of atopy in early childhood . J Allergy Clin Immunol 2006 ; 118: 1375 – 81 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 29 Ebert CS , Pillsbury HC . Epidemiology of allergy . Otolaryngol Clin North Am 2011 ; 44: 537 – 48 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 30 Upchurch S , Harris JM , Cullinan P . Temporal changes in UK birth order and the prevalence of atopy . Allergy 2010 ; 65: 1039 – 41 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 31 Poulton R , Moffitt T , Silva P . The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study: overview of the first 40 years, with an eye to the future . Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 2015 ; 50: 679 – 93 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 32 Hancox RJ , Milne BJ , Taylor DR et al. Relationship between socioeconomic status and asthma: a longitudinal cohort study . Thorax 2004 ; 59: 376 – 80 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 33 Greenland S , Drescher K . Maximum likelihood estimation of the attributable fraction from logistic models . Biometrics 1993 ; 49: 865 – 72 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 34 Rowe AK , Powell KE , Flanders WD . Why population attributable fractions can sum to more than one . Am J Prev Med 2004 ; 26: 243 – 49 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 35 Eide GE . Attributable fractions for partitioning risk and evaluating disease prevention: a practical guide . Clin Respir J 2008 ; 2 (Suppl 1): 92 – 103 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 36 Rückinger S , Von Kries R , Toschke AM . An illustration of and programs estimating attributable fractions in large scale surveys considering multiple risk factors . BMC Med Res Methodol 2009 ; 9: 7 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 37 Efron B . Nonparametric standard errors and confidence intervals . Can J Stat 1981 ; 9: 139 – 58 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat 38 Mistry P , Moles DR , Oneill J et al. The occlusal effects of digit sucking habits amongst school children in Northamptonshire UK . J Orthod 2010 ; 37: 87 – 92 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat 39 Leung AK , Robson WL . Nailbiting . Clin Pediatr 1990 ; 29: 690 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS WorldCat 40 Standl M , Sausenthaler S , Lattka E et al. FADS gene cluster modulates the effect of breastfeeding on asthma. Results from the GINIplus and LISAplus studies . Allergy 2012 ; 67: 83 – 90 . Google Scholar Crossref Search ADS PubMed WorldCat Author notes Hayden H Shin and Stephanie J Lynch contributed equally to this work. © The Author 2017; all rights reserved. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Epidemiological Association TI - How much atopy is attributable to common childhood environmental exposures? A population-based birth cohort study followed to adulthood JO - International Journal of Epidemiology DO - 10.1093/ije/dyx098 DA - 2017-12-01 UR - https://www.deepdyve.com/lp/oxford-university-press/how-much-atopy-is-attributable-to-common-childhood-environmental-2rlYMH7uDi SP - 2009 VL - 46 IS - 6 DP - DeepDyve ER -