Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 14-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

Prevalence of and Factors Related to Discordance About Prognosis Between Physicians and Surrogate Decision Makers of Critically Ill Patients

Prevalence of and Factors Related to Discordance About Prognosis Between Physicians and Surrogate... Abstract Importance Misperceptions about prognosis by individuals making decisions for incapacitated critically ill patients (surrogates) are common and often attributed to poor comprehension of medical information. Objective To determine the prevalence of and factors related to physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis in intensive care units (ICUs). Design, Setting, and Participants Mixed-methods study comprising quantitative surveys and qualitative interviews conducted in 4 ICUs at a major US medical center involving surrogate decision makers and physicians caring for patients at high risk of death from January 4, 2005, to July 10, 2009. Main Outcomes and Measures Discordance about prognosis, defined as a difference between a physician’s and a surrogate’s prognostic estimates of at least 20%; misunderstandings by surrogates (defined as any difference between a physician’s prognostic estimate and a surrogate’s best guess of that estimate); differences in belief (any difference between a surrogate’s actual estimate and their best guess of the physician’s estimate). Results Two hundred twenty-nine surrogate decision makers (median age, 47 [interquartile range {IQR}, 35-56] years; 68% women) and 99 physicians were involved in the care of 174 critically ill patients (median age, 60 [IQR, 47-74] years; 44% women). Physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis occurred in 122 of 229 instances (53%; 95% CI, 46.8%-59.7%). In 65 instances (28%), discordance was related to both misunderstandings by surrogates and differences in belief about the patient’s prognosis; 38 (17%) were related to misunderstandings by surrogates only; 7 (3%) were related to differences in belief only; and data were missing for 12. Seventy-five patients (43%) died. Surrogates’ prognostic estimates were much more accurate than chance alone, but physicians’ prognostic estimates were statistically significantly more accurate than surrogates’ (C statistic, 0.83 vs 0.74; absolute difference, 0.094; 95% CI, 0.024-0.163; P = .008). Among 71 surrogates interviewed who had beliefs about the prognosis that were more optimistic than that of the physician, the most common reasons for optimism were a need to maintain hope to benefit the patient (n = 34), a belief that the patient had unique strengths unknown to the physician (n = 24), and religious belief (n = 19). Conclusions and Relevance Among critically ill patients, discordant expectations about prognosis were common between patients’ physicians and surrogate decision makers and were related to misunderstandings by surrogates about physicians’ assessments of patients’ prognoses and differences in beliefs about patients’ prognoses. Introduction Quiz Ref IDIn 2010, it was estimated that nearly half of US adults near the end of life were unable to make decisions for themselves about whether to accept life-prolonging technologies.1 Family members or other individuals are asked to serve as surrogate decision makers for these often difficult decisions. To effectively function in this role, surrogates require a clear understanding of the likely outcomes of treatment. Numerous studies over the last 3 decades indicate that surrogates of patients with advanced illness often have optimistic expectations about prognosis.2-4 This is problematic because optimistic expectations are associated with more use of invasive treatments in dying patients and delayed integration of palliative care.5-7 Clinicians cite unrealistic expectations by surrogates as one of the most important barriers to high-quality end-of-life care in seriously ill patients.8 Quiz Ref IDAlthough misperceptions about prognosis by surrogates are well documented, an important barrier to progress is the lack of a clear understanding of the causes of these misperceptions. The conventional assumption is that surrogates’ inaccurate expectations arise from misunderstandings of physicians’ assessments of prognosis.3,9 However, insights from decision psychology suggest that other mechanisms may plausibly influence individuals’ risk perceptions, such as optimism biases and alternative value systems that lead surrogates to have beliefs about prognosis that differ from physicians’. Several small qualitative and simulation-based studies among surrogates in intensive care units (ICUs) suggest that such considerations may contribute to physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis,7,10-12 but these hypotheses have not been empirically validated in surrogates making actual decisions for incapacitated patients. Without clarity about factors that contribute to discordance, it will be difficult to design interventions targeting the key barriers to effective conversations about prognosis. We therefore sought to quantitatively determine the prevalence of physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis and to qualitatively understand reasons that surrogates sometimes hold beliefs about prognosis that differ from physicians’ assessments. Methods We conducted a study combining quantitative measurement of physicians’ and surrogates’ perceptions of prognosis with qualitative interviews (“mixed methods”) to explore what factors were related to surrogates’ perceptions of patient prognosis. We enrolled surrogate decision makers for incapacitated patients at high risk of death in 4 ICUs at the University of California, San Francisco Medical Center from January 4, 2005, to July 10, 2009. The ICUs included 2 medical-surgical ICUs, a neurological ICU, and a cardiac ICU. Patients were eligible if they were aged 18 years or older, lacked decision-making capacity on day 5 of their ICU stay, had respiratory failure requiring mechanical ventilation, and had an Acute Physiology and Chronic Health Evaluation (APACHE) II score of 25 or higher, which predicts a roughly 40% risk of in-hospital mortality.13 Patients were excluded if they were awaiting organ transplantation or if they lacked a surrogate decision maker. After obtaining permission from patients’ attending physicians, research staff approached the patients’ surrogates about study participation. If the responsibility for surrogate decision making was shared between multiple individuals, we enrolled those who self-reported having a significant amount of responsibility for decision making. Thus, more than 1 surrogate per patient could be enrolled. All participating surrogates and physicians provided written informed consent; surrogates provided proxy written consent for incapacitated patients. The institutional review board approved all study procedures. Study Procedures On a patient’s fifth day of receiving mechanical ventilation, physicians estimated the likelihood that the patient would survive the hospitalization using a previously validated question and a 0% to 100% scale: “What do you think are the chances that the patient will survive this hospitalization if the current plan of care stays the same?”10,14,15 Within 1 hour, surrogates independently answered the same question on the same 0% to 100% probability scale, blinded to the physician’s response. The study did not mandate or facilitate physician-surrogate conversations about prognosis prior to asking these questions, but the near simultaneity of measures minimized the possibility that changes in a patient’s condition could contribute to differences in physician-surrogate estimates. Surrogates were also asked to record what they perceived to be the physician’s assessment of prognosis with the question “If you had to guess, what do you think the doctor thinks is the chance that your loved one will survive this hospitalization if the current plan of care stays the same?” A standard probability scale was used to record participants’ prognostic estimates, with the anchors comprising simple expressions of risk (“0% chance of survival”; “100% chance of survival”) to minimize the chance of response errors among study participants with limited numeracy (Figure 1).16-18 Race and ethnicity information was collected by self-report from participants. The eAppendix in the Supplement contains a description of other covariates included in the questionnaire. Semistructured Interviews An interviewer blinded to physicians’ prognostic estimates conducted one-on-one, semistructured interviews with surrogates immediately after they completed the questionnaire. The interviewer showed each participant his/her responses to the 2 questions about prognosis (ie, “What do you think are the chances that your loved one will survive …?” and “What do you think the doctor thinks are the chances that the patient will survive …?”). Next, the interviewer read the following standardized prompt: “Please take a moment to look at your responses. I notice that what you think is the patient’s prognosis [differs from/is the same as] what you think the physician thinks is the prognosis. Can you tell me a little bit about this?” The interviewer used techniques of cognitive interviewing19 and standardized probes to pursue and clarify emergent themes as they arose. The content of the semistructured interview was expanded over time to pursue and clarify themes that emerged in prior interviews.19-21 Quantitative Analyses We defined physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis as at least a 20% difference between the physician’s and surrogate’s prognostic estimates based on a modified time–trade-off experiment in patients with serious illness in which patients’ willingness to receive ongoing life support declined substantially when their prognosis worsened by 20%.22 We categorized surrogates’ estimates as discordant pessimistic (prognostic estimate at least 20% lower than physician’s) or discordant optimistic (prognostic estimate at least 20% higher than physician’s) Among cases with physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis, we quantified the proportion due to misunderstandings by surrogates (any difference between the physician’s prognostic estimate and the surrogate’s best guess about that estimate) and the proportion due to differences in belief (any difference between the surrogate’s prognostic estimate and their best guess about the physician’s estimate) (Figure 1). We reviewed the medical record to determine hospital survival and compared the accuracy of physicians’ and surrogates’ prognostic estimates by comparing the area under the receiver operating characteristic curves generated using mixed-effects regression models with patients nested within surrogates within physicians, patient mortality as a dependent variable, surrogate and physician survival estimates as independent variables, and physician as a random intercept (see the eAppendix in the Supplement for additional details).23 We used Stata version 14.0 (Stata Corp) for all quantitative analyses and considered as statistically significant a 2-sided α<.05. We estimated that a sample size of 229 surrogates would be required to detect a prevalence of physician-surrogate discordance of 50% (based on the prevalence observed in a prior study in France2,24) with a confidence interval range (upper to lower bound) of less than 15%, assuming statistical power of 80% and a 2-sided α = .05. Qualitative Analyses A medical transcriptionist transcribed the audiorecorded interviews verbatim. We used constant comparative methods to inductively develop a framework to describe participants’ reasons for the congruence or incongruence of their prognostic estimates with what they believed to be the physician’s prognostic estimates. Constant comparative methods are a general method for inductively analyzing systematically gathered qualitative data. The method is most useful when existing conceptual frameworks for the topics under study are inadequate.19-21 To develop the preliminary coding scheme, 4 investigators independently coded a subset of transcripts line by line. As themes and concepts accumulated, we refined distinctions between concepts and grouped similar concepts into conceptual categories. These categories were developed further by comparing transcripts. Through a series of meetings, investigators arrived at consensus on the final coding framework (see the eAppendix in the Supplement for additional details). Two investigators blinded to participants’ demographic characteristics and each other’s work listened to the audiotapes, read the transcripts, and coded all interviews using the final coding framework. The mean κ statistic based on a random sample of 20% of interviews was 0.86 (range, 0.71-1.00), indicating excellent interrater reliability.25 All discrepancies between coders were reviewed and resolved through dialogue between the coders and the first author. To ensure coding validity,26,27 we used a multidisciplinary approach in developing the framework, which reduced the chance that individual bias threatened the validity of the findings, and we presented the preliminary findings to a sample of study participants for confirmation and/or modification, a process known as member checking. There was strong support for the framework among participants and no new themes were raised during the process of member checking. Results Patient and Participant Characteristics Of 222 eligible physician-patient dyads, 174 (79%) agreed to participate; 20 (9%) attending physicians and 27 (12%) families declined to participate. There were no important differences between enrolled and nonenrolled patients in demographic characteristics, admission diagnosis, or severity of illness. Forty patients had more than 1 surrogate (median number of surrogates, 1 [interquartile range {IQR}, 1-1; range, 1-5] per family). Restricting the analyses to 1 surrogate per patient did not change the findings; therefore, we report data from all surrogates. Demographic characteristics of the patients, surrogate decision makers, and physicians are shown in Table 1, Table 2, and Table 3, respectively. The sample was diverse in terms of sex, race, ethnicity, religious affiliation, and level of education. At enrollment, patients had a median APACHE II score of 28 (IQR, 26-32) and 75 patients (43%) died in the hospital. The median time between surrogate participation and patient death was 7 days (IQR, 3-21 days). A conversation about prognosis by day 5 of mechanical ventilation was reported by 183 surrogates (80%), 199 physicians (87%), and 213 (93%) of either. Physician-Surrogate Discordance Physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis occurred in 122 of 229 instances (53%; 95% CI, 46.8%-59.7%). Among the 229 surrogates participating in the study, 98 (43%) were more optimistic than physicians and 24 (10%) were more pessimistic. Sixty-five instances (28%) were related to a combination of misunderstandings by surrogates and differences in belief between the physician and surrogate about the patient’s prognosis; 38 (17%) were related to misunderstanding only; 7 (3%) were related to different beliefs; and data were missing for 12. Accuracy of Physicians’ and Surrogates’ Prognostic Estimates Surrogates’ estimates of prognosis were much more accurate than random guessing, but physicians’ estimates were statistically significantly more accurate than surrogates’ (C statistic, 0.83 vs 0.74, respectively; absolute difference, 0.094; 95% CI, 0.024-0.163; P = .008) (Figure 2). Surrogates’ Explanations for Optimism and Pessimism Seventy-one of 156 surrogates who participated in the semistructured interview were more optimistic than physicians. Three main themes emerged from the interviews about why surrogates held beliefs about a patient’s prognosis that were more optimistic than what they perceived to be the physician’s prognostic estimate (Table 4). First, some surrogates (n = 34) believed that maintaining optimism would improve the patient’s outcomes or protect themselves from emotional distress. One surrogate said “I really believe in good vibrations …. So I think if we put everything that is in our power to have good vibrations … we might be able to help him with that.” Another surrogate explained “I’m told to always think positive and so I’m kinda trying to make myself feel better.” Second, some surrogates (n = 24) expressed a belief that the patient has unique strengths unknown to the physician. For example, one surrogate of a 63-year-old man in respiratory failure explained “I know him personally. The doctors don’t know him personally, don’t know his will to live and his will power and how strong he is and his family history.” Third, some surrogates (n = 19) explained that their optimism was grounded in their religious beliefs. For example, one surrogate said “I really believe whether someone can live or not is up to God. If God wants someone to heal, even though the doctor thinks it’s impossible, I think someone can recover.” Surrogates cited several reasons for holding a more pessimistic belief about a patient’s prognosis than what they understood to be the physician’s estimate (Table 4). The most common reason was a belief that the physician was intrinsically optimistic. A second reason was the physician’s lack of knowledge of the patient’s unique attributes that negatively affected their prognosis, such as the patient’s weak physical baseline or loss of will to live. Additional reasons included a belief that the physician’s judgment was clouded by emotional investment in the patient and that the surrogate needed to be pessimistic to brace or prepare themselves for the possibility of the patient’s death. Discussion Quiz Ref IDAmong a diverse cohort of critically ill patients at high risk of death, we found a high prevalence of physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis. The discordance was associated with both misunderstandings by surrogates of physicians’ assessment of prognosis and by surrogates holding different beliefs about a patient’s prognosis compared with what they perceived to be the physician’s assessment of prognosis. The findings suggest that interventions are needed not only to improve the comprehensibility of prognostic information but also to attend to the emotional and psychological factors that influence surrogates’ prognostic expectations. Four empirical studies have addressed reasons for physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis in ICUs10-12,28 (eAppendix in the Supplement). Two small studies using hypothetical vignettes found that surrogates’ misperceptions about prognosis arose in part from optimistic biases rather than only from misunderstandings.10,12 Two studies using in-depth interviews of small numbers of surrogates found that surrogates’ religious beliefs, a need to maintain hope, and skepticism about physicians’ prognostic accuracy contributed to differences in beliefs about prognosis between physicians and surrogates.11,28 The limitations of these studies are that they involved small sample sizes and hypothetical cases and did not simultaneously provide quantitative estimates of misperceptions about prognosis and qualitative insights about the factors that contributed to those misperceptions. The present study overcomes these limitations to demonstrate that in actual cases in ICUs, both misunderstandings by surrogates of physicians’ prognostications as well as differences in belief about patients’ prognoses contribute to physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis. The pattern of our findings fits with insights from social psychology about biases in individuals’ risk perceptions. One such insight, termed the better-than-average effect, refers to the tendency of individuals to rate themselves as likely to have better outcomes than most others.29 Our results may in part be explained by a similar phenomenon. If so, our findings suggest that this bias is not limited to individuals predicting their own outcomes but also extends to individuals predicting outcomes of their loved ones. A noteworthy finding from our semistructured interviews is that some surrogates intentionally held overly optimistic prognostic estimates with the hope that doing so would improve the patient’s outcome. This phenomenon has previously been described as performative optimism, which is the belief that by thinking optimistic thoughts, an individual may influence their (or others’) likelihood of experiencing a positive outcome. Sulmasy et al30 found that 36 of 45 research participants explained their optimistic expectations regarding phase 1 trial participation in terms of believing that maintaining an optimistic attitude would improve their outcomes. Should clinicians and policy makers be concerned that a large proportion of surrogates in ICUs hold overly optimistic expectations about prognosis? In one view, it is encouraging that the accuracy of surrogates’ prognostic estimates was substantially better than chance alone and comparable with the accuracy of many accepted clinical risk prediction models. However, surrogates’ expectations differed substantially from physicians’, and physicians’ predictions were statistically significantly more accurate. This finding raises concerns that one of the fundamental assumptions of shared decision making—agreement between parties about the likely outcomes of treatment—is often not achieved.31 Another concern is that the prevalent optimism among surrogates may lead to overuse of expensive, life-prolonging treatments in dying patients and underuse of palliative treatments. This concern is supported by a series of studies indicating that individuals’ willingness to undergo intensive treatment near the end of life is strongly influenced by the likely outcomes. For example, among a cohort of seriously ill outpatients, Fried et al32 found that as the likelihood of successful treatment declined, so too did individuals’ willingness to undergo intensive treatment. Among a cohort of patients with advanced dementia, Mitchell et al5 found that patients with surrogates who had accurate expectations of the patients’ prognoses were less likely to receive invasive treatments near the end of life. Among parents of children with incurable cancer, Wolfe et al6 found that parents with accurate expectations about incurability of the cancer focused treatment more on lessening discomfort and integrating palliative care compared with those who had delayed recognition of the child’s poor prognosis. Quiz Ref IDThere are at least 2 clinical implications of our findings. First, given the high rates of discordance about prognosis, clinicians communicating with surrogates of patients with advanced critical illness should routinely check in with surrogates about their perceptions of prognosis prior to engaging in decision making about goals of care.Quiz Ref ID Second, when clinicians recognize that surrogates’ expectations about prognosis diverge from their own, they should explore the possibility that causes other than misunderstanding may be contributing, such as a belief that the patient is stronger than average, a belief that expressing optimism will improve the patient’s outcome, or a belief that religious rather than biomedical considerations will determine the patient’s outcome. This is important because interventions to reconcile discordance about prognosis may differ for misunderstandings compared with differences in belief. Our findings raise the possibility that standard informational interventions such as decision aids may be insufficient because they do not attend to the emotional and psychological sources of optimism that influence surrogates’ prognostic expectations. This study has several strengths. First, we used a mixed-methods approach, which allowed for both quantification of discordance and qualitative exploration of the reasons behind surrogates’ beliefs. We validated our qualitative findings through a process of member checking in which we returned the results to research participants for their review and comment. The study was conducted in a large and diverse cohort. Additionally, we interviewed actual surrogates facing actual decisions rather than using simulation, which established the clinical relevance of our findings. This study also has several limitations. Although we recruited a diverse cohort, our sample was limited to one region of the country, which may not account for some geographic or cultural factors. An inherent limitation of using interviews to understand what influences individuals is that it only allows for identification of beliefs of which individuals have conscious awareness and are willing to express, and we cannot exclude that processes surrogates were not conscious of or were unwilling to discuss may also have contributed. Conclusions Among critically ill patients receiving care in ICUs, discordant expectations about prognosis were common between patients’ physicians and surrogate decision makers and were related to both misunderstandings by surrogates about physicians’ assessments of patients’ prognoses and differences in beliefs about patients’ prognoses. Section Editor: Derek C. Angus, MD, MPH, Associate Editor, JAMA (angusdc@upmc.edu). Back to top Article Information Corresponding Author: Douglas B. White, MD, MAS, Program on Ethics and Decision Making in Critical Illness, Department of Critical Care Medicine, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, 3550 Terrace St, Scaife Hall, Room 608, HPU010604, Pittsburgh, PA 15261 (whitedb@upmc.edu). Author Contributions: Dr White had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: White, Weissfeld, Curtis, Lo. Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Ernecoff, Buddadhumaruk, Hong, Weissfeld, Luce, Lo. Drafting of the manuscript: White, Ernecoff, Hong, Luce. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: White, Ernecoff, Buddadhumaruk, Weissfeld, Curtis, Lo. Statistical analysis: White, Buddadhumaruk, Hong, Weissfeld. Administrative, technical, or material support: Luce. Conflict of Interest Disclosures: All authors have completed and submitted the ICMJE Form for Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest and none were reported. Funding/Support: This project was supported by National Institutes of Health grants KL2 RR024130 and R01 HL094553 and by the Greenwall Foundation (Dr White). Role of the Funders/Sponsors: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; or decision to submit the manuscript for publication. Additional Contributions: We thank Leslie Scheunemann, MD, MPH (University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine; Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania), for assistance in preparing the systematic review summarized in the Discussion section. We thank the following individuals for assistance with data collection and coding of interviews with surrogates: Elizabeth Boyd, PhD, University of California, Berkeley; Leah Evans-Wong, MD, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester; and Grave L. Malvar, MD, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts. No financial compensation was received. References 1. Silveira MJ, Kim SY, Langa KM. Advance directives and outcomes of surrogate decision making before death. N Engl J Med. 2010;362(13):1211-1218.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2. Azoulay E, Chevret S, Leleu G, et al. Half the families of intensive care unit patients experience inadequate communication with physicians. Crit Care Med. 2000;28(8):3044-3049.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3. Cox CE, Martinu T, Sathy SJ, et al. Expectations and outcomes of prolonged mechanical ventilation. Crit Care Med. 2009;37(11):2888-2894.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4. Teno JM, Fisher E, Hamel MB, et al. Decision-making and outcomes of prolonged ICU stays in seriously ill patients. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48(5)(suppl):S70-S74.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5. Mitchell SL, Teno JM, Kiely DK, et al. The clinical course of advanced dementia. N Engl J Med. 2009;361(16):1529-1538.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6. Wolfe J, Klar N, Grier HE, et al. Understanding of prognosis among parents of children who died of cancer: impact on treatment goals and integration of palliative care. JAMA. 2000;284(19):2469-2475.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7. Zier LS, Burack JH, Micco G, Chipman AK, Frank JA, White DB. Surrogate decision makers’ responses to physicians’ predictions of medical futility. Chest. 2009;136(1):110-117.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8. You JJ, Downar J, Fowler RA, et al; Canadian Researchers at the End of Life Network. Barriers to goals of care discussions with seriously ill hospitalized patients and their families: a multicenter survey of clinicians. JAMA Intern Med. 2015;175(4):549-556.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9. Mack JW, Cook EF, Wolfe J, Grier HE, Cleary PD, Weeks JC. Understanding of prognosis among parents of children with cancer: parental optimism and the parent-physician interaction. J Clin Oncol. 2007;25(11):1357-1362.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10. Lee Char SJ, Evans LR, Malvar GL, White DB. A randomized trial of two methods to disclose prognosis to surrogate decision makers in intensive care units. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2010;182(7):905-909.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11. Schenker Y, White DB, Crowley-Matoka M, Dohan D, Tiver GA, Arnold RM. “It hurts to know … and it helps”: exploring how surrogates in the ICU cope with prognostic information. J Palliat Med. 2013;16(3):243-249.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12. Zier LS, Sottile PD, Hong SY, Weissfield LA, White DB. Surrogate decision makers’ interpretation of prognostic information: a mixed-methods study. Ann Intern Med. 2012;156(5):360-366.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13. Donnino MW, Salciccioli JD, Dejam A, et al. APACHE II scoring to predict outcome in post-cardiac arrest. Resuscitation. 2013;84(5):651-656.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14. SUPPORT Principal Investigators. A controlled trial to improve care for seriously ill hospitalized patients: the Study to Understand Prognoses and Preferences for Outcomes and Risks of Treatments (SUPPORT). JAMA. 1995;274(20):1591-1598.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15. Weeks JC, Cook EF, O’Day SJ, et al. Relationship between cancer patients’ predictions of prognosis and their treatment preferences. JAMA. 1998;279(21):1709-1714.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16. Gigerenzer G, Edwards A. Simple tools for understanding risks: from innumeracy to insight. BMJ. 2003;327(7417):741-744.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17. Holland JC. Now we tell—but how well? J Clin Oncol. 1989;7(5):557-559.PubMedGoogle Scholar 18. Huskisson EC. Measurement of pain. Lancet. 1974;2(7889):1127-1131.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19. Strauss AL, Corbin J. Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and Procedures for Developing Grounded Theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1998. 20. Glaser BG, Strauss AL. Discovery of Grounded Theory. Chicago, IL: Adline Publishing; 1967. 21. Charmaz K. Constructing Grounded Theory: A Practical Guide Through Qualitative Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2006. 22. Lloyd CB, Nietert PJ, Silvestri GA. Intensive care decision making in the seriously ill and elderly. Crit Care Med. 2004;32(3):649-654.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23. DeLong ER, DeLong DM, Clarke-Pearson DL. Comparing the areas under two or more correlated receiver operating characteristic curves: a nonparametric approach. Biometrics. 1988;44(3):837-845.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24. Agresti A. Categorical Data Analysis. 3rd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2013. 25. Sackett DL, Guyatt GH, Tugwell P. Clinical Epidemiology: A Basic Science for Clinical Medicine. 2nd ed. Boston, MA: Little Brown & Co; 1991. 26. Patton MQ. Enhancing the quality and credibility of qualitative analysis. Health Serv Res. 1999;34(5 Pt 2):1189-1208.PubMedGoogle Scholar 27. Giacomini MK, Cook DJ. Users’ Guides to the Medical Literature, XXIII: qualitative research in health care: are the results of the study valid? JAMA. 2000;284(3):357-362.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28. Zier LS, Burack JH, Micco G, et al. Doubt and belief in physicians’ ability to prognosticate during critical illness: the perspective of surrogate decision makers. Crit Care Med. 2008;36(8):2341-2347.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29. Alicke MD, Klotz ML, Breitenbecher DL, Yurak TJ, Vredenburg DS. Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;68(5):804.Google ScholarCrossref 30. Sulmasy DP, Astrow AB, He MK, et al. The culture of faith and hope: patients’ justifications for their high estimations of expected therapeutic benefit when enrolling in early phase oncology trials. Cancer. 2010;116(15):3702-3711.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31. Kon AA, Davidson JE, Morrison W, Danis M, White DB; American College of Critical Care Medicine; American Thoracic Society. Shared decision making in ICUs: an American College of Critical Care Medicine and American Thoracic Society policy statement. Crit Care Med. 2016;44(1):188-201.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32. Fried TR, Bradley EH, Towle VR, Allore H. Understanding the treatment preferences of seriously ill patients. N Engl J Med. 2002;346(14):1061-1066.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png JAMA American Medical Association

Prevalence of and Factors Related to Discordance About Prognosis Between Physicians and Surrogate Decision Makers of Critically Ill Patients

Loading next page...
 
/lp/american-medical-association/prevalence-of-and-factors-related-to-discordance-about-prognosis-rRHp2oG8Pv

References (37)

Publisher
American Medical Association
Copyright
Copyright © 2016 American Medical Association. All Rights Reserved.
ISSN
0098-7484
eISSN
1538-3598
DOI
10.1001/jama.2016.5351
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

Abstract Importance Misperceptions about prognosis by individuals making decisions for incapacitated critically ill patients (surrogates) are common and often attributed to poor comprehension of medical information. Objective To determine the prevalence of and factors related to physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis in intensive care units (ICUs). Design, Setting, and Participants Mixed-methods study comprising quantitative surveys and qualitative interviews conducted in 4 ICUs at a major US medical center involving surrogate decision makers and physicians caring for patients at high risk of death from January 4, 2005, to July 10, 2009. Main Outcomes and Measures Discordance about prognosis, defined as a difference between a physician’s and a surrogate’s prognostic estimates of at least 20%; misunderstandings by surrogates (defined as any difference between a physician’s prognostic estimate and a surrogate’s best guess of that estimate); differences in belief (any difference between a surrogate’s actual estimate and their best guess of the physician’s estimate). Results Two hundred twenty-nine surrogate decision makers (median age, 47 [interquartile range {IQR}, 35-56] years; 68% women) and 99 physicians were involved in the care of 174 critically ill patients (median age, 60 [IQR, 47-74] years; 44% women). Physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis occurred in 122 of 229 instances (53%; 95% CI, 46.8%-59.7%). In 65 instances (28%), discordance was related to both misunderstandings by surrogates and differences in belief about the patient’s prognosis; 38 (17%) were related to misunderstandings by surrogates only; 7 (3%) were related to differences in belief only; and data were missing for 12. Seventy-five patients (43%) died. Surrogates’ prognostic estimates were much more accurate than chance alone, but physicians’ prognostic estimates were statistically significantly more accurate than surrogates’ (C statistic, 0.83 vs 0.74; absolute difference, 0.094; 95% CI, 0.024-0.163; P = .008). Among 71 surrogates interviewed who had beliefs about the prognosis that were more optimistic than that of the physician, the most common reasons for optimism were a need to maintain hope to benefit the patient (n = 34), a belief that the patient had unique strengths unknown to the physician (n = 24), and religious belief (n = 19). Conclusions and Relevance Among critically ill patients, discordant expectations about prognosis were common between patients’ physicians and surrogate decision makers and were related to misunderstandings by surrogates about physicians’ assessments of patients’ prognoses and differences in beliefs about patients’ prognoses. Introduction Quiz Ref IDIn 2010, it was estimated that nearly half of US adults near the end of life were unable to make decisions for themselves about whether to accept life-prolonging technologies.1 Family members or other individuals are asked to serve as surrogate decision makers for these often difficult decisions. To effectively function in this role, surrogates require a clear understanding of the likely outcomes of treatment. Numerous studies over the last 3 decades indicate that surrogates of patients with advanced illness often have optimistic expectations about prognosis.2-4 This is problematic because optimistic expectations are associated with more use of invasive treatments in dying patients and delayed integration of palliative care.5-7 Clinicians cite unrealistic expectations by surrogates as one of the most important barriers to high-quality end-of-life care in seriously ill patients.8 Quiz Ref IDAlthough misperceptions about prognosis by surrogates are well documented, an important barrier to progress is the lack of a clear understanding of the causes of these misperceptions. The conventional assumption is that surrogates’ inaccurate expectations arise from misunderstandings of physicians’ assessments of prognosis.3,9 However, insights from decision psychology suggest that other mechanisms may plausibly influence individuals’ risk perceptions, such as optimism biases and alternative value systems that lead surrogates to have beliefs about prognosis that differ from physicians’. Several small qualitative and simulation-based studies among surrogates in intensive care units (ICUs) suggest that such considerations may contribute to physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis,7,10-12 but these hypotheses have not been empirically validated in surrogates making actual decisions for incapacitated patients. Without clarity about factors that contribute to discordance, it will be difficult to design interventions targeting the key barriers to effective conversations about prognosis. We therefore sought to quantitatively determine the prevalence of physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis and to qualitatively understand reasons that surrogates sometimes hold beliefs about prognosis that differ from physicians’ assessments. Methods We conducted a study combining quantitative measurement of physicians’ and surrogates’ perceptions of prognosis with qualitative interviews (“mixed methods”) to explore what factors were related to surrogates’ perceptions of patient prognosis. We enrolled surrogate decision makers for incapacitated patients at high risk of death in 4 ICUs at the University of California, San Francisco Medical Center from January 4, 2005, to July 10, 2009. The ICUs included 2 medical-surgical ICUs, a neurological ICU, and a cardiac ICU. Patients were eligible if they were aged 18 years or older, lacked decision-making capacity on day 5 of their ICU stay, had respiratory failure requiring mechanical ventilation, and had an Acute Physiology and Chronic Health Evaluation (APACHE) II score of 25 or higher, which predicts a roughly 40% risk of in-hospital mortality.13 Patients were excluded if they were awaiting organ transplantation or if they lacked a surrogate decision maker. After obtaining permission from patients’ attending physicians, research staff approached the patients’ surrogates about study participation. If the responsibility for surrogate decision making was shared between multiple individuals, we enrolled those who self-reported having a significant amount of responsibility for decision making. Thus, more than 1 surrogate per patient could be enrolled. All participating surrogates and physicians provided written informed consent; surrogates provided proxy written consent for incapacitated patients. The institutional review board approved all study procedures. Study Procedures On a patient’s fifth day of receiving mechanical ventilation, physicians estimated the likelihood that the patient would survive the hospitalization using a previously validated question and a 0% to 100% scale: “What do you think are the chances that the patient will survive this hospitalization if the current plan of care stays the same?”10,14,15 Within 1 hour, surrogates independently answered the same question on the same 0% to 100% probability scale, blinded to the physician’s response. The study did not mandate or facilitate physician-surrogate conversations about prognosis prior to asking these questions, but the near simultaneity of measures minimized the possibility that changes in a patient’s condition could contribute to differences in physician-surrogate estimates. Surrogates were also asked to record what they perceived to be the physician’s assessment of prognosis with the question “If you had to guess, what do you think the doctor thinks is the chance that your loved one will survive this hospitalization if the current plan of care stays the same?” A standard probability scale was used to record participants’ prognostic estimates, with the anchors comprising simple expressions of risk (“0% chance of survival”; “100% chance of survival”) to minimize the chance of response errors among study participants with limited numeracy (Figure 1).16-18 Race and ethnicity information was collected by self-report from participants. The eAppendix in the Supplement contains a description of other covariates included in the questionnaire. Semistructured Interviews An interviewer blinded to physicians’ prognostic estimates conducted one-on-one, semistructured interviews with surrogates immediately after they completed the questionnaire. The interviewer showed each participant his/her responses to the 2 questions about prognosis (ie, “What do you think are the chances that your loved one will survive …?” and “What do you think the doctor thinks are the chances that the patient will survive …?”). Next, the interviewer read the following standardized prompt: “Please take a moment to look at your responses. I notice that what you think is the patient’s prognosis [differs from/is the same as] what you think the physician thinks is the prognosis. Can you tell me a little bit about this?” The interviewer used techniques of cognitive interviewing19 and standardized probes to pursue and clarify emergent themes as they arose. The content of the semistructured interview was expanded over time to pursue and clarify themes that emerged in prior interviews.19-21 Quantitative Analyses We defined physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis as at least a 20% difference between the physician’s and surrogate’s prognostic estimates based on a modified time–trade-off experiment in patients with serious illness in which patients’ willingness to receive ongoing life support declined substantially when their prognosis worsened by 20%.22 We categorized surrogates’ estimates as discordant pessimistic (prognostic estimate at least 20% lower than physician’s) or discordant optimistic (prognostic estimate at least 20% higher than physician’s) Among cases with physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis, we quantified the proportion due to misunderstandings by surrogates (any difference between the physician’s prognostic estimate and the surrogate’s best guess about that estimate) and the proportion due to differences in belief (any difference between the surrogate’s prognostic estimate and their best guess about the physician’s estimate) (Figure 1). We reviewed the medical record to determine hospital survival and compared the accuracy of physicians’ and surrogates’ prognostic estimates by comparing the area under the receiver operating characteristic curves generated using mixed-effects regression models with patients nested within surrogates within physicians, patient mortality as a dependent variable, surrogate and physician survival estimates as independent variables, and physician as a random intercept (see the eAppendix in the Supplement for additional details).23 We used Stata version 14.0 (Stata Corp) for all quantitative analyses and considered as statistically significant a 2-sided α<.05. We estimated that a sample size of 229 surrogates would be required to detect a prevalence of physician-surrogate discordance of 50% (based on the prevalence observed in a prior study in France2,24) with a confidence interval range (upper to lower bound) of less than 15%, assuming statistical power of 80% and a 2-sided α = .05. Qualitative Analyses A medical transcriptionist transcribed the audiorecorded interviews verbatim. We used constant comparative methods to inductively develop a framework to describe participants’ reasons for the congruence or incongruence of their prognostic estimates with what they believed to be the physician’s prognostic estimates. Constant comparative methods are a general method for inductively analyzing systematically gathered qualitative data. The method is most useful when existing conceptual frameworks for the topics under study are inadequate.19-21 To develop the preliminary coding scheme, 4 investigators independently coded a subset of transcripts line by line. As themes and concepts accumulated, we refined distinctions between concepts and grouped similar concepts into conceptual categories. These categories were developed further by comparing transcripts. Through a series of meetings, investigators arrived at consensus on the final coding framework (see the eAppendix in the Supplement for additional details). Two investigators blinded to participants’ demographic characteristics and each other’s work listened to the audiotapes, read the transcripts, and coded all interviews using the final coding framework. The mean κ statistic based on a random sample of 20% of interviews was 0.86 (range, 0.71-1.00), indicating excellent interrater reliability.25 All discrepancies between coders were reviewed and resolved through dialogue between the coders and the first author. To ensure coding validity,26,27 we used a multidisciplinary approach in developing the framework, which reduced the chance that individual bias threatened the validity of the findings, and we presented the preliminary findings to a sample of study participants for confirmation and/or modification, a process known as member checking. There was strong support for the framework among participants and no new themes were raised during the process of member checking. Results Patient and Participant Characteristics Of 222 eligible physician-patient dyads, 174 (79%) agreed to participate; 20 (9%) attending physicians and 27 (12%) families declined to participate. There were no important differences between enrolled and nonenrolled patients in demographic characteristics, admission diagnosis, or severity of illness. Forty patients had more than 1 surrogate (median number of surrogates, 1 [interquartile range {IQR}, 1-1; range, 1-5] per family). Restricting the analyses to 1 surrogate per patient did not change the findings; therefore, we report data from all surrogates. Demographic characteristics of the patients, surrogate decision makers, and physicians are shown in Table 1, Table 2, and Table 3, respectively. The sample was diverse in terms of sex, race, ethnicity, religious affiliation, and level of education. At enrollment, patients had a median APACHE II score of 28 (IQR, 26-32) and 75 patients (43%) died in the hospital. The median time between surrogate participation and patient death was 7 days (IQR, 3-21 days). A conversation about prognosis by day 5 of mechanical ventilation was reported by 183 surrogates (80%), 199 physicians (87%), and 213 (93%) of either. Physician-Surrogate Discordance Physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis occurred in 122 of 229 instances (53%; 95% CI, 46.8%-59.7%). Among the 229 surrogates participating in the study, 98 (43%) were more optimistic than physicians and 24 (10%) were more pessimistic. Sixty-five instances (28%) were related to a combination of misunderstandings by surrogates and differences in belief between the physician and surrogate about the patient’s prognosis; 38 (17%) were related to misunderstanding only; 7 (3%) were related to different beliefs; and data were missing for 12. Accuracy of Physicians’ and Surrogates’ Prognostic Estimates Surrogates’ estimates of prognosis were much more accurate than random guessing, but physicians’ estimates were statistically significantly more accurate than surrogates’ (C statistic, 0.83 vs 0.74, respectively; absolute difference, 0.094; 95% CI, 0.024-0.163; P = .008) (Figure 2). Surrogates’ Explanations for Optimism and Pessimism Seventy-one of 156 surrogates who participated in the semistructured interview were more optimistic than physicians. Three main themes emerged from the interviews about why surrogates held beliefs about a patient’s prognosis that were more optimistic than what they perceived to be the physician’s prognostic estimate (Table 4). First, some surrogates (n = 34) believed that maintaining optimism would improve the patient’s outcomes or protect themselves from emotional distress. One surrogate said “I really believe in good vibrations …. So I think if we put everything that is in our power to have good vibrations … we might be able to help him with that.” Another surrogate explained “I’m told to always think positive and so I’m kinda trying to make myself feel better.” Second, some surrogates (n = 24) expressed a belief that the patient has unique strengths unknown to the physician. For example, one surrogate of a 63-year-old man in respiratory failure explained “I know him personally. The doctors don’t know him personally, don’t know his will to live and his will power and how strong he is and his family history.” Third, some surrogates (n = 19) explained that their optimism was grounded in their religious beliefs. For example, one surrogate said “I really believe whether someone can live or not is up to God. If God wants someone to heal, even though the doctor thinks it’s impossible, I think someone can recover.” Surrogates cited several reasons for holding a more pessimistic belief about a patient’s prognosis than what they understood to be the physician’s estimate (Table 4). The most common reason was a belief that the physician was intrinsically optimistic. A second reason was the physician’s lack of knowledge of the patient’s unique attributes that negatively affected their prognosis, such as the patient’s weak physical baseline or loss of will to live. Additional reasons included a belief that the physician’s judgment was clouded by emotional investment in the patient and that the surrogate needed to be pessimistic to brace or prepare themselves for the possibility of the patient’s death. Discussion Quiz Ref IDAmong a diverse cohort of critically ill patients at high risk of death, we found a high prevalence of physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis. The discordance was associated with both misunderstandings by surrogates of physicians’ assessment of prognosis and by surrogates holding different beliefs about a patient’s prognosis compared with what they perceived to be the physician’s assessment of prognosis. The findings suggest that interventions are needed not only to improve the comprehensibility of prognostic information but also to attend to the emotional and psychological factors that influence surrogates’ prognostic expectations. Four empirical studies have addressed reasons for physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis in ICUs10-12,28 (eAppendix in the Supplement). Two small studies using hypothetical vignettes found that surrogates’ misperceptions about prognosis arose in part from optimistic biases rather than only from misunderstandings.10,12 Two studies using in-depth interviews of small numbers of surrogates found that surrogates’ religious beliefs, a need to maintain hope, and skepticism about physicians’ prognostic accuracy contributed to differences in beliefs about prognosis between physicians and surrogates.11,28 The limitations of these studies are that they involved small sample sizes and hypothetical cases and did not simultaneously provide quantitative estimates of misperceptions about prognosis and qualitative insights about the factors that contributed to those misperceptions. The present study overcomes these limitations to demonstrate that in actual cases in ICUs, both misunderstandings by surrogates of physicians’ prognostications as well as differences in belief about patients’ prognoses contribute to physician-surrogate discordance about prognosis. The pattern of our findings fits with insights from social psychology about biases in individuals’ risk perceptions. One such insight, termed the better-than-average effect, refers to the tendency of individuals to rate themselves as likely to have better outcomes than most others.29 Our results may in part be explained by a similar phenomenon. If so, our findings suggest that this bias is not limited to individuals predicting their own outcomes but also extends to individuals predicting outcomes of their loved ones. A noteworthy finding from our semistructured interviews is that some surrogates intentionally held overly optimistic prognostic estimates with the hope that doing so would improve the patient’s outcome. This phenomenon has previously been described as performative optimism, which is the belief that by thinking optimistic thoughts, an individual may influence their (or others’) likelihood of experiencing a positive outcome. Sulmasy et al30 found that 36 of 45 research participants explained their optimistic expectations regarding phase 1 trial participation in terms of believing that maintaining an optimistic attitude would improve their outcomes. Should clinicians and policy makers be concerned that a large proportion of surrogates in ICUs hold overly optimistic expectations about prognosis? In one view, it is encouraging that the accuracy of surrogates’ prognostic estimates was substantially better than chance alone and comparable with the accuracy of many accepted clinical risk prediction models. However, surrogates’ expectations differed substantially from physicians’, and physicians’ predictions were statistically significantly more accurate. This finding raises concerns that one of the fundamental assumptions of shared decision making—agreement between parties about the likely outcomes of treatment—is often not achieved.31 Another concern is that the prevalent optimism among surrogates may lead to overuse of expensive, life-prolonging treatments in dying patients and underuse of palliative treatments. This concern is supported by a series of studies indicating that individuals’ willingness to undergo intensive treatment near the end of life is strongly influenced by the likely outcomes. For example, among a cohort of seriously ill outpatients, Fried et al32 found that as the likelihood of successful treatment declined, so too did individuals’ willingness to undergo intensive treatment. Among a cohort of patients with advanced dementia, Mitchell et al5 found that patients with surrogates who had accurate expectations of the patients’ prognoses were less likely to receive invasive treatments near the end of life. Among parents of children with incurable cancer, Wolfe et al6 found that parents with accurate expectations about incurability of the cancer focused treatment more on lessening discomfort and integrating palliative care compared with those who had delayed recognition of the child’s poor prognosis. Quiz Ref IDThere are at least 2 clinical implications of our findings. First, given the high rates of discordance about prognosis, clinicians communicating with surrogates of patients with advanced critical illness should routinely check in with surrogates about their perceptions of prognosis prior to engaging in decision making about goals of care.Quiz Ref ID Second, when clinicians recognize that surrogates’ expectations about prognosis diverge from their own, they should explore the possibility that causes other than misunderstanding may be contributing, such as a belief that the patient is stronger than average, a belief that expressing optimism will improve the patient’s outcome, or a belief that religious rather than biomedical considerations will determine the patient’s outcome. This is important because interventions to reconcile discordance about prognosis may differ for misunderstandings compared with differences in belief. Our findings raise the possibility that standard informational interventions such as decision aids may be insufficient because they do not attend to the emotional and psychological sources of optimism that influence surrogates’ prognostic expectations. This study has several strengths. First, we used a mixed-methods approach, which allowed for both quantification of discordance and qualitative exploration of the reasons behind surrogates’ beliefs. We validated our qualitative findings through a process of member checking in which we returned the results to research participants for their review and comment. The study was conducted in a large and diverse cohort. Additionally, we interviewed actual surrogates facing actual decisions rather than using simulation, which established the clinical relevance of our findings. This study also has several limitations. Although we recruited a diverse cohort, our sample was limited to one region of the country, which may not account for some geographic or cultural factors. An inherent limitation of using interviews to understand what influences individuals is that it only allows for identification of beliefs of which individuals have conscious awareness and are willing to express, and we cannot exclude that processes surrogates were not conscious of or were unwilling to discuss may also have contributed. Conclusions Among critically ill patients receiving care in ICUs, discordant expectations about prognosis were common between patients’ physicians and surrogate decision makers and were related to both misunderstandings by surrogates about physicians’ assessments of patients’ prognoses and differences in beliefs about patients’ prognoses. Section Editor: Derek C. Angus, MD, MPH, Associate Editor, JAMA (angusdc@upmc.edu). Back to top Article Information Corresponding Author: Douglas B. White, MD, MAS, Program on Ethics and Decision Making in Critical Illness, Department of Critical Care Medicine, University of Pittsburgh Medical Center, 3550 Terrace St, Scaife Hall, Room 608, HPU010604, Pittsburgh, PA 15261 (whitedb@upmc.edu). Author Contributions: Dr White had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Study concept and design: White, Weissfeld, Curtis, Lo. Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Ernecoff, Buddadhumaruk, Hong, Weissfeld, Luce, Lo. Drafting of the manuscript: White, Ernecoff, Hong, Luce. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: White, Ernecoff, Buddadhumaruk, Weissfeld, Curtis, Lo. Statistical analysis: White, Buddadhumaruk, Hong, Weissfeld. Administrative, technical, or material support: Luce. Conflict of Interest Disclosures: All authors have completed and submitted the ICMJE Form for Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest and none were reported. Funding/Support: This project was supported by National Institutes of Health grants KL2 RR024130 and R01 HL094553 and by the Greenwall Foundation (Dr White). Role of the Funders/Sponsors: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; or decision to submit the manuscript for publication. Additional Contributions: We thank Leslie Scheunemann, MD, MPH (University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine; Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania), for assistance in preparing the systematic review summarized in the Discussion section. We thank the following individuals for assistance with data collection and coding of interviews with surrogates: Elizabeth Boyd, PhD, University of California, Berkeley; Leah Evans-Wong, MD, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester; and Grave L. Malvar, MD, Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center and Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts. No financial compensation was received. References 1. Silveira MJ, Kim SY, Langa KM. Advance directives and outcomes of surrogate decision making before death. N Engl J Med. 2010;362(13):1211-1218.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2. Azoulay E, Chevret S, Leleu G, et al. Half the families of intensive care unit patients experience inadequate communication with physicians. Crit Care Med. 2000;28(8):3044-3049.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3. Cox CE, Martinu T, Sathy SJ, et al. Expectations and outcomes of prolonged mechanical ventilation. Crit Care Med. 2009;37(11):2888-2894.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4. Teno JM, Fisher E, Hamel MB, et al. Decision-making and outcomes of prolonged ICU stays in seriously ill patients. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2000;48(5)(suppl):S70-S74.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5. Mitchell SL, Teno JM, Kiely DK, et al. The clinical course of advanced dementia. N Engl J Med. 2009;361(16):1529-1538.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6. Wolfe J, Klar N, Grier HE, et al. Understanding of prognosis among parents of children who died of cancer: impact on treatment goals and integration of palliative care. JAMA. 2000;284(19):2469-2475.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7. Zier LS, Burack JH, Micco G, Chipman AK, Frank JA, White DB. Surrogate decision makers’ responses to physicians’ predictions of medical futility. Chest. 2009;136(1):110-117.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8. You JJ, Downar J, Fowler RA, et al; Canadian Researchers at the End of Life Network. Barriers to goals of care discussions with seriously ill hospitalized patients and their families: a multicenter survey of clinicians. JAMA Intern Med. 2015;175(4):549-556.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9. Mack JW, Cook EF, Wolfe J, Grier HE, Cleary PD, Weeks JC. Understanding of prognosis among parents of children with cancer: parental optimism and the parent-physician interaction. J Clin Oncol. 2007;25(11):1357-1362.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10. Lee Char SJ, Evans LR, Malvar GL, White DB. A randomized trial of two methods to disclose prognosis to surrogate decision makers in intensive care units. Am J Respir Crit Care Med. 2010;182(7):905-909.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11. Schenker Y, White DB, Crowley-Matoka M, Dohan D, Tiver GA, Arnold RM. “It hurts to know … and it helps”: exploring how surrogates in the ICU cope with prognostic information. J Palliat Med. 2013;16(3):243-249.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12. Zier LS, Sottile PD, Hong SY, Weissfield LA, White DB. Surrogate decision makers’ interpretation of prognostic information: a mixed-methods study. Ann Intern Med. 2012;156(5):360-366.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13. Donnino MW, Salciccioli JD, Dejam A, et al. APACHE II scoring to predict outcome in post-cardiac arrest. Resuscitation. 2013;84(5):651-656.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14. SUPPORT Principal Investigators. A controlled trial to improve care for seriously ill hospitalized patients: the Study to Understand Prognoses and Preferences for Outcomes and Risks of Treatments (SUPPORT). JAMA. 1995;274(20):1591-1598.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15. Weeks JC, Cook EF, O’Day SJ, et al. Relationship between cancer patients’ predictions of prognosis and their treatment preferences. JAMA. 1998;279(21):1709-1714.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16. Gigerenzer G, Edwards A. Simple tools for understanding risks: from innumeracy to insight. BMJ. 2003;327(7417):741-744.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17. Holland JC. Now we tell—but how well? J Clin Oncol. 1989;7(5):557-559.PubMedGoogle Scholar 18. Huskisson EC. Measurement of pain. Lancet. 1974;2(7889):1127-1131.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19. Strauss AL, Corbin J. Basics of Qualitative Research: Techniques and Procedures for Developing Grounded Theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1998. 20. Glaser BG, Strauss AL. Discovery of Grounded Theory. Chicago, IL: Adline Publishing; 1967. 21. Charmaz K. Constructing Grounded Theory: A Practical Guide Through Qualitative Analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2006. 22. Lloyd CB, Nietert PJ, Silvestri GA. Intensive care decision making in the seriously ill and elderly. Crit Care Med. 2004;32(3):649-654.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23. DeLong ER, DeLong DM, Clarke-Pearson DL. Comparing the areas under two or more correlated receiver operating characteristic curves: a nonparametric approach. Biometrics. 1988;44(3):837-845.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24. Agresti A. Categorical Data Analysis. 3rd ed. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2013. 25. Sackett DL, Guyatt GH, Tugwell P. Clinical Epidemiology: A Basic Science for Clinical Medicine. 2nd ed. Boston, MA: Little Brown & Co; 1991. 26. Patton MQ. Enhancing the quality and credibility of qualitative analysis. Health Serv Res. 1999;34(5 Pt 2):1189-1208.PubMedGoogle Scholar 27. Giacomini MK, Cook DJ. Users’ Guides to the Medical Literature, XXIII: qualitative research in health care: are the results of the study valid? JAMA. 2000;284(3):357-362.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28. Zier LS, Burack JH, Micco G, et al. Doubt and belief in physicians’ ability to prognosticate during critical illness: the perspective of surrogate decision makers. Crit Care Med. 2008;36(8):2341-2347.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29. Alicke MD, Klotz ML, Breitenbecher DL, Yurak TJ, Vredenburg DS. Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1995;68(5):804.Google ScholarCrossref 30. Sulmasy DP, Astrow AB, He MK, et al. The culture of faith and hope: patients’ justifications for their high estimations of expected therapeutic benefit when enrolling in early phase oncology trials. Cancer. 2010;116(15):3702-3711.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31. Kon AA, Davidson JE, Morrison W, Danis M, White DB; American College of Critical Care Medicine; American Thoracic Society. Shared decision making in ICUs: an American College of Critical Care Medicine and American Thoracic Society policy statement. Crit Care Med. 2016;44(1):188-201.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32. Fried TR, Bradley EH, Towle VR, Allore H. Understanding the treatment preferences of seriously ill patients. N Engl J Med. 2002;346(14):1061-1066.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref

Journal

JAMAAmerican Medical Association

Published: May 17, 2016

Keywords: critical illness,surrogate decision maker

There are no references for this article.