Get 20M+ Full-Text Papers For Less Than $1.50/day. Start a 14-Day Trial for You or Your Team.

Learn More →

Medication-Related Impulse Control and Repetitive Behaviors in Parkinson Disease

Medication-Related Impulse Control and Repetitive Behaviors in Parkinson Disease Abstract A range of behaviors presumed to be related to aberrant or excessive dopaminergic medications are being increasingly recognized in Parkinson disease. These behaviors are linked by their incentive- or reward-based and repetitive natures and include pathological gambling, hypersexuality, compulsive shopping, compulsive eating, hobbyism, and compulsive medication use. Such behaviors can have potentially devastating psychosocial consequences and are often hidden. Whether these behaviors are simply related to dopaminergic medications interacting with an underlying individual vulnerability or whether the primary pathological features of Parkinson disease play a role is not known. We reviewed the literature on these behaviors in Parkinson disease, including definitions, epidemiological and potential pathophysiological features, and management. The study of these behaviors allows not only improved clinical management but also greater insight into a biologically mediated complex behavioral model. Parkinson disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disorder characterized by motor and nonmotor features that include cognitive, neuropsychiatric, and autonomic disturbances. Neuropsychiatric symptoms are common and include mood fluctuations, anxiety fluctuations, apathy, depression, psychotic symptoms, anxiety disorders, cognitive deficits, and dementia. These nonmotor features may be related to the primary underlying pathological features in PD or may be secondary to compensatory mechanisms, treatments used (medical and surgical), unrelated comorbid disorders, or underlying individual vulnerabilities (hereditary, biological, or psychological). An intriguing set of behaviors presumed to be related to aberrant or excessive dopaminergic stimulation is being increasingly recognized in PD. These behaviors include pathological gambling,1-3 hypersexuality,2,4 compulsive shopping,2,4 compulsive eating,5 hobbyism, punding,6,7 and compulsive medication use8,9 (Table). Such behaviors can have potentially devastating psychosocial consequences, with one study reporting a mean loss of more than $100 000 resulting from pathological gambling behaviors.1 These disorders often occur without subjective distress and are frequently hidden or unnoticed by patients. Whether these behaviors are simply related to the presence of dopaminergic medications interacting with an underlying vulnerability or whether the primary pathological features of PD play a role is unknown. There is much disagreement surrounding the classification of these behaviors. The behaviors likely exist on a spectrum and may be idiosyncratic to an individual's susceptibility, hence complicating any phenomenological classification. Classification within psychiatric categories, which include obsessive-compulsive behaviors, impulse control, and addiction processes, is further confounded given the overlapping cognitive processes underlying these behaviors.10 Furthermore, which cognitive processes are dysregulated in these behaviors in PD is poorly understood. For instance, punding, which appears as habitual behaviors, likely differs from but overlaps with pathological gambling, which may have a more prominent reward or incentive component. This review uses the term repetitive behaviors to avoid any reference to the underlying pathophysiological component. We herein review the literature on these behaviors in PD, including definitions, epidemiological and potential pathophysiological features, and management. Definitions and prevalence The overall definition of these behaviors encompasses repetitive actions with or without urges and with associated negative consequences. The inclusion of consequences allows for clinical differentiation from “normal” behavior,4 although subsyndromal forms can also occur (Table). In this review, prevalence rates from studies that used systematic screening and diagnostic criteria only are included. All studies were performed in movement disorder clinics. The term prevalence represents prevalence rates after initiation of medication therapy or sometime during PD. Pathological gambling is classified within the impulse control disorders in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fourth Edition) (DSM-IV).11 The definition overlaps with substance use disorders, suggesting a “behavioral addiction.” The prevalence of pathological gambling in PD in systematic clinical studies in North America, Italy, and the United Kingdom has been reported as 3% to 8%.1-3,12,13 This compares with the lifetime prevalence of pathological gambling of 1.7% among the North American general population.14 An Italian study12 found a prevalence of pathological gambling of 6.1% in PD clinics compared with 0.25% in general medical clinics. Hypersexuality is classified under the impulse control disorders and paraphilias in the DSM-IV. The prevalence of hypersexuality in PD has been reported to be approximately 2.5%.2,4 The prevalence of compulsive shopping in PD has been reported to be 0.4% to 1.5%.2,4 Voon et al1,4 reported the overall prevalence of pathological gambling, hypersexuality, or compulsive shopping in patients with PD (hereinafter referred to as PD patients) to be 5.9%, with a point prevalence of 4.2%. The prevalence of those disorders with levodopa treatment alone was 0.7%, whereas with dopamine agonists it was 13.5% (P < .001).4 In these studies, PD patients were screened using patient-rated questionnaires (the South Oaks Gambling Screen, a clinician-designed screening hypersexuality questionnaire, and Lejoyeux's Compulsive Shopping Questionnaire).1,4 Pathological gambling was defined using DSM-IV criteria; hypersexuality, using a clinician-designed working diagnostic criteria; and compulsive shopping, using the McElroy criteria1,4 (Table). Weintraub et al2 reported similar prevalence rates of 6.6% for problem gambling, hypersexuality, and compulsive shopping in PD patients, with a point prevalence of 4.0%. These authors used systematic but unstructured clinical interview screening and diagnostic criteria based on the Minnesota Impulsive Disorders Interview.2 Despite the screening and definitional differences, the prevalence rates of both studies were similar. Nirenberg and Waters5 described a case series of compulsive eating in 7 PD patients receiving dopamine agonists. Prevalence rates were not reported. Punding behaviors are often related to an individual's previous occupation or interests (ie, prepotent habits) but can occur de novo. A video of punding can be viewed in the online version of the article by Evans et al.7 Using a structured clinician interview, Evans et al7 reported a 14% point prevalence of punding in a tertiary referral center. In contrast, Miyasaki et al15 used a patient-rated adaptation of that structured clinician interview and found a 1.5% point prevalence in a clinic with community-based referrals. These differences were suggested to be related to the clinician- vs patient-rated questionnaires, the referral base, and the potential for the short-acting dopamine agonist apomorphine hydrochloride to accentuate these behaviors; greater availability and use of apomorphine in the United Kingdom than in Canada may account for the differences.15 The prevalence of compulsive dopaminergic medication use, also known as dopamine dysregulation syndrome or hedonistic homeostatic dysregulation, has been reported to be 3.4% to 4.0%.8 Pathophysiological features Neuroanatomy and Theories on Addiction Processes Although differences exist between these behaviors, they are linked by their reward- or incentive-based and repetitive natures. The rewards are learned (pathological gambling and compulsive shopping), intrinsic (hypersexuality and compulsive eating), and drug related (compulsive medication use). The neuroanatomical regions implicated include the ventral tegmental area; its ventral striatal, limbic, and prefrontal cortical projections; and the ventral and dorsal striatum and associated frontostriatal circuitry. Several proposed theories on addiction are relevant to these behaviors.16 For instance, Everitt and Robbins17 suggest that drug-seeking actions, like habits, start out as explicit behaviors but become implicit, automatic, or overlearned stimulus-response pathological behaviors engaging the dorsal striatum. Alternatively, Robinson and Berridge16 suggest that drugs of abuse alter nucleus accumbens–related circuitry engaged in incentive processes (ie, the process of wanting rather than the process of liking, as seen in reward or hedonic tone). Behavioral sensitization (ie, an increase in behavioral drug effects with repeated exposure) occurs with psychostimulants, particularly with high or escalating doses and intermittent administration, and is associated with neuronal changes in the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex. The compulsive pursuit of drugs (eg, neutral stimuli such as drug cues becoming salient out of proportion with other stimuli) is suggested to occur through pavlovian stimulus-stimulus associations and adaptations of accumbens-related circuitry. Finally, aberrant prefrontal cortex functioning observed in drug abuse may also be associated with loss of cognitive inhibitory control over prepotent tendencies, thus resulting in impulsive behaviors. Associations With Dopaminergic Medications In studies that systematically assessed medication associations along with comparisons with PD control subjects, pathological gambling, hypersexuality, and compulsive shopping in PD were robustly associated with the use of dopamine agonists as a class but not with any specific agonists.1,2,4,12,13 Pathological gambling has not been associated with the total levodopa equivalent dose (LED).1,12,13 In systematic comparison studies, pathological gambling was not associated with the dopamine agonist dose in 2 studies1,12 but was associated with the pramipexole dose in 1 study.13 Thus, pathological gambling can be but is not necessarily associated with the dopamine agonist dose. In contrast, hypersexuality was associated with a higher LED, but the dopamine agonist dose was not.4 In a separate study, pathological gambling, hypersexuality, and compulsive shopping assessed as a group were associated with a higher LED.2 The relationship between medications and punding is less clear. Punding has not been systematically assessed for association with medication subtype, but systematic studies have noted an association with a high LED.7 In a case report, punding was reported with the dopamine antagonist quetiapine fumarate, suggesting a nondopaminergic role (given the relatively high serotonin2A receptor–binding affinity of quetiapine) or that these behaviors occur outside an optimal dopaminergic tone.18 Compulsive medication use is associated with a high LED but not with dopamine agonists per se,9 although one case series has suggested an association with short-acting agonists such as apomorphine.8 However, these associations with higher LED may be confounded by the presence of compulsive medication use. Association of Dopaminergic Medications With These Behaviors The administration of dopaminergic medications may be associated with aberrant behavior by several mechanisms: (1) interference with the pattern of dopamine release and its normal physiological role as an error-prediction or teaching signal18; (2) stimulation of particular dopamine receptors, thus resulting in aberrant activity of implicated regions; (3) dopaminergic stimulation enhancing the shift from goal-directed behaviors to stimulus response or habit formation; and (4) chronic stimulation resulting in neuronal sensitization of the ventral or dorsal striatal regions, leading to behavioral sensitization. A brief discussion on the first 2 potential mechanisms follows. A more extensive discussion on the pathophysiology can be found in Voon et al.19 Several lines of evidence suggest that phasic release of dopamine from the ventral tegmental area to the nucleus accumbens occurs at the time of anticipation of reward and at the time of receiving an unanticipated reward (ie, reward prediction error).20 Conversely, phasic suppression occurs when a reward is expected but not received. The magnitude of dopamine release varies with the magnitude of the reward. In contrast to phasic release, tonic dopamine release occurs with anticipation of the greatest reward uncertainty (ie, when there is an equal probability or chance of receiving or not receiving a reward).20 This latter observation has been interpreted to suggest that anticipation of conditions of high uncertainty such as gambling may itself be rewarding.20 Levodopa and dopamine agonists have different pharmacological properties, which may result in differences in phasic vs tonic activity. Levodopa is taken up in the presynaptic neuron and converted to dopamine. In the healthy brain or early in the course of PD with intact presynaptic neuronal density, levodopa may be more likely to mimic the physiological role of dopamine. However, with the loss of presynaptic dopaminergic neurons or with excessive doses, levodopa may be converted to dopamine outside the neuron with subsequent loss of normal physiological activity. Hence, excessive doses, appropriate doses in the context of impaired neuronal density, or postsynaptic dopamine receptor stimulation by dopamine agonists may result in loss of the normal physiological pattern of dopamine activity. A person may then anticipate a reward without the feedback teaching signal, indicating the lack of reward. To illustrate, in a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in healthy individuals, excessive doses of levodopa given in the presence of intact neuronal density increased the reinforcing effect of wins but not losses as measured by ventral striatum blood-oxygen level–dependent activity, a representation of outcome prediction error (Figure 1).21 Thus, in individuals receiving levodopa, approach behaviors would be more likely to be reinforced by rewards than avoidance by punishments, possibly leading to repetitive behaviors. Alternatively, stimulation of specific dopamine receptor subtypes may be associated with aberrant activity. For instance, D3 receptor–binding affinities may differ between dopamine agonists, in addition to the relative ratio of binding affinities of the D3/D1 or the D3/D2 receptors.22 Unlike the D1 or D2 receptors, D3 receptors are found predominantly in limbic regions and have been implicated in the addiction process. Susceptibility The lack of a consistent dose effect1,4,12,13 and the occurrence of symptoms in only a subset of patients suggest an underlying susceptibility. This susceptibility may be mediated by PD-related factors such as (1) the neurobiology of PD (eg, pathological features and compensatory mechanisms that may also modulate underlying temperamental traits or cognitive processes), (2) PD-specific medication practices, or (3) individual factors underlying the vulnerability to pathological gambling, addiction disorders, or impulse control behaviors (Figure 2). The latter suggests that dopamine agonists used for non-PD disorders (eg, restless legs syndrome or depression) may also trigger these symptoms. PD-Related Factors Subgroups. First, PD-related factors could be mediated by subgroups of PD patients with a greater risk for behavioral disorders. For example, recurrent psychosis occurs more frequently in patients with the parkin gene,24 and depression occurs more frequently in early-onset PD. Early-onset PD may also have a greater genetic association. The susceptibility is presumably related to a different distribution and nature of pathological involvement and hence of medication response. Pathological gambling and compulsive medication use are both associated with an earlier onset of PD,9,23 suggesting a potential role for the vulnerability of specific PD patient subgroups to these behaviors. However, this association may be confounded by the relatively greater use of dopamine agonists and higher doses used in early-onset PD. General Neurobiological Features. Second, aberrant functioning secondary to PD (eg, neurodegeneration, α-synuclein deposition, or compensatory up-regulation) of relevant neuroanatomical networks may be implicated. This may be due to direct involvement by the neurodegenerative process or to compensatory changes in intact, “unaffected” regions. The effect of PD and dopaminergic medications on dopaminergic tone may also influence cognitive functions relevant to these behaviors. For instance, PD patients who are “off” medications have been demonstrated to be more sensitive to punishment learning, whereas they are more sensitive to reward learning while “on” medications.25 Impairments in cognitive flexibility (eg, reversal learning, attentional set shifting) have also been associated with PD or with medication administration.26 Which cognitive functions are relevant to these behaviors and how medications and PD affect them are unknown because medications can enhance or impair cognitive function. In PD patients, dopamine influences working memory in a U-shaped curve (eg, optimal dopamine tone) rather than in a linear fashion.27 The overdose hypothesis suggests that, in PD patients, relatively preserved functions associated with the ventral striatum and ventral prefrontal cortex (eg, reversal learning) may be more sensitive to overstimulation and impairment due to dopaminergic medications compared with the dorsal striatum and dorsolateral prefrontal cortical functions (eg, attentional set shifting and working memory). Untreated PD is associated with impairments in dorsolateral prefrontal cortical functioning (eg, working memory), which are improved with dopaminergic medications. The cognitive impairments associated with PD are not within the scope of this review but are reviewed elsewhere.26 However, an exclusive relationship to general PD-related neurobiological features suggests that the repetitive behaviors should be more common than they seem to be or that, as the disease progresses, the onset or severity may be mediated by disease markers such as stage, duration, and levodopa dose. Voon et al23 did not find an association between pathological gambling and disease markers, thus arguing that PD-related neurobiological features, rather than playing a primary role, may interact with individual vulnerabilities to increase susceptibility. A weak association between pathological gambling and right-sided PD onset was found, although the implications are not clear.23 Medication Practices. Third, compared with other disorders for which dopaminergic drugs are used, PD may involve specific medication practices such as the use of higher doses, the diurnal administration of dopamine agonist, or concurrent levodopa use. Voon et al1 have suggested that levodopa may play a priming role potentiating the onset of these behaviors. A robust association of pathological gambling with dopamine agonists as an adjunct therapy rather than as monotherapy was observed, although the data were limited by sample size and differences in agonist doses.1,8 However, clinical studies are prone to confounding factors. Studies in normal rodents and primates suggest that pulsatile and long-term administration of psychostimulants results in psychomotor behaviors associated with functional and anatomical changes in the ventral striatum and prefrontal cortex that are presumed to underlie the behavioral sensitization process. Studies in 1-methyl 4-phenyl 1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP)–lesioned marmosets (a nonhuman primate model of PD) have shown that repeated treatment with levodopa results in the development of similar abnormal psychomimetic behaviors; these have recently been proposed to represent psychosislike behaviors.28 In this model, there is an association between the dose of levodopa and production of psychosislike behaviors. Preclinical studies29 in unilateral MPTP models of PD further suggest that levodopa administration may result in aberrant D3 receptor expression, which has been suggested to play a role in the sensitization processes and in the pathophysiological mechanisms of dyskinesias. Exposure to levodopa may thus result in increased susceptibility to the potential of dopamine D3 agonists to induce repetitive behaviors. Indeed, Dodd et al30 suggested a link between pathological gambling in PD patients and the use of the dopamine D3–preferring agonist pramipexole; however, other clinical studies have not shown this link.1,2,4,12,13 To try to delineate this further, studies in the MPTP-lesioned primate primed to express psychosislike and compulsivelike behaviors with long-term levodopa administration showed no significant differences among the abilities of levodopa, pergolide mesylate, ropinirole hydrochloride, and pramipexole to induce compulsivelike behaviors at doses with equivalent antiparkinsonian actions.31 This suggests that dopamine D3–preferring agonists may be no more likely than levodopa to induce compulsive-like behaviors. However, whether this animal model represents the full spectrum of repetitive behaviors is not known. Individual Susceptibility In contrast to PD-related factors or in addition to them, individual susceptibility may play a role. This susceptibility can include a predisposition to pathological gambling or other addiction disorders through underlying heritable temperamental traits (eg, novelty or sensation seeking and impulsivity), cognitive processes (eg, in temporal discounting, impulsive choice or action, risk taking, reward or punishment learning, reversal learning, and set shifting), aberrant neural networks (eg, ventral or dorsal striatal, prefrontal cortex, and limbic regions), age, sex, or genetic polymorphisms. For example, pathological gambling in the general population is associated with the male sex, a history of substance abuse, higher levels of impulsivity and sensation seeking, cognitive impairments in response inhibition and planning, and higher delay discounting rates. Pathological gambling has been associated with aberrant frontostriatal and limbic activity with decreased activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the Stroop interference task (a measure of the ability to inhibit irrelevant responses) and with decreased activation in the ventral striatum to a forced choice win-vs-loss task. Furthermore, pathological gambling has been associated with increased dopamine function with reduced acoustic startle prepulse inhibition, increased dopamine metabolites in the cerebrospinal fluid, and a study demonstrating the ability of amphetamine to prime the motivation to gamble.32 The published case-control studies that assessed factors associated with repetitive behaviors in PD are reviewed in the following paragraphs. Repetitive Behaviors. Weintraub et al2 demonstrated that a premorbid history of repetitive behaviors predisposed patients to these behaviors while taking dopamine agonists. Pontone et al33 demonstrated that depressed mood, increased irritability, disinhibition, and appetite disturbances may be more common in patients with these behaviors, although whether these behaviors predate the use of medications is not clear. Compulsive Medication Use. In keeping with the addiction literature, Evans et al9 found novelty seeking, depression scores, alcohol intake, and age of PD onset to be independent predictors of repetitive medication use in PD patients. The study did not confirm initial associations with male sex.9 In a positron emission tomography study using raclopride tagged with carbon 11 (D2/D3 receptor ligand), Evans et al34 demonstrated increased dopamine release in the ventral striatum in response to a levodopa challenge in PD patients with compulsive medication use. It was suggested that this finding supported the neural sensitization theory leading to the behavior of compulsive drug seeking. Furthermore, the subjective wanting but not the liking of levodopa was positively correlated with the degree of ventral striatal dopamine release. This observation was suggested to reflect an incentive salience for levodopa use rather than a pleasurable or reward-related quality.35 Pathological Gambling and Hypersexuality. Pathological gambling in the general population has been most consistently associated with male sex, mood and substance use disorders, high sensation seeking, and impulsivity.32 Voon et al23 found independent associations of elevated novelty-seeking traits, a personal or family history of alcohol use disorders, and earlier age of PD onset with pathological gambling in PD patients. Using a logistic regression model, these factors accounted for 62% of the variance. Impulsivity scores, particularly planning for long-term consequences, were more weakly associated with pathological gambling. The data, albeit preliminary, suggested that novelty seeking is more likely to be trait related (ie, to reflect an underlying heritable vulnerability), whereas the impulsivity scores may be more state related (ie, related to the presence of dopamine agonist or the state of active pathological gambling).23 A high level of novelty seeking has been associated with pathological gambling and is characterized by exploratory approach rather than avoidance behaviors, excitement with novel situations, impulsivity, rapid decision making, extravagance, and greater disorganization. A low level of novelty seeking has been variably but not consistently associated with PD and is characterized by a reflective, rigid, and slow-tempered temperament. The lowered novelty-seeking level, presumed to be related to the neurobiological mechanisms of PD affecting the ventral striatum, has been suggested to play a role in the lower rates of alcohol, cigarette, and other drug use in the PD population and may play a protective role against these behaviors before the onset of medication therapy. Medication-induced mania with onset after that of pathological gambling is robustly associated with pathological gambling in PD patients.23 However, rather than a bipolar diathesis being a risk factor, medication-induced mania may be part of the spectrum of dopamine dysregulation.8,23 In contrast to the studies on compulsive medication use and general pathological gambling, there was no association with depression or male sex. However, because pathological gambling in the general population is associated with males more than females in a 2:1 ratio, the lack of an association with sex in PD may be due to limited study power. Hypersexuality in PD is more likely to be associated with younger onset PD and, in contrast to that of pathological gambling, with males.4 Compulsive Eating. In an uncontrolled case series, Nirenberg et al5 found that PD patients with compulsive eating behaviors who were taking dopamine agonists were likely to have younger-onset PD and premorbid histories of repetitive behaviors or being overweight. Management Patients should be warned of the potential risk of these behaviors before initiation of treatment with dopamine agonists. The identification of patients at greater risk to allow for closer follow-up would be an ideal component to the ongoing management of PD. With all studies, earlier age of PD onset is associated with these behaviors, although differences in medication practices may mediate the association.1,4,9,23 Current studies suggest that specific risk factors for pathological gambling may include a personal or family history of alcohol use disorders and increased novelty-seeking traits,23 whereas impulsivity23 (or disinhibition34) may be associated but is more likely state related. Specific risk factors for compulsive medication use include increased novelty seeking, alcohol use, and higher depression scores.9 Hypersexuality is associated with the male sex.4 Nonspecific associated factors include a premorbid history of these behaviors,2 depressed mood, irritability, and appetite disturbances.34 Higher medication doses can be but are not necessarily associated with these behaviors.1,2,4 The management of pathological gambling has been described only in anecdotal reports. In patients with compulsive medication use or who take high doses, a decrease in medication dose may be sufficient. Decreasing or discontinuing therapy or switching to a different dopamine agonist may be but is not consistently effective. Antidepressants have not been found to be useful in case reports. External controls (eg, external control of finances) and referrals for gambling treatment have been anecdotally reported to be useful in a proportion of patients. Ardouin et al35 described a multicenter retrospective series of PD patients with active pathological gambling before deep brain stimulation targeting the subthalamic nucleus. The pathological gambling symptoms resolved immediately after or over the course of months in parallel with the decrease in the LED. The authors suggest that improvement may be related to (1) dose decreases or discontinuation of dopamine agonist therapy; (2) the relative specificity of deep brain stimulation for motor regions compared with medications; (3) the replacement of pulsatile stimulation with chronic stimulation, which may decrease neural sensitization; or (4) the actions of deep brain stimulation targeting the subthalmic nucleus.21 Point 4 is exemplified by studies of lesions in the subthalmic nucleus in rodent models of substance abuse. However, that the behaviors can worsen in the early postoperative period suggests that caution be applied in the selection and postoperative management of these patients. In subjects with compulsive medication use, management includes decreasing doses; external controls of doses; treatment of secondary psychotic, manic, or behavioral symptoms; and management of dopaminergic depressive withdrawal symptoms. Multidisciplinary involvement may be necessary.8,9 Similarly, deep brain stimulation of the subthalmic nucleus has been reported to be effective in well-selected patients. Relapse of addictive dopaminergic drug abuse behaviors is not infrequent in these patients. Conclusions Medication-induced repetitive behaviors as a group are relatively common and can have devastating psychosocial consequences. The study of these behaviors allows not only improved clinical management but also greater insight into a biologically mediated complex behavioral model. Back to top Article Information Correspondence: Valerie Voon, MD, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, 10 Center Dr, Bldg 10, Room 5S213, Bethesda, MD 20892-1428 (voonv@ninds.nih.gov). Accepted for Publication: November 20, 2006. Author Contributions:Study concept and design: Voon. Acquisition of data: Voon. Analysis and interpretation of data: Voon and Fox. Drafting of the manuscript: Voon. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Voon and Fox. Obtained funding: Fox. Financial Disclosure: None reported. Acknowledgment: Anthony E. Lang, MD, and Venkata S. Mattay, MD, provided comments. References 1. Voon VHassan KZurowski M et al. Prospective prevalence of pathological gambling and medication association in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2006;66 (11) 1750- 1752PubMedGoogle Scholar 2. Weintraub DSiderowf ADPotenza MN et al. Association of dopamine agonist use with impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 2006;63 (7) 969- 973PubMedGoogle Scholar 3. Lu CBharmal ASuchowersky O Gambling and Parkinson disease [letter]. Arch Neurol 2006;63 (2) 298PubMedGoogle Scholar 4. Voon VHassan KZurowski M et al. Prevalence of repetitive and reward-seeking behaviors in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2006;67 (7) 1254- 1257PubMedGoogle Scholar 5. Nirenberg MJWaters C Compulsive eating and weight gain related to dopamine agonist use. Mov Disord 2006;21 (4) 524- 529PubMedGoogle Scholar 6. Fernandez HHFriedman JH Punding on L-dopa. Mov Disord 1999;14 (5) 836- 838PubMedGoogle Scholar 7. Evans AHKatzenschlager RPaviour DC et al. Punding in Parkinson's disease: its relation to the dopamine dysregulation syndrome. Mov Disord 2004;19 (4) 397- 405PubMedGoogle Scholar 8. Giovannoni GO’Sullivan JDTurner KManson AJLees AJ Hedonistic homeostatic dysregulation in patients with Parkinson's disease on dopamine replacement therapies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000;68 (4) 423- 428PubMedGoogle Scholar 9. Evans AHLawrence ADPotts JAppel SLees AJ Factors influencing susceptibility to compulsive dopaminergic drug use in Parkinson. Neurology 2005;65 (10) 1570- 1574PubMedGoogle Scholar 10. Potenza MN Should addictive disorders include non–substance-related conditions? Addiction September 2006;101(suppl 1)142- 151PubMedGoogle Scholar 11. American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994 12. Avanzi MBaratti MCabrini SUber EBrighetti GBonfa F Prevalence of pathological gambling in patients with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 2006;21 (12) 2068- 2072PubMedGoogle Scholar 13. Grosset KAMacphee GPal G et al. Problematic gambling on dopamine agonists: not such a rarity. Mov Disord 2006;21 (12) 2206- 2208PubMedGoogle Scholar 14. Shaffer HJHall MNVander Bilt J Estimating the prevalence of disordered gambling behavior in the United States and Canada: a research synthesis. Am J Public Health 1999;89 (9) 1369- 1376PubMedGoogle Scholar 15. Miyasaki JMAl Hassan KLang AEVoon V Punding prevalence in Parkinson's disease [published online ahead of print January 17, 2007]. Mov Disord doi:10.1002/mds.21296Google Scholar 16. Robinson TEBerridge KC Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol 2003;5425- 53PubMedGoogle Scholar 17. Everitt BJRobbins TW Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci200581114811489 [published correction appears in Nat Neurosci.2006;97:979]. PubMedGoogle Scholar 18. Miwa HMorita SNakanishi IKondo T Stereotyped behaviors or punding after quetiapine administration in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2004;10 (3) 177- 180PubMedGoogle Scholar 19. Voon VPotenza MNThomsen T Impulse control and repetitive behaviors in Parkinson's disease. Curr Opin Neurol In press Google Scholar 20. Fiorillo CDTobler PNSchultz W Discrete coding of reward probability and uncertainty by dopamine neurons. Science 2003;299 (5614) 1898- 1902PubMedGoogle Scholar 21. Pessiglione MSeymour BFlandin GDolan RJFrith CD Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behavior in humans. Nature 2006;442 (7106) 1042- 1045PubMedGoogle Scholar 22. Millan MJMaiofiss LCussac DAudinot VBoutin JANewman-Tancredi A Differential actions of antiparkinsonian agents at multiple classes of monoaminergic receptor, I: a multivariate analysis of the binding profiles of 14 drugs at 21 native and cloned human receptor subtypes. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2002;303 (2) 791- 804PubMedGoogle Scholar 23. Voon VThomsen TMiyasaki JM et al. Factors associated with dopaminergic medication–related pathological gambling in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 2007;64 (2) 212- 216PubMedGoogle Scholar 24. Khan NLGraham ECritchley P et al. Parkin disease: a phenotypic study of a large case series. Brain 2003;126 (pt 6) 1279- 1292PubMedGoogle Scholar 25. Frank MJSeeberger LJO’Reilly RC By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science 2004;306 (5703) 1940- 1943PubMedGoogle Scholar 26. Cools R Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function—implications for L-dopa treatment in Parkinson's disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006;30 (1) 1- 23PubMedGoogle Scholar 27. Mattay VSTessitore ACallicott JH et al. Dopaminergic modulation of cortical function in patients with Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol 2002;51 (2) 156- 164PubMedGoogle Scholar 28. Visanji NPGomez-Ramirez JJohnston TH et al. Pharmacological characterization of psychosis-like behavior in the MPTP-lesioned nonhuman primate model of Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 2006;21 (11) 1879- 1891PubMedGoogle Scholar 29. Bézard EFerry SMach U et al. Attenuation of levodopa-induced dyskinesia by normalizing dopamine D3 receptor function. Nat Med 2003;9 (6) 762- 767PubMedGoogle Scholar 30. Dodd MLKlos KJBower JHGeda YEJosephs KAAhlskog JE Pathological gambling caused by drugs used to treat Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 2005;62 (9) 1377- 1381PubMedGoogle Scholar 31. Fox SHVisanji NGomez-Ramirez JJohnston TVoon VBrotchie JM Dopamine receptor agonists and levodopa have similar ability to induce psychosis-like behavior in the MPTP primate model of Parkinson's disease. Arch Neurol 2006;63 (9) 1343- 1344PubMedGoogle Scholar 32. Goudriaan AEOosterlaan Jde Beurs Evan der Brink W Pathological gambling: a comprehensive review of biobehavioral findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004;28 (2) 123- 141PubMedGoogle Scholar 33. Pontone GWilliams JRBassett SSMarsh L Clinical features associated with impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2006;67 (7) 1258- 1261PubMedGoogle Scholar 34. Evans AHPavese NLawrence AD et al. Compulsive drug use linked to sensitized ventral striatal dopamine transmission. Ann Neurol 2006;59 (5) 852- 858PubMedGoogle Scholar 35. Ardouin CVoon VWorbe C et al. Pathological gambling in Parkinson's disease improves chronic subthalamic stimulation. Mov Disord 2006;21 (11) 1941- 1946PubMedGoogle Scholar http://www.deepdyve.com/assets/images/DeepDyve-Logo-lg.png Archives of Neurology American Medical Association

Medication-Related Impulse Control and Repetitive Behaviors in Parkinson Disease

Archives of Neurology , Volume 64 (8) – Aug 1, 2007

Loading next page...
 
/lp/american-medical-association/medication-related-impulse-control-and-repetitive-behaviors-in-mkLpwT79YH

References (46)

Publisher
American Medical Association
Copyright
Copyright © 2007 American Medical Association. All Rights Reserved.
ISSN
0003-9942
DOI
10.1001/archneur.64.8.1089
pmid
17698698
Publisher site
See Article on Publisher Site

Abstract

Abstract A range of behaviors presumed to be related to aberrant or excessive dopaminergic medications are being increasingly recognized in Parkinson disease. These behaviors are linked by their incentive- or reward-based and repetitive natures and include pathological gambling, hypersexuality, compulsive shopping, compulsive eating, hobbyism, and compulsive medication use. Such behaviors can have potentially devastating psychosocial consequences and are often hidden. Whether these behaviors are simply related to dopaminergic medications interacting with an underlying individual vulnerability or whether the primary pathological features of Parkinson disease play a role is not known. We reviewed the literature on these behaviors in Parkinson disease, including definitions, epidemiological and potential pathophysiological features, and management. The study of these behaviors allows not only improved clinical management but also greater insight into a biologically mediated complex behavioral model. Parkinson disease (PD) is a neurodegenerative disorder characterized by motor and nonmotor features that include cognitive, neuropsychiatric, and autonomic disturbances. Neuropsychiatric symptoms are common and include mood fluctuations, anxiety fluctuations, apathy, depression, psychotic symptoms, anxiety disorders, cognitive deficits, and dementia. These nonmotor features may be related to the primary underlying pathological features in PD or may be secondary to compensatory mechanisms, treatments used (medical and surgical), unrelated comorbid disorders, or underlying individual vulnerabilities (hereditary, biological, or psychological). An intriguing set of behaviors presumed to be related to aberrant or excessive dopaminergic stimulation is being increasingly recognized in PD. These behaviors include pathological gambling,1-3 hypersexuality,2,4 compulsive shopping,2,4 compulsive eating,5 hobbyism, punding,6,7 and compulsive medication use8,9 (Table). Such behaviors can have potentially devastating psychosocial consequences, with one study reporting a mean loss of more than $100 000 resulting from pathological gambling behaviors.1 These disorders often occur without subjective distress and are frequently hidden or unnoticed by patients. Whether these behaviors are simply related to the presence of dopaminergic medications interacting with an underlying vulnerability or whether the primary pathological features of PD play a role is unknown. There is much disagreement surrounding the classification of these behaviors. The behaviors likely exist on a spectrum and may be idiosyncratic to an individual's susceptibility, hence complicating any phenomenological classification. Classification within psychiatric categories, which include obsessive-compulsive behaviors, impulse control, and addiction processes, is further confounded given the overlapping cognitive processes underlying these behaviors.10 Furthermore, which cognitive processes are dysregulated in these behaviors in PD is poorly understood. For instance, punding, which appears as habitual behaviors, likely differs from but overlaps with pathological gambling, which may have a more prominent reward or incentive component. This review uses the term repetitive behaviors to avoid any reference to the underlying pathophysiological component. We herein review the literature on these behaviors in PD, including definitions, epidemiological and potential pathophysiological features, and management. Definitions and prevalence The overall definition of these behaviors encompasses repetitive actions with or without urges and with associated negative consequences. The inclusion of consequences allows for clinical differentiation from “normal” behavior,4 although subsyndromal forms can also occur (Table). In this review, prevalence rates from studies that used systematic screening and diagnostic criteria only are included. All studies were performed in movement disorder clinics. The term prevalence represents prevalence rates after initiation of medication therapy or sometime during PD. Pathological gambling is classified within the impulse control disorders in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fourth Edition) (DSM-IV).11 The definition overlaps with substance use disorders, suggesting a “behavioral addiction.” The prevalence of pathological gambling in PD in systematic clinical studies in North America, Italy, and the United Kingdom has been reported as 3% to 8%.1-3,12,13 This compares with the lifetime prevalence of pathological gambling of 1.7% among the North American general population.14 An Italian study12 found a prevalence of pathological gambling of 6.1% in PD clinics compared with 0.25% in general medical clinics. Hypersexuality is classified under the impulse control disorders and paraphilias in the DSM-IV. The prevalence of hypersexuality in PD has been reported to be approximately 2.5%.2,4 The prevalence of compulsive shopping in PD has been reported to be 0.4% to 1.5%.2,4 Voon et al1,4 reported the overall prevalence of pathological gambling, hypersexuality, or compulsive shopping in patients with PD (hereinafter referred to as PD patients) to be 5.9%, with a point prevalence of 4.2%. The prevalence of those disorders with levodopa treatment alone was 0.7%, whereas with dopamine agonists it was 13.5% (P < .001).4 In these studies, PD patients were screened using patient-rated questionnaires (the South Oaks Gambling Screen, a clinician-designed screening hypersexuality questionnaire, and Lejoyeux's Compulsive Shopping Questionnaire).1,4 Pathological gambling was defined using DSM-IV criteria; hypersexuality, using a clinician-designed working diagnostic criteria; and compulsive shopping, using the McElroy criteria1,4 (Table). Weintraub et al2 reported similar prevalence rates of 6.6% for problem gambling, hypersexuality, and compulsive shopping in PD patients, with a point prevalence of 4.0%. These authors used systematic but unstructured clinical interview screening and diagnostic criteria based on the Minnesota Impulsive Disorders Interview.2 Despite the screening and definitional differences, the prevalence rates of both studies were similar. Nirenberg and Waters5 described a case series of compulsive eating in 7 PD patients receiving dopamine agonists. Prevalence rates were not reported. Punding behaviors are often related to an individual's previous occupation or interests (ie, prepotent habits) but can occur de novo. A video of punding can be viewed in the online version of the article by Evans et al.7 Using a structured clinician interview, Evans et al7 reported a 14% point prevalence of punding in a tertiary referral center. In contrast, Miyasaki et al15 used a patient-rated adaptation of that structured clinician interview and found a 1.5% point prevalence in a clinic with community-based referrals. These differences were suggested to be related to the clinician- vs patient-rated questionnaires, the referral base, and the potential for the short-acting dopamine agonist apomorphine hydrochloride to accentuate these behaviors; greater availability and use of apomorphine in the United Kingdom than in Canada may account for the differences.15 The prevalence of compulsive dopaminergic medication use, also known as dopamine dysregulation syndrome or hedonistic homeostatic dysregulation, has been reported to be 3.4% to 4.0%.8 Pathophysiological features Neuroanatomy and Theories on Addiction Processes Although differences exist between these behaviors, they are linked by their reward- or incentive-based and repetitive natures. The rewards are learned (pathological gambling and compulsive shopping), intrinsic (hypersexuality and compulsive eating), and drug related (compulsive medication use). The neuroanatomical regions implicated include the ventral tegmental area; its ventral striatal, limbic, and prefrontal cortical projections; and the ventral and dorsal striatum and associated frontostriatal circuitry. Several proposed theories on addiction are relevant to these behaviors.16 For instance, Everitt and Robbins17 suggest that drug-seeking actions, like habits, start out as explicit behaviors but become implicit, automatic, or overlearned stimulus-response pathological behaviors engaging the dorsal striatum. Alternatively, Robinson and Berridge16 suggest that drugs of abuse alter nucleus accumbens–related circuitry engaged in incentive processes (ie, the process of wanting rather than the process of liking, as seen in reward or hedonic tone). Behavioral sensitization (ie, an increase in behavioral drug effects with repeated exposure) occurs with psychostimulants, particularly with high or escalating doses and intermittent administration, and is associated with neuronal changes in the nucleus accumbens and prefrontal cortex. The compulsive pursuit of drugs (eg, neutral stimuli such as drug cues becoming salient out of proportion with other stimuli) is suggested to occur through pavlovian stimulus-stimulus associations and adaptations of accumbens-related circuitry. Finally, aberrant prefrontal cortex functioning observed in drug abuse may also be associated with loss of cognitive inhibitory control over prepotent tendencies, thus resulting in impulsive behaviors. Associations With Dopaminergic Medications In studies that systematically assessed medication associations along with comparisons with PD control subjects, pathological gambling, hypersexuality, and compulsive shopping in PD were robustly associated with the use of dopamine agonists as a class but not with any specific agonists.1,2,4,12,13 Pathological gambling has not been associated with the total levodopa equivalent dose (LED).1,12,13 In systematic comparison studies, pathological gambling was not associated with the dopamine agonist dose in 2 studies1,12 but was associated with the pramipexole dose in 1 study.13 Thus, pathological gambling can be but is not necessarily associated with the dopamine agonist dose. In contrast, hypersexuality was associated with a higher LED, but the dopamine agonist dose was not.4 In a separate study, pathological gambling, hypersexuality, and compulsive shopping assessed as a group were associated with a higher LED.2 The relationship between medications and punding is less clear. Punding has not been systematically assessed for association with medication subtype, but systematic studies have noted an association with a high LED.7 In a case report, punding was reported with the dopamine antagonist quetiapine fumarate, suggesting a nondopaminergic role (given the relatively high serotonin2A receptor–binding affinity of quetiapine) or that these behaviors occur outside an optimal dopaminergic tone.18 Compulsive medication use is associated with a high LED but not with dopamine agonists per se,9 although one case series has suggested an association with short-acting agonists such as apomorphine.8 However, these associations with higher LED may be confounded by the presence of compulsive medication use. Association of Dopaminergic Medications With These Behaviors The administration of dopaminergic medications may be associated with aberrant behavior by several mechanisms: (1) interference with the pattern of dopamine release and its normal physiological role as an error-prediction or teaching signal18; (2) stimulation of particular dopamine receptors, thus resulting in aberrant activity of implicated regions; (3) dopaminergic stimulation enhancing the shift from goal-directed behaviors to stimulus response or habit formation; and (4) chronic stimulation resulting in neuronal sensitization of the ventral or dorsal striatal regions, leading to behavioral sensitization. A brief discussion on the first 2 potential mechanisms follows. A more extensive discussion on the pathophysiology can be found in Voon et al.19 Several lines of evidence suggest that phasic release of dopamine from the ventral tegmental area to the nucleus accumbens occurs at the time of anticipation of reward and at the time of receiving an unanticipated reward (ie, reward prediction error).20 Conversely, phasic suppression occurs when a reward is expected but not received. The magnitude of dopamine release varies with the magnitude of the reward. In contrast to phasic release, tonic dopamine release occurs with anticipation of the greatest reward uncertainty (ie, when there is an equal probability or chance of receiving or not receiving a reward).20 This latter observation has been interpreted to suggest that anticipation of conditions of high uncertainty such as gambling may itself be rewarding.20 Levodopa and dopamine agonists have different pharmacological properties, which may result in differences in phasic vs tonic activity. Levodopa is taken up in the presynaptic neuron and converted to dopamine. In the healthy brain or early in the course of PD with intact presynaptic neuronal density, levodopa may be more likely to mimic the physiological role of dopamine. However, with the loss of presynaptic dopaminergic neurons or with excessive doses, levodopa may be converted to dopamine outside the neuron with subsequent loss of normal physiological activity. Hence, excessive doses, appropriate doses in the context of impaired neuronal density, or postsynaptic dopamine receptor stimulation by dopamine agonists may result in loss of the normal physiological pattern of dopamine activity. A person may then anticipate a reward without the feedback teaching signal, indicating the lack of reward. To illustrate, in a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in healthy individuals, excessive doses of levodopa given in the presence of intact neuronal density increased the reinforcing effect of wins but not losses as measured by ventral striatum blood-oxygen level–dependent activity, a representation of outcome prediction error (Figure 1).21 Thus, in individuals receiving levodopa, approach behaviors would be more likely to be reinforced by rewards than avoidance by punishments, possibly leading to repetitive behaviors. Alternatively, stimulation of specific dopamine receptor subtypes may be associated with aberrant activity. For instance, D3 receptor–binding affinities may differ between dopamine agonists, in addition to the relative ratio of binding affinities of the D3/D1 or the D3/D2 receptors.22 Unlike the D1 or D2 receptors, D3 receptors are found predominantly in limbic regions and have been implicated in the addiction process. Susceptibility The lack of a consistent dose effect1,4,12,13 and the occurrence of symptoms in only a subset of patients suggest an underlying susceptibility. This susceptibility may be mediated by PD-related factors such as (1) the neurobiology of PD (eg, pathological features and compensatory mechanisms that may also modulate underlying temperamental traits or cognitive processes), (2) PD-specific medication practices, or (3) individual factors underlying the vulnerability to pathological gambling, addiction disorders, or impulse control behaviors (Figure 2). The latter suggests that dopamine agonists used for non-PD disorders (eg, restless legs syndrome or depression) may also trigger these symptoms. PD-Related Factors Subgroups. First, PD-related factors could be mediated by subgroups of PD patients with a greater risk for behavioral disorders. For example, recurrent psychosis occurs more frequently in patients with the parkin gene,24 and depression occurs more frequently in early-onset PD. Early-onset PD may also have a greater genetic association. The susceptibility is presumably related to a different distribution and nature of pathological involvement and hence of medication response. Pathological gambling and compulsive medication use are both associated with an earlier onset of PD,9,23 suggesting a potential role for the vulnerability of specific PD patient subgroups to these behaviors. However, this association may be confounded by the relatively greater use of dopamine agonists and higher doses used in early-onset PD. General Neurobiological Features. Second, aberrant functioning secondary to PD (eg, neurodegeneration, α-synuclein deposition, or compensatory up-regulation) of relevant neuroanatomical networks may be implicated. This may be due to direct involvement by the neurodegenerative process or to compensatory changes in intact, “unaffected” regions. The effect of PD and dopaminergic medications on dopaminergic tone may also influence cognitive functions relevant to these behaviors. For instance, PD patients who are “off” medications have been demonstrated to be more sensitive to punishment learning, whereas they are more sensitive to reward learning while “on” medications.25 Impairments in cognitive flexibility (eg, reversal learning, attentional set shifting) have also been associated with PD or with medication administration.26 Which cognitive functions are relevant to these behaviors and how medications and PD affect them are unknown because medications can enhance or impair cognitive function. In PD patients, dopamine influences working memory in a U-shaped curve (eg, optimal dopamine tone) rather than in a linear fashion.27 The overdose hypothesis suggests that, in PD patients, relatively preserved functions associated with the ventral striatum and ventral prefrontal cortex (eg, reversal learning) may be more sensitive to overstimulation and impairment due to dopaminergic medications compared with the dorsal striatum and dorsolateral prefrontal cortical functions (eg, attentional set shifting and working memory). Untreated PD is associated with impairments in dorsolateral prefrontal cortical functioning (eg, working memory), which are improved with dopaminergic medications. The cognitive impairments associated with PD are not within the scope of this review but are reviewed elsewhere.26 However, an exclusive relationship to general PD-related neurobiological features suggests that the repetitive behaviors should be more common than they seem to be or that, as the disease progresses, the onset or severity may be mediated by disease markers such as stage, duration, and levodopa dose. Voon et al23 did not find an association between pathological gambling and disease markers, thus arguing that PD-related neurobiological features, rather than playing a primary role, may interact with individual vulnerabilities to increase susceptibility. A weak association between pathological gambling and right-sided PD onset was found, although the implications are not clear.23 Medication Practices. Third, compared with other disorders for which dopaminergic drugs are used, PD may involve specific medication practices such as the use of higher doses, the diurnal administration of dopamine agonist, or concurrent levodopa use. Voon et al1 have suggested that levodopa may play a priming role potentiating the onset of these behaviors. A robust association of pathological gambling with dopamine agonists as an adjunct therapy rather than as monotherapy was observed, although the data were limited by sample size and differences in agonist doses.1,8 However, clinical studies are prone to confounding factors. Studies in normal rodents and primates suggest that pulsatile and long-term administration of psychostimulants results in psychomotor behaviors associated with functional and anatomical changes in the ventral striatum and prefrontal cortex that are presumed to underlie the behavioral sensitization process. Studies in 1-methyl 4-phenyl 1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP)–lesioned marmosets (a nonhuman primate model of PD) have shown that repeated treatment with levodopa results in the development of similar abnormal psychomimetic behaviors; these have recently been proposed to represent psychosislike behaviors.28 In this model, there is an association between the dose of levodopa and production of psychosislike behaviors. Preclinical studies29 in unilateral MPTP models of PD further suggest that levodopa administration may result in aberrant D3 receptor expression, which has been suggested to play a role in the sensitization processes and in the pathophysiological mechanisms of dyskinesias. Exposure to levodopa may thus result in increased susceptibility to the potential of dopamine D3 agonists to induce repetitive behaviors. Indeed, Dodd et al30 suggested a link between pathological gambling in PD patients and the use of the dopamine D3–preferring agonist pramipexole; however, other clinical studies have not shown this link.1,2,4,12,13 To try to delineate this further, studies in the MPTP-lesioned primate primed to express psychosislike and compulsivelike behaviors with long-term levodopa administration showed no significant differences among the abilities of levodopa, pergolide mesylate, ropinirole hydrochloride, and pramipexole to induce compulsivelike behaviors at doses with equivalent antiparkinsonian actions.31 This suggests that dopamine D3–preferring agonists may be no more likely than levodopa to induce compulsive-like behaviors. However, whether this animal model represents the full spectrum of repetitive behaviors is not known. Individual Susceptibility In contrast to PD-related factors or in addition to them, individual susceptibility may play a role. This susceptibility can include a predisposition to pathological gambling or other addiction disorders through underlying heritable temperamental traits (eg, novelty or sensation seeking and impulsivity), cognitive processes (eg, in temporal discounting, impulsive choice or action, risk taking, reward or punishment learning, reversal learning, and set shifting), aberrant neural networks (eg, ventral or dorsal striatal, prefrontal cortex, and limbic regions), age, sex, or genetic polymorphisms. For example, pathological gambling in the general population is associated with the male sex, a history of substance abuse, higher levels of impulsivity and sensation seeking, cognitive impairments in response inhibition and planning, and higher delay discounting rates. Pathological gambling has been associated with aberrant frontostriatal and limbic activity with decreased activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the Stroop interference task (a measure of the ability to inhibit irrelevant responses) and with decreased activation in the ventral striatum to a forced choice win-vs-loss task. Furthermore, pathological gambling has been associated with increased dopamine function with reduced acoustic startle prepulse inhibition, increased dopamine metabolites in the cerebrospinal fluid, and a study demonstrating the ability of amphetamine to prime the motivation to gamble.32 The published case-control studies that assessed factors associated with repetitive behaviors in PD are reviewed in the following paragraphs. Repetitive Behaviors. Weintraub et al2 demonstrated that a premorbid history of repetitive behaviors predisposed patients to these behaviors while taking dopamine agonists. Pontone et al33 demonstrated that depressed mood, increased irritability, disinhibition, and appetite disturbances may be more common in patients with these behaviors, although whether these behaviors predate the use of medications is not clear. Compulsive Medication Use. In keeping with the addiction literature, Evans et al9 found novelty seeking, depression scores, alcohol intake, and age of PD onset to be independent predictors of repetitive medication use in PD patients. The study did not confirm initial associations with male sex.9 In a positron emission tomography study using raclopride tagged with carbon 11 (D2/D3 receptor ligand), Evans et al34 demonstrated increased dopamine release in the ventral striatum in response to a levodopa challenge in PD patients with compulsive medication use. It was suggested that this finding supported the neural sensitization theory leading to the behavior of compulsive drug seeking. Furthermore, the subjective wanting but not the liking of levodopa was positively correlated with the degree of ventral striatal dopamine release. This observation was suggested to reflect an incentive salience for levodopa use rather than a pleasurable or reward-related quality.35 Pathological Gambling and Hypersexuality. Pathological gambling in the general population has been most consistently associated with male sex, mood and substance use disorders, high sensation seeking, and impulsivity.32 Voon et al23 found independent associations of elevated novelty-seeking traits, a personal or family history of alcohol use disorders, and earlier age of PD onset with pathological gambling in PD patients. Using a logistic regression model, these factors accounted for 62% of the variance. Impulsivity scores, particularly planning for long-term consequences, were more weakly associated with pathological gambling. The data, albeit preliminary, suggested that novelty seeking is more likely to be trait related (ie, to reflect an underlying heritable vulnerability), whereas the impulsivity scores may be more state related (ie, related to the presence of dopamine agonist or the state of active pathological gambling).23 A high level of novelty seeking has been associated with pathological gambling and is characterized by exploratory approach rather than avoidance behaviors, excitement with novel situations, impulsivity, rapid decision making, extravagance, and greater disorganization. A low level of novelty seeking has been variably but not consistently associated with PD and is characterized by a reflective, rigid, and slow-tempered temperament. The lowered novelty-seeking level, presumed to be related to the neurobiological mechanisms of PD affecting the ventral striatum, has been suggested to play a role in the lower rates of alcohol, cigarette, and other drug use in the PD population and may play a protective role against these behaviors before the onset of medication therapy. Medication-induced mania with onset after that of pathological gambling is robustly associated with pathological gambling in PD patients.23 However, rather than a bipolar diathesis being a risk factor, medication-induced mania may be part of the spectrum of dopamine dysregulation.8,23 In contrast to the studies on compulsive medication use and general pathological gambling, there was no association with depression or male sex. However, because pathological gambling in the general population is associated with males more than females in a 2:1 ratio, the lack of an association with sex in PD may be due to limited study power. Hypersexuality in PD is more likely to be associated with younger onset PD and, in contrast to that of pathological gambling, with males.4 Compulsive Eating. In an uncontrolled case series, Nirenberg et al5 found that PD patients with compulsive eating behaviors who were taking dopamine agonists were likely to have younger-onset PD and premorbid histories of repetitive behaviors or being overweight. Management Patients should be warned of the potential risk of these behaviors before initiation of treatment with dopamine agonists. The identification of patients at greater risk to allow for closer follow-up would be an ideal component to the ongoing management of PD. With all studies, earlier age of PD onset is associated with these behaviors, although differences in medication practices may mediate the association.1,4,9,23 Current studies suggest that specific risk factors for pathological gambling may include a personal or family history of alcohol use disorders and increased novelty-seeking traits,23 whereas impulsivity23 (or disinhibition34) may be associated but is more likely state related. Specific risk factors for compulsive medication use include increased novelty seeking, alcohol use, and higher depression scores.9 Hypersexuality is associated with the male sex.4 Nonspecific associated factors include a premorbid history of these behaviors,2 depressed mood, irritability, and appetite disturbances.34 Higher medication doses can be but are not necessarily associated with these behaviors.1,2,4 The management of pathological gambling has been described only in anecdotal reports. In patients with compulsive medication use or who take high doses, a decrease in medication dose may be sufficient. Decreasing or discontinuing therapy or switching to a different dopamine agonist may be but is not consistently effective. Antidepressants have not been found to be useful in case reports. External controls (eg, external control of finances) and referrals for gambling treatment have been anecdotally reported to be useful in a proportion of patients. Ardouin et al35 described a multicenter retrospective series of PD patients with active pathological gambling before deep brain stimulation targeting the subthalamic nucleus. The pathological gambling symptoms resolved immediately after or over the course of months in parallel with the decrease in the LED. The authors suggest that improvement may be related to (1) dose decreases or discontinuation of dopamine agonist therapy; (2) the relative specificity of deep brain stimulation for motor regions compared with medications; (3) the replacement of pulsatile stimulation with chronic stimulation, which may decrease neural sensitization; or (4) the actions of deep brain stimulation targeting the subthalmic nucleus.21 Point 4 is exemplified by studies of lesions in the subthalmic nucleus in rodent models of substance abuse. However, that the behaviors can worsen in the early postoperative period suggests that caution be applied in the selection and postoperative management of these patients. In subjects with compulsive medication use, management includes decreasing doses; external controls of doses; treatment of secondary psychotic, manic, or behavioral symptoms; and management of dopaminergic depressive withdrawal symptoms. Multidisciplinary involvement may be necessary.8,9 Similarly, deep brain stimulation of the subthalmic nucleus has been reported to be effective in well-selected patients. Relapse of addictive dopaminergic drug abuse behaviors is not infrequent in these patients. Conclusions Medication-induced repetitive behaviors as a group are relatively common and can have devastating psychosocial consequences. The study of these behaviors allows not only improved clinical management but also greater insight into a biologically mediated complex behavioral model. Back to top Article Information Correspondence: Valerie Voon, MD, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke, National Institutes of Health, 10 Center Dr, Bldg 10, Room 5S213, Bethesda, MD 20892-1428 (voonv@ninds.nih.gov). Accepted for Publication: November 20, 2006. Author Contributions:Study concept and design: Voon. Acquisition of data: Voon. Analysis and interpretation of data: Voon and Fox. Drafting of the manuscript: Voon. Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Voon and Fox. Obtained funding: Fox. Financial Disclosure: None reported. Acknowledgment: Anthony E. Lang, MD, and Venkata S. Mattay, MD, provided comments. References 1. Voon VHassan KZurowski M et al. Prospective prevalence of pathological gambling and medication association in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2006;66 (11) 1750- 1752PubMedGoogle Scholar 2. Weintraub DSiderowf ADPotenza MN et al. Association of dopamine agonist use with impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 2006;63 (7) 969- 973PubMedGoogle Scholar 3. Lu CBharmal ASuchowersky O Gambling and Parkinson disease [letter]. Arch Neurol 2006;63 (2) 298PubMedGoogle Scholar 4. Voon VHassan KZurowski M et al. Prevalence of repetitive and reward-seeking behaviors in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2006;67 (7) 1254- 1257PubMedGoogle Scholar 5. Nirenberg MJWaters C Compulsive eating and weight gain related to dopamine agonist use. Mov Disord 2006;21 (4) 524- 529PubMedGoogle Scholar 6. Fernandez HHFriedman JH Punding on L-dopa. Mov Disord 1999;14 (5) 836- 838PubMedGoogle Scholar 7. Evans AHKatzenschlager RPaviour DC et al. Punding in Parkinson's disease: its relation to the dopamine dysregulation syndrome. Mov Disord 2004;19 (4) 397- 405PubMedGoogle Scholar 8. Giovannoni GO’Sullivan JDTurner KManson AJLees AJ Hedonistic homeostatic dysregulation in patients with Parkinson's disease on dopamine replacement therapies. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000;68 (4) 423- 428PubMedGoogle Scholar 9. Evans AHLawrence ADPotts JAppel SLees AJ Factors influencing susceptibility to compulsive dopaminergic drug use in Parkinson. Neurology 2005;65 (10) 1570- 1574PubMedGoogle Scholar 10. Potenza MN Should addictive disorders include non–substance-related conditions? Addiction September 2006;101(suppl 1)142- 151PubMedGoogle Scholar 11. American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994 12. Avanzi MBaratti MCabrini SUber EBrighetti GBonfa F Prevalence of pathological gambling in patients with Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 2006;21 (12) 2068- 2072PubMedGoogle Scholar 13. Grosset KAMacphee GPal G et al. Problematic gambling on dopamine agonists: not such a rarity. Mov Disord 2006;21 (12) 2206- 2208PubMedGoogle Scholar 14. Shaffer HJHall MNVander Bilt J Estimating the prevalence of disordered gambling behavior in the United States and Canada: a research synthesis. Am J Public Health 1999;89 (9) 1369- 1376PubMedGoogle Scholar 15. Miyasaki JMAl Hassan KLang AEVoon V Punding prevalence in Parkinson's disease [published online ahead of print January 17, 2007]. Mov Disord doi:10.1002/mds.21296Google Scholar 16. Robinson TEBerridge KC Addiction. Annu Rev Psychol 2003;5425- 53PubMedGoogle Scholar 17. Everitt BJRobbins TW Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion. Nat Neurosci200581114811489 [published correction appears in Nat Neurosci.2006;97:979]. PubMedGoogle Scholar 18. Miwa HMorita SNakanishi IKondo T Stereotyped behaviors or punding after quetiapine administration in Parkinson's disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2004;10 (3) 177- 180PubMedGoogle Scholar 19. Voon VPotenza MNThomsen T Impulse control and repetitive behaviors in Parkinson's disease. Curr Opin Neurol In press Google Scholar 20. Fiorillo CDTobler PNSchultz W Discrete coding of reward probability and uncertainty by dopamine neurons. Science 2003;299 (5614) 1898- 1902PubMedGoogle Scholar 21. Pessiglione MSeymour BFlandin GDolan RJFrith CD Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behavior in humans. Nature 2006;442 (7106) 1042- 1045PubMedGoogle Scholar 22. Millan MJMaiofiss LCussac DAudinot VBoutin JANewman-Tancredi A Differential actions of antiparkinsonian agents at multiple classes of monoaminergic receptor, I: a multivariate analysis of the binding profiles of 14 drugs at 21 native and cloned human receptor subtypes. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 2002;303 (2) 791- 804PubMedGoogle Scholar 23. Voon VThomsen TMiyasaki JM et al. Factors associated with dopaminergic medication–related pathological gambling in Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 2007;64 (2) 212- 216PubMedGoogle Scholar 24. Khan NLGraham ECritchley P et al. Parkin disease: a phenotypic study of a large case series. Brain 2003;126 (pt 6) 1279- 1292PubMedGoogle Scholar 25. Frank MJSeeberger LJO’Reilly RC By carrot or by stick: cognitive reinforcement learning in parkinsonism. Science 2004;306 (5703) 1940- 1943PubMedGoogle Scholar 26. Cools R Dopaminergic modulation of cognitive function—implications for L-dopa treatment in Parkinson's disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2006;30 (1) 1- 23PubMedGoogle Scholar 27. Mattay VSTessitore ACallicott JH et al. Dopaminergic modulation of cortical function in patients with Parkinson's disease. Ann Neurol 2002;51 (2) 156- 164PubMedGoogle Scholar 28. Visanji NPGomez-Ramirez JJohnston TH et al. Pharmacological characterization of psychosis-like behavior in the MPTP-lesioned nonhuman primate model of Parkinson's disease. Mov Disord 2006;21 (11) 1879- 1891PubMedGoogle Scholar 29. Bézard EFerry SMach U et al. Attenuation of levodopa-induced dyskinesia by normalizing dopamine D3 receptor function. Nat Med 2003;9 (6) 762- 767PubMedGoogle Scholar 30. Dodd MLKlos KJBower JHGeda YEJosephs KAAhlskog JE Pathological gambling caused by drugs used to treat Parkinson disease. Arch Neurol 2005;62 (9) 1377- 1381PubMedGoogle Scholar 31. Fox SHVisanji NGomez-Ramirez JJohnston TVoon VBrotchie JM Dopamine receptor agonists and levodopa have similar ability to induce psychosis-like behavior in the MPTP primate model of Parkinson's disease. Arch Neurol 2006;63 (9) 1343- 1344PubMedGoogle Scholar 32. Goudriaan AEOosterlaan Jde Beurs Evan der Brink W Pathological gambling: a comprehensive review of biobehavioral findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004;28 (2) 123- 141PubMedGoogle Scholar 33. Pontone GWilliams JRBassett SSMarsh L Clinical features associated with impulse control disorders in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2006;67 (7) 1258- 1261PubMedGoogle Scholar 34. Evans AHPavese NLawrence AD et al. Compulsive drug use linked to sensitized ventral striatal dopamine transmission. Ann Neurol 2006;59 (5) 852- 858PubMedGoogle Scholar 35. Ardouin CVoon VWorbe C et al. Pathological gambling in Parkinson's disease improves chronic subthalamic stimulation. Mov Disord 2006;21 (11) 1941- 1946PubMedGoogle Scholar

Journal

Archives of NeurologyAmerican Medical Association

Published: Aug 1, 2007

There are no references for this article.